The Jordan B. Peterson Podcast - 143. Jean Piaget (Constructivism)

Episode Date: November 1, 2020

Jean Piaget, renowned developmental psychologist, helped us understand how the child built its own personality during exploration, and how that personality was further shaped by the games people play....--Go to https://Surfshark.deals/peterson and use code PETERSON to get 83% off a 2-year plan and 3 extra months for free!--For Advertising Inquiries, visit https://www.advertisecast.com/TheJordanBPetersonPodcast

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hey there! Welcome to the Jordan B Peterson Podcast. This episode is from the personality series and is available on YouTube as well. It's episode 30 season three called Jean Piaget so you can guess who it's about. It's very informative or I hope you enjoy it. As you regular listeners know, I haven't spoken to you guys in quite a while so here are our updates. One, dad released a new video called Return Home. This is the first content he's put out in over a year and a half. Check it out if you haven't. It was super exciting.
Starting point is 00:00:34 I partied on Instagram for like a week afterwards. I had son is YouTube. Number two, JBP is back producing content. I'm actually recording a podcast with him next week and that'll be out on my podcast. He will be having live guests on his eventually but he decided he'd rather do him with me to get warmed up. Lucky me, right?
Starting point is 00:00:56 I'll keep you posted on those but they will be available wherever podcasts are available and on YouTube at the McAle Peterson podcast. Number three, we have officially brought Dad's podcast in-house and we're producing it ourselves now with an awesome production team, Shadow to Eric. We have some new sponsors that are helping us continue, which is awesome and I will introduce one of them right now.
Starting point is 00:01:17 I hope you enjoy this episode on Jean-Pierre J. and stay tuned for a new podcast with Dad featuring new content. Thank goodness, 2020 is almost over. Also, I hope you had a great Halloween, even though they tried to cancel it. I was a lobster. If I'm lucky, I'll get Dad in his lobster outfit
Starting point is 00:01:34 up on his Instagram soon too. So here's to hoping for that. I would love to introduce one of our new podcast sponsors, Surfshark. Surfshark is a VPN. The user interface is really simple and easy to use, but provides robust security options, like encrypting all data sent via the internet, protecting passwords, private messages, photos, videos, and all other sensitive data, just like a VPN should surf shark also allows you to control your privacy settings, like white listing your bank, Amazon and other trusted websites, while blocking blocking information since all the websites you do not choose to white list. Surfshark set me up with a trial account, and now I can be sure that when I buy things on the dark web using Bitcoin, I'm not going to be tracked.
Starting point is 00:02:15 I'm kidding, but did you know Bitcoin actually isn't a good way to buy things online anonymously? That's a side note. I'm going on a tangent, but they can track it, so don't do it that way. Anyway, with some of the other VPN services I've tried, it can be a bit confusing, I am blonde, and not very intuitive. Surfshark is super easy to set up and easy to use. They also offer a 30-day money-back guarantee,
Starting point is 00:02:36 allowing you to try this VPN service for yourself, and for a limited time, you can get 83% off of a two-year plan and three extra months for free at surfshark.deals slash Peterson. I don't know how they chose 83% off but that's massive. The special offer makes your subscription just $2.21 since U.S. per month so you can browse securely on all your devices. That's surfshark.deals slash Peterson. Please enjoy this episode about Jean Piaget. We are thrilled dad is doing better. Now maybe the Peterson family can come out of the shell it's been hiding in for
Starting point is 00:03:13 the last year. I certainly am. Hope you have a good week. He is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a generalist, he is a general a term that I think has ever ever only been applied to him. An epistemologist is someone who studies the manner in which knowledge, who studies knowledge itself, and that's what PHA thought that he was doing. And he wanted to understand how it was that knowledge unfolded across time. And more importantly than that, he wanted to understand how the cognitive structures that made up an individual developed across time. So that's generally why he's regarded as a developmental psychologist because, of course,
Starting point is 00:04:12 he also studied children. Now I'm going to read you something that Piaget wrote to begin this. He wrote an awful lot of books and a lot of them haven't yet been translated. And some of the ones that are translated are translated pretty badly. And I think this is probably an example of a fairly bad translation. But regardless of that, the point that's being made is both valid and interesting. So, the golden postulate of various traditional epistemologies, which are theories of valid knowledge,
Starting point is 00:04:47 is that knowledge is a fact, say, or a set of facts, so that you would go to class and learn a set of facts, and that is what would give you knowledge. Instead of a process, and if our various forms of knowledge are always incomplete, as we know they are, because they're replaced sequentially. For example, recently in the domain of physics, physicists revealed to us that they really didn't understand what some massive percentage of the universe was made of. I think I can't remember, it's 75% or 95% made out of dark matter, which has never been detected, and we know how to think about it. So that's a good example of how our fundamental assumptions
Starting point is 00:05:22 being detected and we know how to think about it. So that's a good example of how our fundamental assumptions can be challenged at any moment and how a fact can turn from a fact into a clear fallacy. If our various forms of knowledge are always complete and our various sciences still imperfect, that which is acquired is acquired and can therefore be studied statically. So the proposition there is that you come and you learn the facts and the facts themselves are solving bits of information and they don't change across time and so that once you have them, you have them. Now of course, PSJA obviously doubts that.
Starting point is 00:05:55 Hence the absolute position of the problems, what is knowledge or how are the various types of knowledge possible as an alternative? Under the converging influence of a series of factors, we are tending more and more today to regard knowledge as a process more than a state. Any being or object that sciences attempt to hold fast dissolves once again in the current of development.
Starting point is 00:06:19 It is the last analysis of this development and of it alone that we have the right to state. It is a fact. What it means by that is the fact analysis of this development and the Vidalon that we have the right to state. It is a fact. What it means by that is the fact that human beings learn facts is in fact a fact, right? So you are capable of learning things. And he thinks that's sort of the fundamental fact. People assimilate information, and they
Starting point is 00:06:40 transform as a consequence of it. And so studying that, as he says, what we can and should then seek is a law of this process. We're well aware on the other hand of the find book by Koon on Scientific Revolutions. Now, the reason he has a throw of that in, at the end, how many of you know about Thomas Koon's book on Scientific Revolutions? No one? Just three people?
Starting point is 00:07:07 Okay, well, that's probably not good. Kun was one of the 20th century's foremost philosophers of science, and he made a distinction between two different modes of scientific process, one he called normal science and the other he called revolutionary science, and normal science was the kind of science that you'll do as undergraduates, unless you're incredibly lucky or unbelievably brilliant, or probably unbelievably brilliant and incredibly lucky. And so, normal science occurs when you generate knowledge incrementally from within the confines of an already developed scientific theory.
Starting point is 00:07:46 So a normal science would be, say, the application of big five models of personality to the prediction of some other variable, like, say, relationship success. You're going to uncover something new, but it's not going to shape the foundations of knowledge itself. Now, Kunims, from time to time, there were discoveries that were made that shook the foundation of knowledge itself. Now, Kunims are from time to time. There were discoveries that were made that shook the foundation of knowledge itself. There's some well-known revolutions in science. So the Newtonian revolution was a revolution in science. Einstein's work produced another revolution in science. Darwin's work, Mendels's work.
Starting point is 00:08:21 Those are the revolutionary transformations of thought that people often consider. Did I say Darwin? I certainly should have said Darwin because it was probably the most revolutionary of all. Now, the reason that I wanted to tell you that and the reason I kept it in here, even though it seems like it was funny,
Starting point is 00:08:41 little additional statement pinned to the end of Piaget's quotation is that who observed that science seemed to progress in accordance with the same mechanisms that Piaget observed that the knowledge of children appeared to progress. Now, one of the things that you'll learn about Piaget, which is generally all you learn if you learn about Piaget is his idea that children go through sequential stages of development and usually people have you memorize those stages.
Starting point is 00:09:10 I should let you know that PHA really can care that much about those stages and when they occur and how they could be sped up at exactly what they were. He was much more interested in deep philosophical questions and that element of his work generally goes unrecognized by the North American psychologists who report to understand what he had to say. Now, because he wrote dozens of books, some of which are not yet translated, it's not surprising that people come away with a partial view of PHA.
Starting point is 00:09:41 When you're that prolific, he was so damn smart. He was offered the curatorship of a museum when he was 10, because he published a scientific paper on, I think it was on snails. And, but his parents requested that he turn it down. So, you know, Piaget was a major genius, and if someone puts their mind, like that, puts their mind to work for an entire lifetime,
Starting point is 00:10:04 and he lived to be an old man, they could produce an awful lot of intellectual material. And it's not necessarily that can't be summarized easily as a single, you know, coherent theory that you could memorize in one page. It's pretty hard for people to keep up. So it's not surprising that his thought gets reduced to, you know, a set of axioms, in a sense. But the devil's often in the detail with great thinkers. And what that means is that to really understand what they had to say, you actually have to read them. Because a lot of the information
Starting point is 00:10:33 is at the sentence level of analysis and not at the summary level of analysis. You know, if you're really smart, it's not that easy to summarize what you have to say. Because most of what you have to say is in fact informative, so you can't just throw it away. It's hard to compress it. So anyways, this initial paragraph opens up the world of constructivism to you. Now the constructivists are interesting people because you know you often hear people ask
Starting point is 00:10:58 is it genetic or environmental which is it's not a good question because it's a false dichotomy. which is, it's not a good question, because it's a false dichotomy. The genes formulate structures in accordance with environmental demand right from the beginning of the organism's emergence. And so there's a constant interplay between the environment and genetics. But the environment isn't also just a thing that's out there
Starting point is 00:11:20 that's made out of lions and tigers, of moose and buildings and the sky. It's an information field. And the constructivist point out rightly that partly what you're doing when you're operating in the world is interacting with this field of information and incorporating its structure into the structure of your mind and body,
Starting point is 00:11:40 which is how you adapt. So you could say in some sense that you're built out of information and matter. That's a good way of thinking about it. And the constructivists are very interested in how you go about acquiring information and how you then transform that information into the sort of knowledge that you can apply to the world.
Starting point is 00:11:58 So they're also very pragmatic, especially PHA, because PHA regards knowledge as like the prerequisite for adaptive action. So again, he's less concerned about the facts that you know about the structure of the world. Then he is about how it is that you modify and adapt your behavior so that you can survive in the work. So that's the first positive, that's constructivism.
Starting point is 00:12:23 And then the next positive is that there are revolutions in this internal structures that emerge as a consequence of you acquiring new information. Sometimes that information can already be fit into a knowledge structure that you possess. But sometimes the information is so anomalous or novel that it blows out fundamental presuppositions that you've already established
Starting point is 00:12:46 and forces you to not only add some information to your repertoire of information, but to reconfigure the structure you use to represent the information. You don't like that. People don't like it when that happens. It's too dramatic and upsetting. Which is often why revolutions in science or in any other field are first resisted. I mean it's very complicated. The reasons people are opposed to new ideas are very complicated. There's lots of
Starting point is 00:13:15 theories about why that occurs precisely. But I'll briefly outline a good theory. There's a bunch of them, but I'll outline one. So let's say there's already a scientific theory and it's instanti of them, but all online one. So let's say there's already a scientific theory, and it's instantiated in the world, so people accept it. And let's say that you are a proponent of that theory. So you might say, well, that theory, therefore, governs your worldview. And if I threaten it, then your worldview
Starting point is 00:13:38 is going to fall apart, and that's going to make you fall apart. And that's not a bad theory. But here's a variant of that, which is similar, but which I think is better. So I'm a professor, let's say I have a theory. Now what I'm doing with my theory is buying my right to be a professor. So I would come to the University of Toronto
Starting point is 00:13:58 as a job candidate, and I would say, here's my theory, and here's why I think the fact supported. Here's what it's good for and they'll say, okay, it looks like you know enough about what you're doing so that you can occupy this position in this dominant hierarchy. Okay, and I'm pretty happy about that because that position in that dominant hierarchy
Starting point is 00:14:16 is pretty permanent. That's one of the advantages to an academic job. It's like once you have it, as long as you're relatively confident and relatively ethical, then you can maintain it across time. So it's a big deal to be granted that slot. And then being in that dominant turkey is not only a matter of what you believe, obviously. The fact that I'm in a dominant turkey means I get a certain salary.
Starting point is 00:14:41 And that's not hypothetical. That allows me to eat. So I'm happy about that. It's not only psychological. and I can pay my housing payments with it and so on. So it protects me from the cold, and offers me something to eat, and it gives me a certain public status. And so if some others Joker comes along, say they're young, and they say, well, I have a different theory that makes your theory look stupid, then I'm not going to be very happy about that, partly because it upsets the way that I can figure my understanding of the world, and that's a drag, but I might recover from that. But it also undermines my claim, it undermines the validity of my claim to that position in the hierarchy. And so it sort of makes me an imposter, say that I turns out to be right, it's like,
Starting point is 00:15:23 hoo, I'm an imposter, and I no longer really have the right to occupy that position. Someone might even point that out, although it doesn't have very often in the case of professors. You know, it happens fairly frequently in other sorts of occupations. You know, all of a sudden your position is made useless because somebody figured out how to automate you and hoof, you're gone. And like that's hard on your worldview, but it's a lot harder on your salary. So a lot of the reasons that people claim to the validity of their theories is because it gives them a claim to a certain kind of what we call skill set and utility that then gives them a claim to occupy a certain position in the dominance hierarchy, and that protects them from, from, you know, all the horrors of reality, not completely, but I have health insurance, for example.
Starting point is 00:16:07 Sometimes that's really, really helpful. So I don't want some Joker coming along and point out that my theory isn't right. Anyway, so far that hasn't happened. So that's a good thing as far as I'm concerned. So PHA concludes and says, if all knowledge is always in the state of development and consists in proceeding from one state to a more complete and efficient one, evidently, it is a question of knowing this development and analyzing it with the greatest possible
Starting point is 00:16:35 accuracy. Okay, so, PHA figures you're a information foraging machine, so to speak, and the process that you engage in while you're foraging from information and then figuring out what to do with it is typical to human beings, and there's some constancy of structure across human beings. Okay, so that's the first idea. It isn't that we all do it in a different way, even though there's individual variation, obviously.
Starting point is 00:17:03 The other sort of fundamental postulate of constructivism, especially the The other fundamental postulate of constructivism, especially the Piagetian version of constructivism, is that you build yourself from the bottom up, starting with your body. So Piag, this is one of the things about Piaget's theory that's unbelievably sophisticated, I think. So when people were first developing models of artificial intelligence, they thought
Starting point is 00:17:23 they'd be able to develop machines that sort of modeled the world and then figured out how to act in the world, sort of abstractly. And then, after they figured out how to act in the world, would then act in the world. But that proved to be impossible, as you can tell, because we don't have ambulatory robots that could like bus tables at a restaurant, which turns out to be, by the way, a very complex job. Fast mathematical operations,
Starting point is 00:17:49 computer can handle that easily. Or easily, by busing a table, it's like no computer smart enough to do that. So that's pretty peculiar. But it turned out, after like 40 years of investigation, that you couldn't build computers that would operate as independent robots by teaching them to model the world and then by having the model the potential action that they were going to undertake and then by implementing it that did not work partly because modeling the world is way more complicated than anybody ever suspected like infinitely more complicated. And so some robotics engineers, such as Rodney Brooks, who worked at MIT, started building robots from the bottom up. He made these little mindless robots that really didn't
Starting point is 00:18:29 even have a central processor that were action oriented. So like the first things he built, with these little insect-like things that could skitter away from light, that's all they could do. Turn a light poof, they'd go find some dark. And it's as if, so imagine the world to that robot was a binary place. It was either a light place or a dark place. And then you might say, well, what did life or dark mean to this little robot? And then you have to ask yourself, well, what does meaning mean?
Starting point is 00:19:00 And that's a very good question. It's one of the PSJA answers. Meaning means to that little robot, move to a different place. So light to that robot meant move to a different place. So what the robot, in a sense, was doing, was transforming one form of information, light versus start, into another form, which was skill away. And so I love that because it's not easy to understand
Starting point is 00:19:25 what meaning means until you're related to the body. And so PHA's fundamental proposition is that the elements of your understanding are not perceptual abstractions. In fact, there's even elements of understanding that underlie your perceptual abstractions that are more fundamental and what those are essentially are sensory motor skills, things you do with your body. It's a lovely idea. It's extremely profound. And I think it's absolutely correct. It does wreak havoc with the idea of disembodied intelligence, however, because for
Starting point is 00:20:02 Piaget and also for Rodney Brooks, who is responsible by the way, just so you know, some of you seem big dog. You seem big dog, look up DARPA, D-A-R-P-A, how many of you seem big dog? How many of you are terrified by big dog? Yes, big dog is this robot that's being developed by the US Army that is about this big and it's four-legged, it's got a head. And it can run like faster than you. And it can run in snow, and it can run on ice,
Starting point is 00:20:30 and it can run uphill. And if you kick it, if valences and stands back up, but hypothetically, it's going to be used to transport like the heavy things that soldiers have to carry. But don't believe that, that's a stupid idea. Once these things can amulet by themselves and they can already follow each other, because now they have visual systems,
Starting point is 00:20:52 arming them is going to be a very simple matter. And so the probability that we'll have unbelievably super fast robots that can shoot you in 10 years is like, to me, as far as I can tell from the development is absolutely certain. I have a friend who's a computer engineer and he's a really good one and he said, you know how in science fiction movies sometimes when those robots shoot at you they miss, said when the robot shoot at you not only will they shoot at where you are but they'll shoot at the six places that they calculate you're most likely the dark two and they will never miss.
Starting point is 00:21:23 So that's a lovely thing to think about. So hopefully that won't come to pass, but you probably will. So here's the sort of thing that PHA was interested in. These are very fundamental questions. And he was a very deep intellect. So he tried to go right to the bottom of the structures of knowledge to find out what was down there.
Starting point is 00:21:48 Upon what does an individual basis judgements? That's a good one. How do you know whether you A or B, or what the difference is between right and wrong? How is that instantiated in your being? What are your norms? How is it that those norms are validated? What's the interest of such norms for the philosophy of science in general, which is a question like,
Starting point is 00:22:12 well, there's a consensual reality that we all share to some degree, which is why we can communicate, but there's no one to one relationship between that consensual reality and the categories of science. So, Piaget was interested in the similarities between our consensual viewpoint and the categories of science. So PHA was interested in the similarities between our consensual viewpoint and the scientific viewpoint and the differences. So for example, people used to hypothetically assume that the world was flat. And you could say, well, the reason they assumed that was
Starting point is 00:22:36 because it looks flat when you look at it. So it was an empirical observation. But obviously, there were other observations that we managed to produce that indicated that the world wasn't flat and then our scientific conceptions and our interpersonal, uh, consensual norms became divorced from one another and that's become a really serious problem, say with branches of science like quantum mechanics, which, you know, they're, completely incomprehensible from a pragmatic perspective,
Starting point is 00:23:07 even to those who formulate them mathematically. A PSA would say the reason for that is that, well, when we interact with objects at the phenomenal, phenomenological level, which is the level that we can most easily perceive, they act Newtonian, so can I take your pen? So you can sort of predict what this pen will do because it acts like other objects of about its size and shape with mass. And so, your understanding of this is actually based upon your knowledge of what will happen.
Starting point is 00:23:38 If you manipulate this thing with your body, you know, it's solid. You'd be very surprised if you could like put your fingers through this pen. You'd be surprised if it broke because you expect a certain hardness. You do expect it to write, although PEMS off the don't. You expect it to be pointy. You know that you can take it apart into many objects even though,
Starting point is 00:23:57 like you can ask how many objects is this? Well, it's one pen, right? But you could take it apart. You know? And now it's one pen, right? But you could take it apart. And now it's two objects. And then you can see that there's other objects in here. And sometimes you can put it back together, and it'll still work. And so,
Starting point is 00:24:22 so P.A.J.'s point is that what we regard as understanding, to say that you understand something, is to indicate that you can predict what is going to occur if you interact with that object with your bottom. OK, and that gives you your intuitive understanding of things. And so because this is material, you already know something about other things that are
Starting point is 00:24:46 material, right? And you can transfer it from place to place. And that's again in body knowledge, because material things are those things through which you cannot put your hand, right? You get some weird things like smoke or clouds and it's like, well, are they, what are they? Are they objects? Is a cloud an object? Well, no, not really. Because an object is one of these things that you can sort of manipulate as a unit. Now, the categories of quantum mechanics, of course, which deal with these incredibly tiny things that are really not particles and are really not waves, they're incomprehensible to us because we never manipulate anything at that scale. And so because we can't play with it, we have no real way of understanding it because our understanding is based on the mapping of objects onto our body.
Starting point is 00:25:32 So any object we can't map onto our body is therefore fundamentally incomprehensible. It's a very cool theory, you know, and a very body-centric theory. So how does the fact that children think differently than we do affect our presumption of fact itself? That's a very interesting question too, is like, so you've got three year olds, and of course,
Starting point is 00:25:51 they're pretty clueless about the world, but and you know more, but there's the three year old is alive and everything and functioning. And so then you have to ask yourself, well, if their conception of the world is qualitatively different than your conception, how is it that you can both survive in the same world? And what does that mean about what knowledge means and about the limits of knowledge?
Starting point is 00:26:12 I mean, I could say the same thing about you that you might say about a three-year-old, which is, well, if I took you from this place and dropped you in the jungle, soon you'd be dead, first you'd be miserable, then you'd be dead. And so that be miserable, then you'd be dead. And so that's sort of like the three-year-olds state of being if you were supposed to take care of them and you disappear.
Starting point is 00:26:32 So obviously, even your knowledge of the world is limited by what? It's limited in its necessary generality by the context. But that also has something interesting to say about the validity of your knowledge. So context dependent. So OK, so that's the sort of thing that PHA was interested in. Here's some other ones. What do you mean by number? So that's an interesting one. So you know how they say you can't compare apples and oranges? Well, you can. So if I said,
Starting point is 00:27:00 well, what's two oranges plus two apples? For fruits, who said that? That's very good. So the way you solve that was by generalizing up a level. So if I said, what's two desks plus two rocks, what would you say? Yes, man. You're very good at this. That's an IQ. That's all the IQ questions, by the way.
Starting point is 00:27:23 So if you didn't get them now, you could feel disappointed and way. So if you did get them, now you could feel disappointed to press. So if you got them, well, then you can cut yourself from that. So that's good. Yeah. So number is a funny thing, because, well, as I pointed out with the pen, well, one can become many,
Starting point is 00:27:37 very rapidly, and many can become one. And well, the whole idea of a number is extremely difficult to understand, you know, like what is it in common between singular entities that allows you to represent all of them with one? Well, animals don't do that. They can in a sense, they can sort of intuit three or four, which is about all we can intuit, too. But once we get the nomenclature done properly, man, we can use numbers like crazy. And then they enable us to manipulate reality like mad. So whatever it is we're extracting from the commonality
Starting point is 00:28:12 between objects seems to be something that gives us incredible power. So that's a problem. What do you mean by space? Since we know, for example, from Einstein's work that space is not an absolute in any sense. It isn't the speed we move. So what do you mean by time and speed? What is an object permanent and when it isn't it? And when do you learn that? What does it mean that an object is the same across time?
Starting point is 00:28:39 That's a good one. So I don't know if there's a single molecule in your body that was there, you know, five months ago, six months ago. There's some, I don't remember what the turnover duration is, but it's fairly quick. So it's weird, eh, because there you are, and you were there two years ago, but none of your constituent elements are the same. So how is it that you can be the same? Now, the physicist Schrodinger of the Schrodinger's cat fame solved that by claiming that you were a dissipative structure. It's a very interesting way.
Starting point is 00:29:12 That's what he thought life was. Disappearative structures. A dissipative structure is the same as a, you know, when you pull up the, you pull that at the bottom of the sink. Stopper. I think so, stopper. If you're sink is full of water and you pull up the stopper, you know, you get a whirlpool, right?
Starting point is 00:29:29 And all the water spins as it goes down the drain hole. And you'll notice that that whirlpool looks sort of the same across time, even though obviously the water molecules that make it out are different. He called that a dissipative structure. It was a pattern that maintained itself in spite of the movement of that makes it up are different. He called that a dissipative structure. It was a pattern that maintained itself in spite of the movement of matter through it.
Starting point is 00:29:49 And that's what you are, by the way. So it requires energy to keep a dissipative structure intact, but you take in energy. So thanks to the sun. What do you mean by chance? Why do you have moral concerns and what does it mean that you have moral concerns that you have ideas about how people should behave and how they shouldn't behave and you have a very deep understanding of that and it's characteristic of all human beings and many, many animals. So where does that come from and is it developed for them knowledge?
Starting point is 00:30:24 What are children doing when they're playing? What are children doing when they're playing? What are people doing when they're dreaming? And what's the significance of the fact that you can imitate other people? Now, I'll start with the last one briefly. You know, you might think that one of the things that really distinguishes us from other creatures, animals is the fact that we have a thumb.
Starting point is 00:30:45 And that's a big one. We've got very good functional thumbs and hooray for that. And we stand up on two feet, so we get the chance to use our thumbs and hands to carry things around and to break things and to take the department to swing sticks and so on. But there are other animals who can do that to a limited degree. And then there's our ability to talk. That's a major one.
Starting point is 00:31:03 But one of the other things that really differentiates people from other animals is, you know, you hear monkey C, monkey 2, right? Well, that's wrong. Monkey C, that's the end of that. Monkey's really can't imitate one another, and they certainly can't imitate any even randomly generated novel behavior
Starting point is 00:31:25 than another monkey produces. Think about it this way. You know, you hear these claims that chimpanzees have culture? It's like, well, no, they don't, not really. And the reason for that, it's easy to figure out that they don't have culture. I mean, let's say chimps have been around
Starting point is 00:31:39 roughly speaking for 15 million years, which is not a bad estimate because we diversed from them about seven million years ago, and evidence is pretty clear that the thing that we diverged from looked a lot more like a chimp than it looked like us. So like we've been booting it ahead, madly developing like mad in the chimps of the playback and eating leaves, you know, so, so do they have a culture? Well they've had 15 million years. You know, they haven't even built a hut yet.
Starting point is 00:32:07 And the reason I'm telling you that is because if you're a culture-generated creature, and you only imagine Matt, you only manage to say one discovery of 100 years between all of you. If you have 15 million years to get your act together, that's a lot of 100-year statements. Even if the chimpanzee was building culture at the rate of 0.001% a year, if you compound that over 50 million years, you have the Empire State Building.
Starting point is 00:32:36 There are Empire State buildings that chimpanzees are living in, and so therefore they don't have culture. Now, they might be able to recognize their peers in the dominance hierarchy, they can do that, and they can use maybe, they can use simple tools. But the tools they use seem to be dependent on the environment that they inhabit. Because some people say, well, some chimps use one tool
Starting point is 00:32:57 and some chimps use another. But if you're gonna live in a rocky place, then you might use rocks, and if you're gonna live in a place that has sticks, you might use sticks. But that's not because you're different. It's because the environment's different. So anyways, chimps can't imitate one another very much. But humans, man, we're ridiculous.
Starting point is 00:33:16 Like, we're so imitative, it's absolutely crazy. And that's so cool because what it means is that once you get a pattern of behavior, you know, you whip up a new pattern of behavior, I can watch you and I can just instantiate it in my own body, you know, maybe a bit awkwardly to begin with, although maybe sometimes better than you can, and bang, I've got that. And so we're always looking around at each other, seeing what we're up to. And as soon as we see someone who's up to something interesting, then we can do the same thing. Children do that because they imitate, right?
Starting point is 00:33:47 But they're even better at it, man. They generalize across instances of imitation. So for example, remember when you're a kid, then you're playing, maybe you're playing house, and you're the mom, or maybe you're, I don't know, maybe you're the house cat. Because children will certainly do that. And so let's say the child is being the house cat. So he or she is down on all fours, zoomed in like a cat, maybe mowing, and rubbing up against her mother's leg and looking for a milk in the bowl.
Starting point is 00:34:12 They're not exactly imitating their cat. Because if they were exactly imitating their cat, they would be moving exactly the same way the cat moved. But they're not. What they're doing is they've observed the cat across a wide variety of environments. They've extracted what constitutes generalized cat behavior from all of those instances, and then they can instantiate the spirit of the cat,
Starting point is 00:34:38 which is sort of the movements that make up a catness. And then you, because you're so smart, you can watch them zooming around on the ground, and despite the fact that they don't have a tail or ears or a fur, you're gonna figure out pretty quick that that is now a cat. And children do the same thing with their parents, like if they're playing house, and they imitate their father,
Starting point is 00:34:57 you know, they may, to some degree, try to imitate the sound of his voice, and maybe even use a couple of his favorite phrases. But basically what they're doing is trying to act like a father rather than imitating their father. So it's like it's meta-imitation, right? So I watch you, I watch you, I watch you, I watch you. There's commonalities across all those instances.
Starting point is 00:35:19 I extract the commonalities I embody it. And then when I play, that's what I'm doing. I'm figuring out how to embody the commonalities, I embody it, and then when I play, that's what I'm doing. I'm figuring out how to embody the commonalities across multiple exposures. And that's what you're doing that one. You're like three. You're so smart. And then even if you're asleep, you're doing it
Starting point is 00:35:34 because you're doing it in your dreams. And so human beings do that. No other animals do. So when you're thinking about the ways that were weird, you want to put imitation way up there on the list because you can think about deaf people, like, genetically deaf people. They don't have much access to language,
Starting point is 00:35:52 say they don't learn sign language because that used to happen in the past. It's like they can still wander around in the world and fit in. Well, why? Well, because they're so good at imitating, they get all the nonverbalbal stuff and that's a lot. So, languages, you know, great and all that.
Starting point is 00:36:10 Language actually enables you to imitate across space and time. That's what it's for. Because, you know, maybe you whip up a new action, and then you write it down in words, and then you send it to someone, and those are instructions, instructions by the way because that's what instructions are. You send that to someone and then they read your code for behavior and then they act it out. And so language enables you to move imitation across space and time. And that's a really good way of conceptualizing language because it's a Piagetian way because sometimes we might think that language is there to describe the world, you know, in a scientific sense. George Kelly kind of thinks that, you know, in a scientific sense, George Kelly kind of thinks that, that thought that, that human beings are sort of like natural
Starting point is 00:36:50 scientists, but natural scientists are much more concerned with what is than with how to act. So Kelly's wrong, what human beings are more like is natural engineers, because we're always, you know, zooming around, trying to figure out how to fiddle with things, much more than what they are. And as I've mentioned to you earlier, a pragmatist would say, well, it's an artificial distinction, because things are what they are when you fiddle with them. That's in fact what they are.
Starting point is 00:37:19 So, it's not pretty smart. And as they also says, this is part of the constructivist notion, because constructivism is a philosophical school. He says, no, it does not begin in the eye, and it does not begin in the object. It begins in the interactions. There's a reciprocal and simultaneous construction of the subject on the one hand and the object on the other. There is no structure apart from construction, either abstract or genetic. I really like that. I showed you some of the symbolic categories in a couple of, you know, previous lectures. And I mentioned, for example, that one of the symbolic categories is
Starting point is 00:37:57 the Great Father, and the Great Father kind of stands for Cultural Structure, whereas the Great Mother stands for novelty and sometimes the terrifying. Anyway, back to the great father. There's a precondition in constructivism that there has to be three things that exist. They map right onto that symbolic structure. One is culture. So when you look at something, or really when any creature
Starting point is 00:38:24 looks at something, there's an inbuilt structure that characterizes the creature, some of which would be biological, built in, and some of which would be acquired, that they must have in order to structure their perceptions of what they're looking at. So you don't come to the situation blank, you know, for example, you have two eyes, so you're going to have stereo vision most of the time. And you have five senses, and they're the same senses. And there's more than that, you have snake detection circuits, for example, so you're sort of primed
Starting point is 00:38:53 to respond to a certain class of predators. You like sweet tastes and sour taste, but you don't really like bitter tastes and so on and so forth. Like right from the beginning, you've written a landscape of interpretive structures in order to frame and simplify the world that you are exposed to. Now, the world itself is this sort of amorphous thing. It's amorphous, because it's so multi-dimensional and complex.
Starting point is 00:39:22 There's so much of it. It's like a fog that contains everything. And so unless you can frame that and simplify it and narrow it, it's very difficult for you to understand and interact with it at all. I mean, you just can't deal with everything at once. It's hard enough to deal with one little thing at a time. And this internal structure is partly
Starting point is 00:39:42 what enables you to deal with one little thing at a time. And then, so there's you, and there's the source of all information. There's the structure of you and the source of all information. And then, the process that's you, and the process that's you, is you using your appendages, fundamentally, and your senses, to interact with things, and to make them manifest new properties, right? And so those are properties that they might not manifest without you there. And it's very difficult for you to tell when you're interacting with the world,
Starting point is 00:40:13 even at a perceptual level, how much what you observe wouldn't be there if you weren't there. Now, so because one of the big philosophical questions is, well, what's there when you're not there? And that is a much more complicated question than you might imagine. The Taoist would say, well, what's there is so much an amalgam of everything at once that it might as well be nothing at all. And it's sort of a, and give you a way of understanding that.
Starting point is 00:40:43 So let's say you took every symphony that was ever written, recorded, okay? And then you took all those recordings together and you laid one on top of the other. And you'd say, well, now I've got like every symphony at once on a tape and then you played the tape, what would it sound like? Would sound like white noise, it would sound like, sound like, it sound like white noise, it would sound like, shhh, shhh, shhh, shhh, shhh, shhh, shhh, shhh, and so you could say white noise is functionally equivalent to every single symphony that's ever been written, every piece of music that's ever
Starting point is 00:41:14 been written, all being played at once. So what? You know, so in the Dallas would also say, you have to remember that what something is is just as dependent on what it isn't as it is dependent on what it is. So that's a tough one, but it's very, very smart. So the constructivist would certainly agree with that. Now, Bruner, who is a constructivist of sorts, put a little twist on PHA's idea, which
Starting point is 00:41:40 I'm going to borrow because I think it makes explaining PHA easier, at least it makes it easier for me, and since I'm explaining it, that's what we're going to borrow because I think it makes explaining P.A.J. easier, at least it makes it easier for me. And since I'm explaining it, that's what we're going to have to go for. Rooner said, we seem to have no other way of describing lived time, save in the form of a narrative. Now, the reason I think this is a P.A.J. Indian constructivist claim is because P.A.J.
Starting point is 00:42:00 is concerned with knowledge as it emerges from action. And action is clearly represented in narratives, right? Because a narrative is about what the characters are doing. So narrative is the way that we represent information about doing. So, and knowledge for Piaget is about doing. So I just put the two together and that, and that, and that, there's other reasons for two, and that makes it simpler to explain. So here's, here's a way of thinking about how we put a structure
Starting point is 00:42:27 on the world. So I've mentioned, I kind of introduced you to this idea before. So you're headed somewhere wherever that happens to be, because you're an active organism. And so even if you don't have any immediate needs, you're going to poke about things just because you're curious. Turns out that your dopamine energy system, which is the system that drives curiosity,
Starting point is 00:42:47 fires at a certain constant rate, even if you're not hungry or tired or thirsty or, you know, even if no primary needs are clamoring for your attention. So your default comfortable state is mildly curious about everything. And so that's part of what makes you an information for it, because when you have nothing better to do, so to speak, you'll just poke around and see what happens because you never know that knowledge might come in useful in the future.
Starting point is 00:43:12 So anyways, you're always going from point A to point B because you're active. And the way you get from point A to point B, even if point B is an abstraction, because it often is. I'm going towards a better future Well, that's kind of a weird abstraction. How are you gonna get there? Well, you can be sure that at the base It's gonna involve movement right because you get to that future. There's there's movements you're gonna have to make so
Starting point is 00:43:38 Start with an abstract philosophical concept I want to be a good person. Well, you write debate forever about what the good means. It's the sort of philosophical conundrum that you can fall into because you can take an abstractions. And then you might think of it as a property of the world. And it's something that's subject to the kind of debate, which is exactly what's happened in the history of mankind. But there's another way of looking at it that's actually probably more accurate
Starting point is 00:44:06 and I would say also more useful. And this is the way it's like. So you're gonna try to be a good person. Well, the PSGETIAN view would be being a good person. It's not a state of mind. What it is is an abstraction that represents, well, first, other associated abstractions because we could say, well, being, other associated abstractions,
Starting point is 00:44:25 because we could say, well, being a good person is a multi-dimensional problem, right? You can't just be good, and then it's done. It's like being a good person might be being good at, well, what? Well, good at being a friend, good at being a lover, good at being a parent, good at being a child, good at being an employee, you can know you could say, good is what's the same
Starting point is 00:44:47 across all those things. And PSJA would call that a scheme, just so you know, because a scheme is what's the same across multiple different things. So, and the scheme is the basis of abstraction for PSJA. So, good person is the sum total, or the commonalities between being a good teacher, student, employee, etc. Okay? So then we'll say, okay, so now we can move the problem of analysis one level down
Starting point is 00:45:16 and we can say, okay, well what does it mean to be a good parent? And then you'd say, well, maybe you have to have a good job because otherwise you and your child starve, but we'll forget about that one for a moment. And then you might say, well, maybe you have to have a good job, because otherwise you and your child starve, but we'll forget about that one for a moment. And then you might say, well, you have to do a good job and take care of your family. Okay, so you notice we're zeroing in on an element of the good here. And it's still an abstraction.
Starting point is 00:45:38 Care for your family, that's an abstraction. So then you might say, well, one of the things you would do if you cared for your family is play with the baby and then another might be complete a meal. So it will take the play with the baby example. And then there's three things you can do with your baby. You can peek at it. They like that. That's all I've to permanent say. There's nothing more thrilling to a baby than discovering that you're still there when you disappear. Like a baby, you can amuse a baby for hours with that discovery, because it's by no means self-evident to the baby.
Starting point is 00:46:07 That's a Piagetian notion. They lack object permanence. So as far as they're concerned, it's a hell of a shock when you disappear because they were just appreciating you being there. And then they're just a shock when you reappear. And you can, it's so interesting to watch them because when a baby gets startled,
Starting point is 00:46:23 like when you get startled, you know, you might go like that. If you because when a baby gets startled, like when you get startled, you might go like that. If you're really startled, a baby, it's like, it's whole body is startled, right? It makes a weird face and it moves its arms and its legs and then it takes it like five seconds to recover. They really have a startled reflex. And they'll often laugh.
Starting point is 00:46:40 I'm sure you've seen the laughing babies on YouTube, because of course there's like two billion views of laughing babies. She's a good thing. It shows that we like babies. I think there's one that's very famous where a baby is reacting to his mother either sneezing or coughing.
Starting point is 00:46:54 You know, one, God, it's absolutely hilarious. This baby just has a fit. And what's basically happening is it's mother coughs. I think it's coughs or maybe sneezes. Oh, okay. Oh, yes, she blows her nose. So she makes this weird elephant-like noise, and the baby is like shocked that mother could do this. And then it gets surprised at its own startle,
Starting point is 00:47:14 and that makes it laugh. So that's a really interesting phenomena from a Piagetian perspective, because one of the things that Piaget noticed that children did, because they could imitate, was imitate themselves. That's how they got to know who they were. So for example, if a child would say a child would ask the other dog to knock that off,
Starting point is 00:47:33 when they're eating, well that's a hell of a thing to discover. You know, if you move your arm, just a little bit, that'll fall off, that's cool. So then maybe you can do that 60, 70 times, well you're supposed to be eating. And the other thing that's really cool about doing that is that mom will immediately rush over
Starting point is 00:47:52 and pick them up and put it back. And this is an excellent game. You can entertain yourself with that for like a month. And basically what you're discovering is the relationship between your movements and gravity, right? It's like this is a majorly discovery. It's no wonder you're obsessed with it. So, so anyways, the point with regards to amitation is maybe the baby does this
Starting point is 00:48:12 accidentally to begin with that startled them. And then it's put up again and then they think, well, that was cool. I wonder if we can do that again. And then they're pretty happy about that. And then they'll keep practicing that until they get like expert at doing that. So instead of using like gross body movements to do it, which is how they start, they get
Starting point is 00:48:35 finer and finer and finer body movements until they've got a whole not-kate light thing off a table down. And it can even be fascinating as an adult. Some of you probably play table hockey with a quarter. You know how to do that. Put a quarter on a table. It's not very complicated.
Starting point is 00:48:51 You make little go. Your job is to flip the thing so that it just sits that far over the table and so that the opponent can flip it up. That's like your opportunity to score a goal. My point is playing with how things move in relationship to friction and gravity is so engrossing to human beings that you can even make a game out of it
Starting point is 00:49:12 if you're a relatively bored and stupid adult. So I played that game all the time with my son, by the way, so I put myself in that category as well. Now, what the child is doing is, it'll accidentally, in some ways, bumble into something interesting. And then it recognizes that it's interesting in sense because that's a reflex action on its part.
Starting point is 00:49:34 And then once it recognizes that it's had that reflex action, which is kind of, it's pleasurable, as long as it's not too intense, it's at least interesting, then they'll try to imitate what they just did. So part of the way you master yourself is by imitating yourself after you've accidentally done something interesting.
Starting point is 00:49:52 It's so smart, so that's a PSGedia notion. So anyways, you're playing with the baby, and you can play a peep with the baby, that's a good one. You're teaching it that you're still there even though you went away, and you can tickle the baby, which is best done in moderation.
Starting point is 00:50:06 Because it's not exactly clear that babies enjoy that. They just laugh when you do that so that you like them and don't throw them up window when they're being annoying. And then you can clean the baby too, say. Those are elements of the care of a baby. I know that's not very sophisticated, but you know, those are some of the basics. Now, the thing that's different about playing with baby and tickle baby is that play with baby is an abstraction. Because tickle baby is an action, right? And so when you're trying to
Starting point is 00:50:33 solve the mind body problem, this is how you solve it. The mind is those abstractions. The body is the actions that those abstractions ground themselves in. So it's not like the mind is attached to the body. It's the mind is a sequence of hierarchically arranged abstractions, the bottom level of which are non-abstractions. They're actions. There's a qualitative transformation
Starting point is 00:50:54 at the bottom of the highest resolution level of the hierarchy. It's action. Now, you can understand PHA very, very easily if you understand that the way a baby develops as far as PHA is concerned, it's from the bottom up. So the baby starts with, it's lying in its crib. It's a useless thing.
Starting point is 00:51:16 It's laying there. It can't even focus its eyes. It's just barely getting going. So it's sort of floating in space, trying to figure out what's going on. You know, and it doesn't even know that it has arms really, just sort of detects these things off to the side. You've lost it, bump itself in the face with their, its arms, or scratch itself, because you have to cut babies' nails really short, otherwise they scratch themselves,
Starting point is 00:51:39 because their arms are just, you know, wandering around randomly. And the baby starts from a piagetian perspective. The baby starts to learn what it's body is capable of doing. And some of that just discovers by accident. Now, there's no doubt some natural neurological progression that's going on. So the baby is learning along the past that babies can learn. But still, from a piagetian perspective, the baby is doing quite a bit of exploring
Starting point is 00:52:07 to facilitate its neurological development. And more complex animals make it doing that in the womb, trying out their legs and so on, so that they can run as soon as they get out. So it's not even obvious that all that sort of thing is reflexive and automatic in an animal that is more active when it's born. So the baby begins by basically it sort of develops from the middle outward.
Starting point is 00:52:32 So what's the baby got when it first pops into the world? Well, it can't see very well, though it can focus on about 12 inches, which conveniently is about the distance that it's had is from its mother's eyes if it's breastfeeding. So that's good for social communication. So the mother can gaze at the baby and the baby can gaze at the mother. And for some reason, they both find that fascinating. So, baby's also very wired up around the mouth.
Starting point is 00:52:56 So it can use its mouth quite a lot and it can use its tongue. Those come pre-wired. So, and this is kind of a frightening observation. So at the beginning, the baby really is oral because that's all that works. Like it could smell too. You know, but it's voluntary capacity for action. It's really centered around its mouth.
Starting point is 00:53:18 And you'll notice that babies, infant, are toddlers, even loved to put things in their mouth. My son, when he was a kid, used to go in the backyard. We did feed him, so that was the reason. He'd go in the backyard and pick up acorns, and stuff his cheeks with him, just like a chipmunk. And so, you know, then we'd take him up to the bath, and we'd have to like, dig all the acorns out of his cheeks,
Starting point is 00:53:39 and he did that for, oh God, way longer than you'd expect any same child to do it. So, but the reason that he would put things in his mouth, and you think about this, is there's nothing that's a better exploratory organ than your tongue. I mean, check out one of your teeth. Bloody thing feels like it's about this big to your tongue. As you'll notice, you know, I never get a tooth removed.
Starting point is 00:54:01 It's like the grand canyon was just instantiated inside your mouth. And then of course, your tongue will work like mad of its own accord to investigate every tiny little crevice in that new hole because your brain wants to know what's your mouth and what isn't so that if there's something in your mouth that either should or shouldn't be, a, it can tell it from you and b, it can tell whether or not it's supposed to be there. Turns out to be very important, right? Because while there's tooth decay, that's a problem. But there's also insects and poison and all sorts of other things that you shouldn't put
Starting point is 00:54:34 in your mouth. So, don't underestimate the degree to which you can zoom around the world like a Hoover vacuum cleaner and pick up a god-off amount of information. And of course, you're also feeding that way, obviously. And so you're checking out your mother with your mouth and your tongue in a very meaningful way, too. And while you're feeding at the breast, you're establishing the basis of social relationships.
Starting point is 00:54:57 Now, this puts a bit of a warp into the Piagetian hypothesis because Piaget sort of assumed that the baby builds himself from the bottom up, right? But one of the things you have to understand is that the baby is always picking up how to behave and see in a very, very, very social context, even when it's so young that Piaget would regard it as primarily egocentric, because what the hell does a baby know about you? You know, but the mother is teaching the child how to act right from the time it's a little tiny thing.
Starting point is 00:55:29 Like, if it's going to breastfeed, it has to do it in a relatively civilized manner. Because if it bites his mother or her mother, which a baby can do, they can really chomp you a good one if their minds are made up to do that. It's like unpleasant consequences are going to ensue. At minimum, the mother is going to startle and, you know, stop feeding the baby, and, you know, maybe she'll put the baby down or
Starting point is 00:55:51 or God only knows. One time when my son was very young, I guess he was about 30 months, he'd just learned to walk. And he walked up to my wife who was wearing shorts, and he bit her right here. And a good charm, he was just teething, eh? And she reflects, they show him his reflex, and he's like, he must have flown six feet. It's like, that's how you socialize children against sudden bias in the middle. So even the simulta with alterations of their behavior take place within an intensely social context. So even at this level, the society is helping guide and restrict the development of the child's motor activities.
Starting point is 00:56:35 Now children tend to develop, as I said, they develop gross body movements first, so they kind of learn to fling their arms. So maybe you put a mobile in the crib for your baby to look at. By the way, most mobiles will notice, maybe they're fish. So your parent you're standing here, and this is the side of the fish. And this is the bottom of the fish. And that's what the baby is looking at. It's like the baby is looking at lines. Because the fish are here for the adults. It's like, that's what the baby is looking at. It's like the baby is looking at lines, because the fish are here for the adults.
Starting point is 00:57:06 It's like, that's a stupid mobile. You get that fish turned over, so that baby you can see the fish, and then you make them out of like black and white, because babies are very good at picking up high contrast. And that's a baby mobile, rather than a mother mobile. So you got to decide whether the mobile is for you or for your baby.
Starting point is 00:57:23 So anyways, you put the mobile above the baby and you kind of want to put it within limb length and then the baby will watch this thing. You know as maybe it's annoyed to death by this that we don't know it's like, you know, and then it'll it'll sort of flail about like it does. It's not very well-controlled batter, maybe it'll flail an arm or a leg, and now it'll get lucky and it'll nail one of those fish, and now it's starving to be. And so, usually, it might cry then,
Starting point is 00:57:53 because that might have just, if it's a neurotic baby, it'll cry. It's like, that's too much for today. But if it's kind of an outgoing exploratory baby, then the next thing it's really good to want to do is to figure out how to make that fish move again. And then it'll sit there and practice flailing its leg. It's like it's throwing its leg at the fish. And if it gets lucky, it'll nail in a good one,
Starting point is 00:58:16 and then that'll make it laugh. And then, you know, it'll practice doing that over a sequence of babies are persistent, man. So when my daughter was little, she was about 18 months old. We bought her this little cardboard box that had little cardboard Disney books in it. And she didn't care what the Disney books were. What she was really interested in
Starting point is 00:58:35 is getting those three books out of the Disney box and then trying to get them back in. Because it turned out that was quite difficult because they fit tightly. So this was a great puzzle for her, because she was still getting the whole coordination thing going. And she'd sit for like three hours, getting those books
Starting point is 00:58:55 in that box, and that was like a toy for a week, till she figured it out. And then she was on to make her and better things. But that's a big deal, right? You can imagine what you're doing neurologically when you're doing that. It's like, first of all, you got to grip that book properly. Second, you got to orient it precisely.
Starting point is 00:59:15 Third, as you add the additional books to the box, the shape changes, because the books flop over, you know, because they'll flop over diagonally. So the shape changes, you've got to figure out how to adjust that. And then the book itself will open, and that'll get in the way. So you've got to keep the book closed. And, you know, the tolerance is like at 18th of an inch.
Starting point is 00:59:35 So, you want to master that. And like some people don't. I had a client once who had a very low fluid intelligence, probably 7580. You would have known it by looking at him, but he couldn't find employment. Surprise, surprise. There's no jobs in our society for people
Starting point is 00:59:54 who are at that end of the cognitive distribution. I got an involuntary job at one point, and his job was to put paper in envelopes, had to fold it up in three, because that's how you fold up a piece of paper, and then you have to put it in envelopes, had to fold it up in three, because that's how you fold up a piece of paper, and then you have to put it in envelope. But that actually turns out to be hyper complex. I probably trained him for 28 to 30 hours to do that. And because when you do it next time, or you could do it now, you just think about what you're doing. First of all, merely by observing the piece of paper,
Starting point is 01:00:24 you have to figure out how to make the first fold. And it better be done close to 1.3. Because if it isn't, when you make the second fold, you're going to compound your error, and then you're going to find to your chagrin, then a piece of paper does not fit in the envelopes because the envelope is exactly the same size as the piece of paper.
Starting point is 01:00:40 So if you're out by your estimates, say, a quarter of an inch, in your first fold, you're out by half inch in your second fold, it's like it's not going in there. Or if you don't fold it completely at 90 degrees, you know, so the edges lie down, say you're out by a sixteenth of an inch, and then you do that again. So now you're out by eight. It won't fit in sideways. So then you have to mangle the envelope to get the paper in there. And then by the time you've done mangling,
Starting point is 01:01:05 the envelope not only does it look ugly, but it will not go through an automatic sorting machine. So and then added to that was the problem that on these pieces of paper, there were often photographs stable because he was working for a charity. And but the photographs were always stable at exactly the same place on the piece of paper. So then you have to look at the piece of paper, and you'd have to figure out where the photograph is, and then you'd have to figure out how place on the piece of paper. So then he'd have to look at the piece of paper,
Starting point is 01:01:25 and he'd have to figure out where the photograph is, and then he'd have to figure out how to fold the piece of paper, so that he didn't bend the photograph so that it would still fit in the envelope. And that just used to, he'd just sweat himself to death trying to solve that problem, because there was time pressure, too. So, and then there's an added level of complexity on top of that. And the reason I'm telling you this is because you'll get some sense of how simple actions are
Starting point is 01:01:50 aggregated into increasingly complex operations. Some of the envelopes were French and some of them were English, because it was Canada. And so you couldn't put a French letter in the English envelope, the French ones had to go in a separate pile. And then there was a huge stack, say of English letters, and a huge stack of English envelopes, all of which hypothetically had been stacked so that each one corresponded to the other. But now, and then, one of them would be out of alignment.
Starting point is 01:02:15 So then you had to figure out whether it was the letters that were out of the alignment or the envelopes. Well, that's a high complexity working memory problem, and that just bring them to a standstill. So, anyways, during typical development, you develop those incredibly fine motor skills, eased early, which is part of the reason why it's kind of nice to have your baby toddler infant
Starting point is 01:02:39 do something by itself, like sit there with a box till it gets a little bored, and then it'll start to figure out what to do with the box. And that's part of play and that's part of the development of its embodied conceptual structure. So, okay, so you basically chain these things together. Now, the way that PSJA, he thought,
Starting point is 01:03:00 well, what's the motivation for doing this? So the motivation essentially is that, well, sometimes's the motivation for doing this? So the motivation, essentially, is that, well, sometimes it can be sort of random. You accidentally do something interesting, eventually I'd repeat it. But as you sort of develop the number of times if you do something accidentally that results in something interesting starts to decline.
Starting point is 01:03:19 Now, part of what happens to you as you mature is that the opportunities that are provided for you by your body as you exercise and develop it, start to really ramp up and develop so that you can do a lot more interesting things with your body by the time you're two, then you can, when you're nine months old, for example, you can zoom around, which just opens up a whole universe of things to pull over and dump over and pull off tables and you know, and there's things lying around that you can write on walls with and you can pull out cupboards and it's like it's paradise for an exploratory baby.
Starting point is 01:03:55 So, but what happens is that as the child puts himself or herself together physically and develops additional skills, then they can elicit additional manifestations of novelty from the world. So for example, here's a good example. So the child finally manages to get itself upright, which is like a colossal, colossal, amazing accomplishment. You know, I mean, first of all, that's hard to do. You know, like no, there aren't too that's hard to do. You know, like, no, there are two-legged creatures in the world except for us, you know?
Starting point is 01:04:28 And we're so good at this, you can even, it's not wonderful, I learned that when I was two. So, you know, you can do these amazing things with your body, but the child is like trying to put itself together neurologically, and it's a hell of an operation to get this column of bone. It's like jellyfish on a bone, you know, or stack bones, and you gotta get the thing
Starting point is 01:04:49 to stand up upright. God, ridiculously difficult. So the child manages that. There's a lot of pain in English associated with that, right? Because those little creatures, they just fall down. You know, they got these little short arms, so they're not much good at protecting themselves from impact.
Starting point is 01:05:04 They're just bouncing off the walls like mad in the floor when they're trying to learn to walk. Then they find to get themselves upright and taunt them along and fall and so on. But then, you know, then the world turns into a different place because all the sudden now it's stand up underneath the table. That's the, that's a fun thing to do for the first time. It's like, WAH! So now your new body has taught you something about the world that you did not know. So what Piaget would say is, well, you've got this scheme work done, which is the standing-up scheme. And it turns out that that works pretty well if you're in an empty space.
Starting point is 01:05:41 But if you're in a closed space with a low ceiling, the whole standing up thing is just not going to produce the results intended. So you stand up and whack yourself, and that's a sign that it's time to update your representational structures. And so you can think of that technically as the emergence of the normally into what would have otherwise been a conceptually protected space. Now, PHA has this idea of equilibration. So let's say your baby crawls. And let's say it's an equilibrated crawler, which
Starting point is 01:06:15 means it's kind of an expert. And babies get pretty expert crawlers, by about say, 12, 30 months, taking zoom around. And they know how to not to bump into things and how to crawl around without hurting themselves. So when they crawl around, only the things that they want to have happen have, most of the time, and they're recuperating at that point
Starting point is 01:06:36 because they're actions and their conceptions of the consequence of those actions match. They've got no problem, what they expect or want to have happen when they crawl is what happens. That baby's, as mastered a developmental stage. So that's the stage idea. And that's the equilibrated stage idea. Acroboration is a brilliant idea.
Starting point is 01:06:58 And I'll tell you why in a minute. But then all of a sudden, the baby learns to stand up. It's like, oh, oh, an advance, but it's like a revolutionary advance, right? It's equivalent to a revolution in science, which is why Piaget mentioned Koon. It's like, well, now I'm a standing creature. The whole crawling expertise is hardly worth it at all.
Starting point is 01:07:22 There's some transfer of knowledge, but the new universe that's made accessible to the baby by its now-dawning ability to stand forces it to revolutionize its entire cognitive structure. Now, I'll give you, I wanna tell you a very quick story because it's a very good one. So, this is a good indication of the function of dream in play. So when my daughter was three, she couldn't talk very well. She learned to speak late. We lived
Starting point is 01:07:53 in Boston and when she learned to speak, she developed a Boston accent, which we were completely flier-gasted by because we didn't have a Boston accent and neither did any of our neighbors. Turns out there's a speech impediment that sounds just like a Boston accent and neither did any of our neighbors. Turns out there's a speech impediment that sounds just like a Boston accent. But anyway, she usually goes away, accepted Boston. Yeah. So anyway, she was three.
Starting point is 01:08:19 And when her brother was born, there's always the possibility of sibling rivalry, right? And for good reason, it's like, time to get rid of that thing. All it's doing is taking a fall of mum's attention, you know? Like, that's a hard reality for a child who's under three to manage. Because they're still pretty dependant and it's like, well, why should they be liking this horrible noisy attention grabbing interloper? So we tried to, we tried to teach her right from the time he came home, her brother, that the idea was she was going to lose something, which was some parental attention,
Starting point is 01:08:53 especially for her mother, but she was potentially going to gain something, which was maturity, independence, and the possibility of a new relationship. Now we could tell her that some parents will tell their three-year-olds that. It's like, in your three-year-olds, it's learning to look at you when you talk. But they're actually hearing, you know, how dogs and cartoons hear people talk. It's like, wow, wow, wow, wow, wow, wow, wow, wow,
Starting point is 01:09:16 that's how children hear you talk when you're talking about something like independence. Like, what the hell did they know about that? Way up there on hierarchy. But we tried to teach her to interact with the baby in such a way that she would elicit positive feedback from him and so that they could be friends. And then we also taught her that she should take care
Starting point is 01:09:33 of the baby and that gave her something to do. So that worked. We also didn't let them tease each other. Not much. They're gonna tease each other a bit because siblings often bloody have paid each other when they're young. And sometimes that just destroys the relationship
Starting point is 01:09:47 for the whole life. You don't want that, because you're stuck with your damn brother till he dies, hopefully, or till you do. So, anyway, so, my daughter was taking care of this little guy, and she used to watch him on the steps and sort of shepherd him around,
Starting point is 01:10:03 and she was pretty good at it. And then one day he started to walk. And she kept talking about her baby. And we told her that, well, he wasn't really a baby anymore. And we didn't realize that this was setting off a cognitive revolution of the Piagetian sort, because of course, she kind of grown attached to this baby, just like mothers do. And mothers sometimes get so attached to their babies that they really don't want them to stop being babies
Starting point is 01:10:27 And so they end up living in their house till they're 55, you know wanting the destruction of the world So you don't want to have that happen So but it was happening in a microwave with her. It's like well, there's this baby And I spent a lot of time getting used to it and figuring out what to do with it Now it's not a baby anymore. What the hell is it? So she had this amazing dream and it ties back to the shomomic stuff that I taught you guys about earlier. So this is the dream.
Starting point is 01:10:51 She dreamt that the baby crawled into a hole in the backyard. Now the way the hole got there was that a tree from the park beside us had moved into our backyard and then had burned down and left this hole. And then the hole was full of water. And so the baby crawled into the water and it reduced him to a skeleton and then there was a bug in the water and the bug pulled him out and-old voice, and I go to all type downloads. Absolutely spectacular, because of the straight, shamanic dream, like the tree burned out, and so that's a transformation motif. And then water is the place of rebirth.
Starting point is 01:11:31 And the kid was dissolved into a skeleton. And the little bug that Pokemon is like a representation of the underlying process that guides transformation. For no brain was like working like mad, trying to figure out continuity over change. That's a tough one, right? Because it's almost like a butterfly reaching from a cocoon. It's a big deal. Baby, did toddler, that's a big difference.
Starting point is 01:11:56 She's supposed to figure out, well, that's the same thing. Well, no, it's not. So underneath her unconscious mind, three worlds are not stupid, even though they can't talk. They've got this brain that's like 3.5 billion years old. It's not stupid. So, anyway, now what was I telling you that? Oh, well, it's another indication of how. So, the transformations of cognitive structure are forced upon a child, at least in part
Starting point is 01:12:28 by the development of increasing physical ability, right? Because as you increase its physical ability and capacity, then the world transforms and then your cognitive processes have to transform to keep up with that, and sometimes that's a radical transformation. So PSJ's fundamental hypothesis is that part of the reason that people are motivated to underdole cognitive transformations
Starting point is 01:12:50 and learning per se is because as they mature, they automatically come into contact with anomalous information, information that they cannot process from within the confines of their current world model. And because they can't process it, it interferes with them getting what they want. And so they're motivated to keep their car
Starting point is 01:13:08 going up structures updated. So in some times, the revolutions occur at a micro level. And that might be like when you're learning the difference between this and this, it's like it's a major difference, but it doesn't disrupt the whole fabric of your conceptual universe. Whereas a divorce might, the baby transformed me into a toddler might,
Starting point is 01:13:30 puberty does, it's because you just finally got used to your body, you're 12 years old, hey? So you're sort of at the pinnacle of childhood, you're like an adult child. You know what you're doing? And 11 and 12 year olds are often lovely creatures, because they're pretty mature, and they've got their act together. And then almost sudden bang, sex hormones kick in. It's like, you are no longer the same thing. And neither is anyone you interact with.
Starting point is 01:13:54 And so it's like turbulence for 3, 4, 5, 20, 30 years until you sort of sort that out, which you never really do. And so you can see how the physiological transformations that are intended on development, some biologically predicated, and some an emergent consequence of learning, disrupt the cognitive structures that people use to orient themselves in the world, and that some of those disruptions are sort of low-key equivalent to normal science, not to be assimilation, and some of those disruptions are sort of low key equivalent to normal science,
Starting point is 01:14:25 not to be assimilation, and some of them much broader, and that's equivalent to accommodation. A simulation means you learn something new that you can already handle using the constructs and schemas that you have at hand. You know, so maybe you have to pick the thing up like this instead of like this, but you already know this and you know that so who cares It's like a little minor alteration and sometimes well know you have to re-adap your whole body in order to handle the next level of Complex problem solving learning to drive is like that learning to write a bicycle is like that and you can see that there's sort of a There's not really normal versus revolutionary transformations. There's sort of a continuum that maps themselves onto that hierarchy, I showed you. So at the very highest levels of resolution, its minor transformations, then at the higher
Starting point is 01:15:16 levels of abstraction, you can blow out a whole huge chunks of yourself. So it's a continuum. But Piaget and Kuhon sort of conceptualized it as a dichotomy. Alright. Oh yes, this is the, this, this is the thing I really want to get at to. This is, it's so brilliant. And so Piaget was concerned about morality. And he's one of the few psychologists I think who like, he nailed it. This is, this is like the most important thing Piaget discovered. And maybe it's one of the most important things that a psychologist has ever discovered. It is like the most important thing the J.J. discovered. And maybe it's one of the most important things
Starting point is 01:15:45 that the psychologist has ever discovered. It's like, how do you become moral? Well, we already mentioned that when you acquire a behavior, you inevitably acquire it in a social context. So what that means is that right from the time you're little and learning things, the demands of society are encoded in the behaviors that you're allowed to manifest. And so science, to some degree, by the time you behave,
Starting point is 01:16:07 the way you do behave when you're three, if you've been properly socialized, you are already act out the embodied moral structure of your entire community. So when you talk about laws, you say there's a body of laws. And the body of laws used to be the king. So you have to do with the king's self. but now the body of laws is an abstract conceptual representation, but those representations are actually semantic representations of allowable and not allowable behavior, and so by
Starting point is 01:16:37 the time you're three and you're a law biting three-year-old, which means you're well-socialized, you're already acting in accordance with the law, the patterns that characterize your being, the behavior of patterns that characterize your being, have already been molded into the patterns that you manifest. And so what that means in some sense, is you're already, as Nietzsche would say, an unconscious advocate of your culture
Starting point is 01:16:58 because you're acting it out. Now, here's an example of how that can be transformed into actual moral knowledge. So, he has to go to study kids playing a game. So, here's an example of how that can be transported to actual moral knowledge. So he has to go to study kids playing a game. So here's an example. So there's a bunch of kids that are standing around in the playground and they're all playing helicopter. Maybe they just got sticks and they're always, you know, when they're flying helicopters
Starting point is 01:17:17 around and they're all doing this and maybe they're diving in each other. So what's happened is each kid wants to have a helicopter and each kid wants to be a good helicopter pilot, but each kid wants to be a good helicopter pilot in the way that other kids appreciate, well, there are being good helicopter pilots. That's tough, eh? So not only do you have to figure out how to coordinate your behavior,
Starting point is 01:17:37 you have to figure out how to coordinate your behavior with other people coordinating their behavior in such a way that the shared activity not only does not come to a stop, but proceeds in an enjoyable way. Jesus, that's really tough. Now, if your kid can play well with other kids, that's what they've managed. Okay, so you've got four kids doing this. And there's rules, actually there's not.
Starting point is 01:18:01 There's rituals that the children are embodying that they've agreed upon while they set up the game. They've just sort of bashed the ideas against each other and they came up with the solution and you can tell that because the game is continuing and everyone is having fun. It's like is everyone playing nice? Yes, that's an equilibrated play state and it's a moral organization because all the children are participating voluntarily towards a shared end. And V.H. for P.A.J. that was the model of a functional society. That's so smart. So now imagine that a kid comes along, and maybe he's a popular kid. So he has a reasonable chance of getting into this play group.
Starting point is 01:18:35 He'll still be rebuffed like 50% at the time. Whereas if he's not a popular kid, it's like, forget. They're just not going to let him into the little helicopter circle. And that's probably because he's developmentally delayed, or in some other way, he doesn't understand how he has to configure his behavior so that he can enter this complex dramatic scene without disrupting it.
Starting point is 01:18:55 Or maybe he's one of those kids who said, I don't want to point helicopter. We should place something else. It's like, no, unless you're like a creative genius, that's a really good idea. So what the popular kids do is they sort of side lost to the group. Maybe they find the stick, because they watch what they're doing.
Starting point is 01:19:14 And maybe they find the stick, and they start going, with the stick, and they're looking to see if that stick helicopter behavior is acceptable to them here. And maybe one of their friends sort of notices that they're playing helicopters, and they open up the circle, and that gets to come in and play helicopters. But he might fail at that, even if he's popular,
Starting point is 01:19:34 because once the little play thing is going, it's like a play, right? It's got a dramatic structure. You can't just come in there and start playing basketball or something. So now, what B.S.A. noticed was that when children were playing a game, maybe they were playing marbles, and that's a rough game because you can win and lose at marbles. And you might say, well, children shouldn't play competitive games,
Starting point is 01:19:56 but if you say that, that's a sure sign that you don't know what the hell you're talking about. Because all games are competitive and cooperative at the same time, if they share a single goal, and it's really not a game unless there's a goal, because games have goals. And so what the children do is they organize themselves so they all decide what constitutes an acceptable goal. And the goal with marbles is win some marbles, and lose some marbles without whining about. That's another goal, and that's an important thing to learn. So the children say, well, here's
Starting point is 01:20:26 the foundation of our local moral universe. Here's the rules that we're going to abide by. And a lot of those are ritualistic. They might have learned them from other kids. So they play marbles. You extract out a kid and you say, what are the rules for marbles? And if the kids like seven, they don't know. They can't tell you.
Starting point is 01:20:42 But if you put them in with a bunch of other seven-year-olds, they can play marbles perfectly well. So what that means is the morality is coded in their behavior, or in the behavior of the group, like a bunch of bees, you know, because there's knowledge in a hydropheas or maybe a school of fish. So it's embedded in the child's behavior and in their patterns of action across time. And then if you pull them out, they can only give a partial account of it. They're not conscious of the morality. It's like you pull a wolf out of a wolf pack and say, well, what's up with the wolf pack? And the wolf bites you because that's
Starting point is 01:21:15 how it answers questions. It's not going to tell you about the rules. It's only later in development that the children become conscious of the rules. And then they become very irritated if people break them. And then only much later do they start to understand that the rules can be adjusted by mutual agreement. And that is associated with the P.A.J.
Starting point is 01:21:34 Indian development of higher order morality. It's brilliant. You learn your actions. The actions are conditioned by other people. The actions are integrated into a voluntary game. Then the actions are integrated into a series of voluntary games, that's social interaction. Then you learn the descriptions.
Starting point is 01:21:53 Then you learn how to manipulate the descriptions. Brilliant. Pompant up to hire Satya P. Asgette and Therian and Nachelle. Constructed as a Nachelle. See you on Thursday. you

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.