The Jordan B. Peterson Podcast - 146. Sigmund Freud and the Dynamic Unconscious

Episode Date: November 22, 2020

In this lecture, 9th in the 2017 personality series, Dr. Peterson discusses some of the essentials of Sigmund Freud's theories, concentrating on his conceptualizations of the dynamic (living) unconsci...ous.-Thanks to our sponsors:Wondery"Hosted by Taraji P. Henson, Wondery (Dr. Death, Dirty John, The Shrink Next Door) and Universal Music Group present Jacked: Rise of the New Jack Sound. The story begins with a keyboard player in the heart of Harlem: Teddy Riley. Teddy’s entire world was music. Playing it. Composing it. Producing it. Teddy and his friend Timmy Gatling followed their dreams and started a revolution in the world of hip-hop and R&B.They crafted hit after hit, and were on top of the world. Their group’s quick rise to fame came with betrayals and broken friendships. Tensions within the band began to flare, and rivalries with other artists turned violent. As the stakes grew higher, their troubled manager pushed the group past their breaking point, with deadly results. Listen today at http://wondery.fm/Jacked_JBP"-For Advertising Inquiries, visit https://www.advertisecast.com/TheJordanBPetersonPodcast 

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Welcome to season three, episode 33 of the Jordan B. Peterson podcast. I'm Michaela Peterson, Jordan's child with a better sense of humor than the other child. It's so good Julian doesn't listen to these intros or perhaps I will find out Monday if he does. This episode is from Dad's personality series Freud and the dynamic unconscious. In this episode he explores Sigmund Freud's theories and delves into Freud's work on the unconscious mind. A few quick updates. One, if you haven't checked out Dad's video course on an intro to personality, it is on sale this Black Friday from November 26th this Thursday, 2 December 4th. It's $60
Starting point is 00:00:43 for hours of video content instead of the regular price of $140. It gives a university level introduction to personality and is incredibly insightful. If you think you're dating a psychopath or a pushover, find out. Check it out at jordanbeapeterson.com slash personality on sale November 26th. Second update, tomorrow November 23rd, 2020, my dad is releasing a YouTube video with a huge announcement. 10 a.m. EST, be sure to check it out. Like I said, it's a doozy. Enjoy this episode and your week and the course if you buy it
Starting point is 00:01:18 and the announcement tomorrow. Also, I released an episode on my podcast with Matthew McConaughey. So if you want to check that out, please do. Also, I released an episode on my podcast with Matthew McConaughey. So if you want to check that out, please do. And I'll kill appears some videos on YouTube. Okay, I'm done. Enjoy this episode. So unto Sigmund Freud, we're going to give him some what short shrift I'm afraid because we only have an hour to
Starting point is 00:01:50 talk about Freud, but that's okay. We can get a fair way through it. He's still persona non-grata, I would say, among experimental psychologists and probably clinical psychologists as well, but that seems to me to be very unfair. Freud is one of those thinkers who, all that's left are his mistakes. And the reason for that is that everything that he discovered or put forward
Starting point is 00:02:18 is so entrenched in our culture now that we think it's self-evident. And so everything correct has been assimilated, and that just leaves everything that's more or less floating on top to look wrong. And but Freud is also one of those thinkers who was always wrong in an interesting way. And that's very useful. And so I also think that many of the things
Starting point is 00:02:38 that he put his finger on that are still disputed, for example, the idea of the Etappas complex are much more useful than people are willing to admit, especially in the clinical realm, because the etepus complex, which we'll talk about quite a bit, is actually a description of a fairly stable form of familial psychopathology where child gets trapped within the confines of a family, because the relationship with one parent or the other or both is so tight that they can't
Starting point is 00:03:13 break beyond it, and maybe because of their own inability to move towards independence, but more frequently because of what you might describe as a kind of conspiracy between the son and the parent or the child and the parent that prevents them from moving towards autonomous life and keeps them in a state of essentially a state of childhood dependence. Freud said, I started my professional activity as a neurologist trying to bring Larry Le leaf to my neurotic patients under the influence of an older friend and by my own efforts I discovered some important new facts about the unconscious in psychic life. The role of instinctual urges and so on, out of these findings grew a new science psychoanalysis, a part of psychology and a new treatment for the neuroses.
Starting point is 00:04:03 I had to pay heavily for this bit of good luck. People did not believe in my facts and thought my theory is unsavory. Resistance was strong and unrelenting. In the end, I succeeded in inquiring pupils and building it up in international psychoanalyst association, but the struggle is not over. He made that recording just shortly before he died.
Starting point is 00:04:22 He moved to England to escape the Nazis. Before Freud, I guess, the mind was, it's complicated because Freud, of course, was not the only person to be thinking along the lines that he thought. Pierre Genet, who was one of his teachers, had originated and started to develop many of the ideas that I would say were popularized by Freud started to develop many of the ideas that I would
Starting point is 00:04:45 say were popularized by Freud. But the idea of the unconscious mind was not, certainly not as well developed prior to Freud as it became afterwards. Before that, I suppose you might say that in so far as people thought of the mind at all, they thought in philosophical terms, and the mind would be that part of you that you're aware of. In the Cartesian sense, Descartes said, I think, therefore I am. And it kind of seems, in some sense, self-evident that you're aware of and have control over
Starting point is 00:05:24 the contents of your own mind. But that was what Freud really questioned. And he questioned it deeply, said, well, first of all, the idea that you're one thing, like one mind is a dubious idea to begin with because people are full of internal contradictions. And then the idea that your mind is all of one type, it's all of one form, was also very questionable, as far as Freud was concerned, because you could be fractionated into subcomponents,
Starting point is 00:05:52 and know the idea, for example, that your anger or your sexual desire could be an autonomous part of your personality in some sense, that it could overtake you and control you. That's really a Freudian idea. And one of the classic Freudian ideas really is that people are made out of subpersonalities and those subpersonalities are alive. And that's one of the things I really like about the psychoanalytic thinkers, because
Starting point is 00:06:16 even the psychologists who say over the last 30 years are there about since maybe longer now. Anyway, since the demise of behavior is a massive ideology, and the admission by psychologists that there was an active unconscious or many active unconsciouses, which is a better way of thinking about it, psychologists still really haven't come to terms with the idea in any deep sense that these unconscious processes are living things. You know, they're, when psychologists talk, for example, about the cognitive unconscious, they're talking about something that they describe in more machine-like, with more machine-like metaphors, and that's not reasonable. You understand things a lot better if
Starting point is 00:07:02 you understand that the subcomp components that make up people, the fragmentary bits of them, and also the biological subsystems that are part and parcel of your being, are much more intelligently viewed as personalities. They're kind of unidimensional personalities in some sense, so that, for example, if you're angry, you're nothing but angry. I mean, that's an overstatement, obviously, or if you're afraid, you're nothing but afraid, or if you're angry, you're nothing but angry. I mean, that's an overstatement, obviously. Or if you're afraid, you're nothing but afraid. Or if you're hungry, you're nothing but hunger. Well, that's certainly true if you get hungry enough for thirsty or too hot or any of those things.
Starting point is 00:07:33 You kind of collapse to a simpler personality that only has one motivation in mind. And we'll talk a lot as we progress about the grounding of those unidimensional, motivational systems in biology. But I'd have to say that Freud was among the first, at least the first to synthesize a coherent theory of this multiplicity, and to put it forth,
Starting point is 00:07:57 while also insisting that much of what was happening to you and inside of you was not immediately accessible to your awareness. And it's a very profound discovery. It means among many other things that you can formulate ideas. First of all, it means that you can act out things that you don't understand for reasons that you don't understand. It also means that your memory can contain things that's represented in one way, but that can't be understood in another.
Starting point is 00:08:34 So, for example, and we know this is true because there are independent memory systems. There's an independent memory system for procedures. That's for actions. There's an independent memory system for what you might describe as imagination for the memory that uses images, and then there's another system that articulates knowledge, that's the semantic memory system, and it's not obvious at all
Starting point is 00:08:57 that the contents of all of those are equivalent, and that's why, for example, you can dream things that you don't know. Because one of the things you might think is that your dreams watch you act. And they watch other people act. And then they make a little drama out of that. And that drama has information in it. But you don't necessarily know what that information is. In that you can't describe it consciously.
Starting point is 00:09:19 Right? It's a kin to the Piagetian idea that kids can play a game. And you can take them away from the game. And then they won't know how to describe theian idea that kids can play a game, and you can take them away from the game, and then they won't know how to describe the rules, even though they can play the game. And so dreams can contain information that's full of the encoding of behavior that has information in it that you're not consciously aware of.
Starting point is 00:09:38 And so then you can become consciously aware of that in kind of a revelation, say. Maybe that's what you do when you become aware of the meaning of a dream or the meaning of a revelation, say, maybe that's what you do when you become aware of the meaning of a dream or the meaning of a fantasy or something like that. And that's all our ability to think that way in some ways can be traced back to Freud. Now, Freud concentrated mostly, I would say, at least in terms of pathology, on sexual and aggressive impulses.
Starting point is 00:10:05 And I don't think that there's any mystery for modern people about why aggressive impulses might be particularly difficult to integrate into the personality and might remain underdeveloped or we'll say repressed, although those aren't the same thing. And I think in order to, you might think that in different times in society, some things are allowed to surface and express themselves, and other things are less allowed.
Starting point is 00:10:30 And so Victorian times had a number of characteristics that made the repression of sexuality particularly likely, and perhaps also the repression of aggression, and we're talking about Victorian times in Europe, obviously, in only one time and one place. As Henry Elmerge says, this is a great book, by the way, the discovery of the unconscious. If you're really interested in psychoanalytic ideas, Freud, Jung, and Adler, and also the history of those ideas, there's no better book than the discovery of the unconscious.
Starting point is 00:11:00 It's an absolutely remarkable book, a great work of scholarship. I think it goes for about 250 pages before it even gets to Freud. And so it places Freud's discoveries in their historical context. So that's a really good thing to know. Alan Berger says it was a world shaped by man, for man, in which women occupied the second place. Political rights for women did not exist. This separation and dissimilarity of the sexes was sharper than today. Women who wore slacks, their hair short or smelt were hardly to be found, and the university's
Starting point is 00:11:29 admitted no female students. Man's authority over his children and his wife was unquestioned. Education was authoritarian. The despotic father was a common figure and was particularly conspicuous only when he became extremely cruel. Laws were more repressive, delinquent youths sternly punished, and corporal punishment was considered indispensable. Now, so the times themselves, I would say, were harsher and more repressive, but then there was an element to sexuality that was also extraordinarily problematic.
Starting point is 00:12:01 I mean, the first thing you might notice might consider, and people generally don't. It's almost impossible to overstate how revolutionary the birth control pill actually is. You know, people like to think that the political rights that women have attained have been a consequence of a political struggle, but I don't buy that for a second. I don't think that's true, even in the least.
Starting point is 00:12:23 I think that what happened was that we underwent a biological revolution in the 1950s, late 1950s, with the emergence of the birth control pill. And that, for the first time in human history, gave women pretty reliable control over their reproductive function. And that really transformed them into entirely different biological beings in many, many ways. Like here's an example, a subtle example. So, you know, if you track women through their ovulation cycle and you show them a picture of a man, the same man, and you do nothing but very his jaw width, when they're ovulating the guy with the wider jaws
Starting point is 00:12:58 more attractive and when they're not ovulating the farthest away from that, the guy with the thinner jaws more attractive, and that's associated with testosterone levels. And so women who are fertile like more masculine men, and basically if you're on the pill, then you're never in that ovulation phase. And so one thing that may have happened, and I don't know this for sure, but it's interesting to consider, is that since women have been taking the birth control pill, their preference for less masculine men has become more pronounced.
Starting point is 00:13:24 And that could easily be one of the things that's fueling at least some of the tension that's existed and exists now politically between men and women. But the point is, is that you just cannot ignore the massive consequences of a biological revolution like that and to make any other factor causal when you're trying to understand the political movement, movements especially in the last, say, 40 years, it's you're putting the cart before the horse. Now, it's reasonable to point out that the pill wouldn't have been accepted as a technology
Starting point is 00:13:55 if certain political changes with regards to the emancipation of women hadn't already been in place, right? No one would have even been allowed to do something like investigate contraception. So you can't separate the biological from the political entirely, but it's still very useful to organize your thinking, to realize just how profound
Starting point is 00:14:16 a revolution that was. But now back in the Victorian times, see there's another thing about sexuality. Modern people like to think that there's nothing dangerous about sex, and that is like the stupidest thing you could possibly ever hypothesize, because everything about it is dangerous. It's dangerous emotionally. It's dangerous socially.
Starting point is 00:14:35 It's dangerous because of the possibility of unwanted pregnancy, and it's dangerous because of the possibility of sickness, and that's a major one. I mean, so when AIDS emerged in the 1980s, that could have easily killed all of us. Now, the fact that it didn't was wonderful, but it did kill hundreds of millions of people. So it was no joke. It was a big deal, and AIDS mutated to take advantage
Starting point is 00:14:57 of promiscuity, and so the relationship between sexual behavior and the transmission of disease is actually mediated at the biological level. But anyways, back in the 1890s, they had the same problem, right? They had the problem with syphilis. And syphilis is one nasty disease. It can mimic almost any other disease, and it's devastating to your nervous system, and you can pass it on to your children.
Starting point is 00:15:19 And so, part of the reason that sexuality was heavily repressed in the Victorian period was not only because of the possibility of unwanted pregnancy, the relative poverty of people. Back in 1895, in Europe, the average person lived on less than a dollar a day in modern terms. It's almost impossible to understand how poor people were. And so sex in a poverty-stricken place is also a lot more dangerous than it is in a rich place because especially if you were given the lack of employment opportunities for women back in the Victorian period, if you happen to get pregnant out of wedlock, you were in serious trouble. And so the fact that sexuality was repressed is hardly a surprise because it was so difficult to integrate into
Starting point is 00:16:05 the full-fledged personality, you know, as it still is. So sexual oppression, supposedly characteristic feature of the Victorian period, was often merely the expression of two facts, the lack of diffusion of contraceptives and the fear of venereal disease. It was all the more dangerous because of the great spread of prostitution, and because prostitutes were almost invariably contaminated and therefore potential sources of infection. We can hardly imagine today how monstrous sylophus syphilis appeared to people of that time. Well, we can imagine a little bit better than they could in 1970 because it hasn't, you
Starting point is 00:16:37 know, AIDS is still with us, although it's nowhere near the plague that it was, say, 25 years ago. plague that it was, say, 25 years ago. Well, here's the Freudian world. Freud, so let's take a look at the history of the idea, the unconscious, to begin with. And one of the things that you might want to consider conceptually is that there are many different forms of unconscious. There's not just one. And so, Alan Berger points out that by 1900,
Starting point is 00:17:03 four functions of the unconscious had been described. There's a conservative function. So, the unconscious stores memories, often inaccessible to voluntary recall. Well, that's a strange one. You know, obviously, you remember your past, but you don't remember all of what you can remember at any given time, and you don't really have access
Starting point is 00:17:22 to that full store of memories, although you can try to remember. So the unconscious is the, you could imagine the memories are represented somehow neurologically, but the neurological structure isn't exactly the mind, like the neurological structure isn't exactly your consciousness. There's some relationship between them that we don't know. And the unconscious, from a conceptual perspective, is the place that your memories are that you sometimes can get access to and sometimes can't.
Starting point is 00:17:50 And so you might think, well, that there are, the memories that you can't get access to, there might be a variety of reasons you can't get access to. And one might be that you've just forgotten them. And one might be that they're so painful that you don't wanna bring them to mind. You'll engage in tricks to stop yourself from getting access to them. And one might be that they're so painful that you don't want to bring them to mind. You'll engage in tricks to stop yourself from getting access to them. Or maybe there are memories that are so complex and painful that even if you did get access to them, you wouldn't
Starting point is 00:18:16 exactly know what to do with them. And so there's not a lot of reason for you to bring them to mind because all it is is pain without any utility. And when you understand that a little bit, you understand more about what Freud meant by repression. The thing about Freud is that he kind of believed that, like many people believe now, that when you remember an event in the past, it's almost as if you're using a videotape recorder. And that when you experience that, the memory
Starting point is 00:18:43 is somehow recorded in you like it happened. But that's not a very accurate version of how memory works. I mean, we know that memories can be easily distorted. For example, if you interview someone about an event and you make suggestions that there was something present in the event that wasn't there and then you bring them back a couple of weeks later and you ask them about the same event, they'll often incorporate the thing that they were told into the event. And so, and the idea that you can make
Starting point is 00:19:12 an objective record of something that's happening to you is kind of a strange notion anyways, because so for example, if you're having an argument with someone, and later you're asked what the argument was about, and the other person is asked what the argument is about and the other person is asked what the argument is about. There's no necessary reason why the accounts will jive at all because a lot of time when you're having an argument with someone you're arguing about what the argument is about, right? Say, well, you're angry at me. Well, why? This is why I think you're angry at me. You say,
Starting point is 00:19:40 no, this is why I think this event has occurred. And you're thinking about, especially if we know each other well, you're thinking about the contextualization of that event across our entire history. And I'm doing the same thing. And I'm going to highlight things that you're not going to highlight. And I'm going to draw causal inferences that you're not going to draw. And for us, just to get on the same page about the memory is going to be very difficult.
Starting point is 00:20:03 So the idea that, especially with complex interactions with people, that you can somehow make a video recording of the memory and actually capture what happens is very, very... it's not true. You can't. I mean, you might be able to extract out certain objective facts, but generally, if it's a dialogical issue, if it's a relationship issue, it spans such a long period of time that just cutting a slice of it out doesn't constitute a reasonable record of what it means. And that's what you're more concerned with, too. Like, when you have an experience, you're not so much concerned about what happened from
Starting point is 00:20:39 an objective perspective. You're more concerned about what the experience means, and then you might ask, well, what does it mean to mean something? And that was the question I was trying to answer in that paper I had you read right at the beginning of the class. But one of the things that meaning means is that it has implication for the way you look at the world or the way you act in the world. And so if I tell you something meaningful, what that's going to mean is in the future, you're going to act slightly differently, or maybe radically differently, depending on how meaningful it is, but also that the way that you look at the world has shifted, and the way
Starting point is 00:21:14 that you look at the world is actually an unconscious process. I mean, you don't know while you're looking at the world, how it is or why it is that you're looking at the world in that way. Because, well, first of all, it would just be too complicated. And second, you wouldn't be able to concentrate on what was actually going on. So your attention, for example, is mediated by unconscious forces. And you know that. You know that perfectly well. And this is another Freudian observation. You know, if you're sitting down to study, for example, your conscious intent is to study,
Starting point is 00:21:50 but you know perfectly well that all sorts of distraction fantasies are going to enter the theater of your imagination, non-stop and annoyingly. And there isn't really a lot you can do about that except maybe wait it out. You know, so you'll be sitting there reading and your attention will flicker away. You'll think about, I don't know, maybe you want to watch Jane the Virgin on Netflix or something like that or maybe it's time to have a peanut butter sandwich or you should get the dust bunnies out from underneath the bed or it's time to go outside and have a cigarette or maybe it's time for a cup of coffee or it's like all these subsystems in you that would
Starting point is 00:22:23 like something, aren't very happy just to sit there while you read this thing that you're actually bored by. And so they pop up and try to take control of your perceptions and your actions non-stop. Maybe you think, well, this is a stupid course. Anyways, why do I have to read this damn paper? And what am I doing in university? And what's the point of life? It's like, you can really, well, you can really get going if you're trying to avoid doing your homework. And then you might think, well, what is it in you that's trying to avoid? Because
Starting point is 00:22:51 after all, you took the damn course and you told yourself to sit down. Why don't you listen? Well, because you're a mess. That's basically why. You haven't got control over yourself at all. And no more than I have control over this laptop. Okay, so there's the memory function of the unconscious, and there's the disillutive function. That's an interesting one. The unconscious contains habits, once voluntary, now automatized and dissociated elements of the personality, which
Starting point is 00:23:25 may lead to parasitic existence. That's an interesting one. I would relate that more to procedural memory. So what you've done is practice certain habits, whatever they might be, let's call them bad habits, and you like those things to get under control, but you can't. So maybe when you're speaking, for example, you use like and you know and you say, I'm a lot. And you've practiced that, so you're speaking, for example, you use like and you know and you say, I'm a lot. And you've practiced that.
Starting point is 00:23:46 So you're really good at it. And you'd like to stop it, but you don't get to because you've built that little machine right into your being, right? It's neurologically wired and it's not under conscious control. And anything you practice becomes that. It becomes part of you. And that's another element of the unconscious, a different part. And then there's a creative part, which is that, well, you know, you're sitting around and
Starting point is 00:24:09 maybe you're trying to write something or maybe you want to produce a piece of art or a piece of music, or maybe you're just laying in bed dreaming. And you have all these weird ideas, and especially in dreams. It's like, where do those things come from? And even more strange, and dreams. It's like, where do those things come from? And even more strange. One of the things that's really weird about dreams and almost impossibly weird is that you're an observer in the dream. It's like a dream is something that happens to you. Well, you're dreaming it theoretically. So how is it that you can be an observer? It's almost like you're watching a video game or a movie, but you're producing it, at least in principle, although the psychoanalysts would say,
Starting point is 00:24:47 well, no, not exactly, your ego isn't producing it, your unconscious is producing it, it's a different thing. It's a different thing, and of course Jung would say, well, it's deeper than that, the collective unconscious might be producing it. It's in some sense, it isn't you exactly, or it isn't the you that you think of when you think of you. And that's the ego from the Freudian perspective, the you that you think of when you think of you. And that's the ego from the Freudian perspective,
Starting point is 00:25:05 the you that you identify with, that's the ego. And outside of that is unconscious. The id, that's more the place of impulses. And you could think about those as the biological subsystems that can derail your thinking, right? And that govern things like hunger and sex and aggression. And your basic instincts is another way of putting it. And it's a reasonable way of thinking about it
Starting point is 00:25:25 because these are subsystems that you share with animals. You share them certainly with mammals. You share most of them with reptiles. You share a lot of them with amphibians and even going all the way down to crustaceans. There's commonality, for example, in the dominance hierarchy circuits. And so these are very, very old things.
Starting point is 00:25:42 And the idea that you're in control of them is, well, you're not exactly in control of them. And I would say the less integrated you are, the less you're in control of them, and the more they're in control of you. And that can get really out of hand. You can be, like with people who have obsessive-compulsive disorder, for example, which seems to be,
Starting point is 00:26:07 I would say, the disillutive elements in some sense of the unconscious, the way that it's portrayed here, poor people with obsessive compulsive disorder, they can spend half their time doing things that they can't really control, and they have very strong impulses to do them, and it's very hard on them to block them. They'll almost panic if those things are blocked, And then you have people with Tourette syndrome, you know, that they'll be doing all sorts of weird dances and spouting off obscenities and and and and and imitating people without being able to control it. And and sometimes a little bit of anti-psychotic medication can dampen that down. But it's as if there are these autonomous semi-spirits inside of them that grip control over their behavior and make them do things. And
Starting point is 00:26:53 you know, you find that to some degree in your own life because maybe you've become very attracted to someone, even maybe you don't want to be attracted to the person, and then you find yourself, you know, texting them when you know perfectly well that you should be going to bed, and you know, you're in a grip of something and and you can't control it and that's all part of the unconscious and all part of what Freud was studying. The dynamic unconscious it's alive and it's a composite. The mind is a composite of contradictory drives. Now, the way Freud thought about this basically was that with the id and the ego and the super ego. So if you think about the id as the place where these contradictory drives emerge, so it's sort of nature within, the ego is the thing that's sort of being pushed back and
Starting point is 00:27:33 forth by those contradictory drives. And the super ego is the thing that's on top saying, you better behave yourself, you better behave yourself. And so it's a different model than the Piazzetian model, because Piazzetia assumed that what would happen is that, as the child, and I like the Piagetian model better, I think in healthy development, the Piagetian model is correct,
Starting point is 00:27:55 but in unhealthy development, I think the Freudian model is correct, that instead of integrating, say, the aggressive and sexual drives for the sake of argument, into your personality as you develop, what happens is the super ego just represses them instead. So they don't become a dynamic part of you integrated into your ego. They're just repressed. You just don't manifest them. And so that's how you be a good person and you can be the victim of a very harsh super ego. And that often happens if you've had a particularly tyrannical
Starting point is 00:28:24 parent one or both or maybe a tyrannical grandparent, or maybe you're your own inner tyrant and you've picked up tyrannical voices through your whole life and aggregated them into this terrible judge that's always watching you, that's criticizing everything you do and restricting you badly and really badly in what you're allowed and not allowed to do. You see that with anorexic women. Well, men could be anorexic too, but it's much, much less rare. They have super egos that are just, or it's one way of thinking about it,
Starting point is 00:28:52 that just, they're just deadly. They're just criticizing every bit of them. Well, right to the point, they're really criticizing the bout of existence, right? It's, you have to be so perfect that the perfection is not aligned with the ability to live. You don't get to eat, you know? And people like that, they look at their bodies, they even look at their bodies incorrectly. Like anorexic seem to be unable to see their bodies as a whole. They can only see their bodies as parts.
Starting point is 00:29:19 And when you start seeing your body as parts, you're really in trouble because you can't get a sense of actually what it looks like and body perception is very, very complicated. But anyways, Piaget thought about the ego as in some sense as the game that's played by all these dynamic drives that shaped by the broader community. And so that could all be integrated, but Freud would say, well, look, when that doesn't happen, instead, you're subject to the tyranny of the super ego, and it just says, you should never be angry, right? You should never express yourself sexually, because if you do, there's something wrong with you. You're a bad person, and you're a bad person if you ever get aggressive. And so, and then people who are living like that under those circumstances, youances, they get, well, they're repressed. Is the right way to think about it. Now, Freud was interested in the idea
Starting point is 00:30:12 that mental disorders could be caused for two reasons. One would be purely bodily, like maybe a head injury or say in the case of schizophrenia, which is a good example of manic depressive disorder. We have reason to believe that there's something physiological going on, even though identifying that has been very difficult. And it's probably because there isn't one form
Starting point is 00:30:31 of schizophrenia. There's probably many pathways of brain injury that lead to schizophrenia-like symptoms. And there's likely not one form of manic depressive disorder, either if you think of the form as having a standard causal pathway. We know that there are, because we've done genetic studies on people who have manic depressive disorder in their family, and you can identify genes within a
Starting point is 00:30:53 family that seem to be contributing to the disorder, but the problem is is that those genes don't seem to be, so then you'll take another family group with manic depressive disorder and it'll be a different genetic combination that causes that. So, so part of the reason why it's difficult to associate the even the more biological mental disorders with biology all the way down is because they're so complex. And then there are other forms of mental disorder that don't seem to be structural at all. Structural at all. They seem to have more to do with, well, let's call it the psyche, right?
Starting point is 00:31:26 And that it's more like the contents of your thought have a problem rather than the structures underlying your thought. And of course, that distinction is difficult to make in a fine-grained way, but you kind of get the point. I mean, just because there's an error in your thinking doesn't really mean that the underlying biology in some sense has been compromised. It's complicated, because if the error is bad enough,
Starting point is 00:31:48 then it can compromise the underlying biology. But whatever, it's a conceptual distinction. And part of the conceptual distinction is helpful if you're trying to think, at least in part, about how you might cure it. Because if you're thinking about a brain disease, then that implies a different course of treatment, at least in principle, then it does if you're thinking about a psychological disorder,
Starting point is 00:32:12 where you might think about talking to someone, for example, and straightening out their thoughts or helping them learn to behave in a different way. And it was really Freud who started to think that he was the first person to really pause it. And this is pretty interesting, that directly pause it, that dialogue or conversation or speaking could be curative. And that's another thing that people don't like to give him credit for. I mean, there wouldn't be all these helping industries, social work and psychology and
Starting point is 00:32:44 biological psychiatry and so far is that also involves communication and counseling and all these things. Now that would have existed in all likelihood if Freud wouldn't have made the original hypothesis that there was something about communication that could be curative. Now Freud believed that experiences that hadn't been, now he thought about experiences as repressed. And this goes back to the videotape idea of memory. So the idea would be that you have a record of everything that's happened to you, and the records actually accurate.
Starting point is 00:33:20 And then some of those things that happened to you were very, very shocking to you, were very hurtful or very depressing or very threatening. And so you've decided that you're... Those have become repressed. You're not paying any attention to them. Now, he has a complex mechanism to account for that. And I actually think this is a place where his theory went badly wrong,
Starting point is 00:33:40 because you don't have a videotape memory. And it isn't obvious that the memories that you have of traumatic events are fully fledged and causally appropriate, but just not paid attention to. It's more like they're murky and unclear in and of themselves and they contain too much. And I don't think that people so much repress as they do refuse to attend to
Starting point is 00:34:07 or are unable to attend to. So it's more like a passive avoidance than a passive avoidance of something that needs to be explored and gone through rather than it is something you know that you don't want to look at that you are part of you has put away. And I think that's a major weakness in his theory and has led to a lot of problems with the idea of repression per se. But anyways, that was his idea that terrible things have happened to you. And you or some part of you doesn't want to know about them. And so they live this, those repressed experiences live an autonomous life of their own too. And here's an example of a trivial example of how that might work. Imagine that you're at work and your boss says something to you that
Starting point is 00:34:53 disturbs you. Maybe it makes you question whether your job is stable. And so you're kind of upset about that, but it's a casual offhand comment and you go back to work and you just sort of forget that that even happened. You know, maybe because you're attending to something else. But then you go home and you're just crabby as can possibly be. And you go home and one of the people there says something a little annoying and you snap at them. It's like, well, that's analogous to what Freud would call a complex, right? Is that this, because you could imagine what's happened is that the boss's words have brought up a whole little subpersonality predicated on doubt up to the surface.
Starting point is 00:35:29 And who knows how deep that would be? Well, what happens if I lose my job? And if I lose my job, well, what sort of person am I exactly? And what about all these other times that I've failed? And then maybe you remember the other times that you've failed? And what am I going to do in the future? And so it's this whole cluster of ideas that surrounds that doubt. And that's been activated.
Starting point is 00:35:46 It's just a little part of you. And then maybe you're not attending to that because you're busy doing some other work. But when you go home, something triggers it. And it's already there. It's all you get way more upset than you should. And that's what a complex is, except in a much more complicated manner.
Starting point is 00:36:01 Like a complex might be a whole series of experiences that you've had that are united by some emotion like threat that are having been transformed into a coherent representation but that can rise out of the unconscious and possess you. If you guys, many of you guys have been depressed at least one point in your life, you know, it's actually very common for University of Toronto students, especially in their first year. It's about one in three. If you have students, the Beck Depression inventory, about one in three University of Toronto
Starting point is 00:36:37 students in our research have had criteria for hospitalization. I mean, the Beck is a little over sensitive as far as I'm concerned. But you know what it's like when you're depressed? It's like it's a part of your personality sort of subsumes the whole and depression quite classically is, well, you can't think of anything good that happened to you in the past. And you can't think of any reason why the present is good for anything. And you're pretty damn hopeless about the future.
Starting point is 00:37:02 And so that's a complex as well. And it's a complex that consists of nothing but negative emotion, and it structures your memory and your perception and your plans for the future, all at the same time. Now, Freud had a very lengthy list of ways that people could be treacherous towards experiences they had that they wanted to repress. And so he called them defense mechanisms.
Starting point is 00:37:27 This is how you fool yourself into believing that you don't have to take into account a certain set of negative experiences. You know, it's like, well, we'll go through the repression. Okay, well, we talked about that denial. Well, that often denials are very complicated one. See if I can come up with a good example. What is a classic example for people who have, I think it's called anosignosia, I don't remember exactly, it's neglect. That's a less technical way of thinking about. So let's say you have a right parietal damage from a stroke,
Starting point is 00:38:05 and you'll lose the left side of your body. So you can't move it anymore, but worse, you don't know it's there, and you don't know the left side of anything is there anymore. And God only knows how that happens. But like you'll only eat half the food on your plate, only on the right-hand side. And if someone asks you to draw a clock,
Starting point is 00:38:20 you'll cram all the numbers into the one side. And so you kind of lose the idea of left. And I think it's sort of like, you know how, when you're looking forward, there's nothing behind you, you can't see anything back here. It's not black, it's not even gone. It's just simply not there at all.
Starting point is 00:38:36 And so if you could imagine that sort of stretching around halfway, that seems to be something what neglect is like. But anyways, if you take someone with neglect according to Ramashandran, if you irrigate their ear with cold water, the ear on the opposite side, then they'll kind of have a little convulsion, and then all of a sudden they become aware
Starting point is 00:38:58 of their missing left side. If you talk to them before you do the irrigation, you say, well, what's up with your left arm? And they'll say, well, my arthritis is bothering me, and I don't want to move it. They come up with something that sounds akin to denial. And then if you can snap them out of that with that irrigation, then they'll have a catastrophic emotional response, logically enough, to the loss of their entire left side.
Starting point is 00:39:22 And Ramashandran reports that lasting about 20 minutes, and then they'll snap out of it and go right back into the denial. And sometimes people deny things because they can't update what's happened to them is so overwhelming that they cannot construct a new model. They just rely on the old one. And you see this, well, imagine first that you've just had
Starting point is 00:39:43 a tooth pulled and you know how long your tongue tongue takes to remap the inside of your mouth. It's really hard to come up with a new concept of you if something catastrophic happens. And so sometimes the denial is just that something the thing that has happened is so overwhelming that the person can't model it, but then maybe also they refuse to think about it. And you see this emerging in lots of strange ways. So for example, if people develop diabetes, for example, they're often not very good at
Starting point is 00:40:11 taking their medication or regulating their diet. And you might say, well, they're denying the existence of their illness. And to some degree, they're probably doing that because who the hell wants to think that they're diabetic. But even worse than that, it's complicated to be diabetic. You're no longer the same person that you were. And so you have to learn a whole bunch of new ways to be this new person, what to eat, when to eat,
Starting point is 00:40:32 how to check your blood, you have to be careful whenever you go out and eat. There's 100 new things a day that you have to learn. And so separating denial from inability is a hard one, but you can also understand that people might deny. No, that's just not happening. That's, I'm not gonna admit to that. Reaction formation.
Starting point is 00:40:51 Oh, that's one. Maybe you hate your sister and maybe you have your reasons, but you shouldn't hate your sister. So what you do is act as if you really, really like her. That's an overcompensation. So that's another form of defense mechanism. Displacement. My boss yells at me, my husband yells at the baby, the baby bites the cat.
Starting point is 00:41:13 While they're not really dealing with the problem, which is the boss, it's just pushed on down the road. And identification, you're bullied. And instead of coming to terms with the fact that bullying occurs, you start bullying other people. Rationalization. Well, you know what that means already. Maybe you don't do your homework.
Starting point is 00:41:34 You're procrastinating. I bet you can come up with 15 rationalizations. No problem for why it's actually not necessary for you to do your homework right then. Intellectualization, while Woody Allen's movies are about like that, he's got all these neurotic problems, but he's smart, and so he can come up with intelligent reasons why he's so messed up, even though he knows he's messed up, and it doesn't help.
Starting point is 00:41:56 Sublimation, well, that was one of the things that Freud thought characterized art. So, for example, there's a lot of erotic content in art. And so if you're having trouble establishing a relationship or if you want to have a relationship with many people, then maybe what you do is sculpt nudes or paint them. And then there's projection, which is, I'm having an argument with you, and I'm unwilling to admit to my dark motivations, and I'm very skeptical of you.
Starting point is 00:42:24 And so I assume that you're characterized by all the dark motivations that I won't admit to my dark motivations, and I'm very skeptical of you. And so I assume that you're characterized by all the dark motivations that I won't admit to in myself. So Freud also believed that it was unconscious ideas that were at the core of psychological conflicts. And he described those conflicts as incomprehensible distress, psychosomatic symptoms. And so those would be the manifestation of psychological, of the manifestation of psychological content in bodily form.
Starting point is 00:42:53 That might be stress, as stress-related illness might be one way of thinking about that. I've had clients who had hysterical epilepsy, so that was quite interesting, so that was a somatic manifestation of a psychological problem. Back when Freud was practicing, hysteria was much more common, and maybe that was partly because Victorian society was so centered on the theater, and so dramatic, and people would come in with like a paralyzed arm or something like that, that he could sort out with hypnosis. And so they were manifesting their psychological distress in bodily form, often in a manner that was representative of that psychological conflict in some way. Behavioral anomalies, hallucinations, and delusions, he thought that all of those could be manifestations of internal psychological conflict with their sets of unconscious ideas.
Starting point is 00:43:48 So, you know, let's go back to the boss example. Your boss says something nasty to you, come home, someone says something a bit provoking and you fly off the handle and then you have an argument about what the hell is up with you because they say, well, look, what I said was, you know, this big and you reacted like this and you're going to say, well, no, no, you're always annoying like that. And which is kind of a denial thing. And maybe the person doesn't let up. And they say, no, no, I really know that something's wrong. And you do like six other things to keep them the hell away from you. And finally, they're persistent enough. So you break down crying. And you say, well, I had this terrible day at work. And you didn't even really notice that you knew that until the moment of moment of the tears. And you see that very frequently in psychotherapy too.
Starting point is 00:44:34 If you're talking to people, for example, maybe they're relating a story about their, their marriage that collapsed badly and they're talking and all of a sudden they'll say something and they'll tear up and then they'll continue continue and you can grab that. You say, look, you just said something. I noticed that your eyes filled with tears when you said that. What was going through your mind? Now often they'll, unless you catch it quick, they'll forget. So they're talking and they'll have, and the talking about the past is, you know, flashing off images to memories, and you'll say, well, that made you cry, and they often don't like that,
Starting point is 00:45:09 because for obvious reasons, that some things come up, that they don't wanna talk about. And so, you say, well, what was flashing through your mind and the person will tell you, like quite a lengthy, little memory fantasy about a sequence of events that is still a hot button issue. And that's another example of this underlying complex.
Starting point is 00:45:29 And if you watch people, you can watch people in normal conversation, this happens all the time, their eyes will move or they'll smile or you can see as they're speaking that all sorts of different ideas are flooding through their head. It's dreamlike in a sense, too. It's sort of as if the person is talking and they're dreaming at the same time. There's this image-laden set of memories that's going on at the same time. And that can be quite broad, far broader than they could encapsulate in the words. And so you can catch that. And if you're really listening to someone really paying attention to them, you can see when they're doubtful or when they pause for a long time. That's another one. You know that something's come up that's occupying their mind and interfering with the flow of conversation.
Starting point is 00:46:11 Freud was very good at listening in that manner. Well, that happens with jokes too, you know. And like, for example, when I was showing you guys the Lion King still is the other day, and I showed you that picture of Nella laying on her back with that peculiar expression on her face, everybody immediately laughed. And before I would have considered that an entry point into that and conscious, because there's a reason you were laughing about it, it goes along with it well, it would have gone along with the sexual complex in that situation.
Starting point is 00:46:42 And everybody recognizes it instantly, and they laugh about it, and comedians are really good at that, because if they're good comedians, they say what everyone's thinking, but no one will say. And it's relieved to everyone, you know? What's his name? Canadian comedian. He's always making racial jokes. No, no, it's Canadian.
Starting point is 00:47:04 Yeah, Russell, Russell Peters. I mean, he's a great example of that. He feels a whole stadium with people of all different ethnicities. And every single one of them is dying to be insulted because of their racial background. It's a relief to everyone. So he insults the herbs and then he insults the Jews and then he insults the Christians.
Starting point is 00:47:22 And he's going, oh, I'm so glad, finally someone said that. So he's speaking to part of their unconscious, and it's the part that's actually uncomfortable with all of that kind of discussion being repressed and staying below the surface. It's too weighty for people. So jokes express in playful language, which would culture will not formally express.
Starting point is 00:47:42 So you know too that when the culture starts going after the comedians, the things are not good. So you should leave the damn comedians alone, because there are the people that can tell the truth, and if you start to get annoyed at them, then that's not good. So Freud was also extraordinarily interested in dreams. Poor Freud, we're just not going to be able to cover him in enough detail. Well, how will we do this? Because I should tell you about the dreams. Freud wrote a book called The Interpretation of Dreams.
Starting point is 00:48:18 And he was the first person, I would say, who subjected dreams to a really comprehensive analysis, and he used them to investigate the place of complexes in his psychotherapeutic practice, so his clients would recount their dreams to him. Now, he believed that dreams always expressed an unconscious wish, and that was tied into his theory of repression, and so, for example, if you were very, very sexually repressed, which was very common at the time,
Starting point is 00:48:45 then you'd have dreams with sexual content that were expressing the undesirable fantasy, essentially, and by analyzing the dream, you could get down to what was being repressed. Now Freud believed that the dream more or less tied itself in knots trying to hide its content in some sense. Young believed instead that the dream was actually trying to be as clear as it could. It just wasn't part of the, let's call it the semantic memory system. It was more like a feeler out into the unknown. It was trying to represent things as clearly as it could. And so its use of symbols and that sort of thing wasn't so much to hide the actual unpleasant content
Starting point is 00:49:28 from the dreamer, but to express it in the only language that the dream could use. And so Freud, of course, also believed that some of that was true. All right, well, we're gonna have to stop there. I hope you enjoyed this episode. Next week is Dad's first actual interview podcast I hope you enjoyed this episode. Next week is Dad's first actual interview podcast in two years.
Starting point is 00:49:51 You heard that right. An actual interview podcast. The following is a message from one of our sponsors, Wondery, the new Jack Swing sound. You know the songs, Bobby Brown's my prerogative, Bellevue de Vaux Poison, Black Streets no digitty. It wasn't just hit songs, it became a movement that took over music, culture and style. Wondry and the Universal Music Group
Starting point is 00:50:17 present Jacked, Rise of the New Jack Sound, hosted by Terragi P. Henson. Jacked follows the story of a group of young musicians who used their unique style and undeniable talents to create a new kind of music, new Jack Swing. Go behind the scenes and into the recording studio with movement's most influential artists as they create music unlike anything that had been heard before
Starting point is 00:50:43 and forever changed American culture. You're about to hear a preview of Jack, Rise or the New Jack Sound, while you're listening, be sure to subscribe to Jack on Apple Podcasts. Join Wondery Plus in the Wondery app to listen to the entire series ad-free. Listen today at Wondery.fm-japped-underscoredJBP. Have you ever had a dream? Something you wanted so bad you just couldn't let it go? Or Timmy Gatling? His dream was music. And then we were hip hop and RB.
Starting point is 00:51:28 We were unique, because we dressed fly. We knew all the hip hop stuff. You like wildies, little kids, that are dangerous. And in 1989, the group he founded had made it. They're getting ready to go on stage in front of 41,000 people at the Los Angeles Coliseum in the crowd of fly girls wearing Donna Karen jumpsuits, guys wearing leather-tru bomber jackets, and obsession cologne is all in the air. This is the biggest black music
Starting point is 00:52:01 tour of the day. I was dead. It was in LA, it was the boy-wilded superfest. The Bud Wiser superfest. You know, I remember what a big deal the superfest was back in the day, honey. It was the kind of show we would get our hair done, our nails did, our everything did, did. In the summer of 89, rappers cool moldi and MC Hammer are on the bill. Patty LaBelle. And Timmy Gottling's group, Guy. Trent, flat top haircut, well dressed.
Starting point is 00:52:35 Timmy moves through the crowd of VIPs and people recognize him. Hey, what's up, what's up, man? But here's the thing. Timmy isn't playing with his group that day. And so people act as new. Why, you not on stage? What's up, you not performing?
Starting point is 00:52:50 Yo, Timmy, you not performing? Because what they don't know is that Timmy is no longer in the group. What? The fans didn't know. It was like a conundrum. People was like, OK, wait a minute. It was confusing.
Starting point is 00:53:06 And that was worse than anything. Instead, Timmy watches his former bandmates as they go on stage without him. And now, he's watching as the crowd at the LA Coliseum Gold War. Guys' dreams have come true, but not Timmy's. There's no other I can put it. It was totally bittersweet. You're hearing your song.
Starting point is 00:53:46 It takes you back to how y'all wrote the song, where you were at in the hood, in Teddy's living room, and now they are up on stage and you're not a part of it. Yeah. Yeah. Well, what happens to Timmy's dreams? I mean, how did this group of kids from Harlem rise to the top of the music industry only to come apart? The story of Guy and New Jack swing is a story of friendship, double crosses, gangsters, two timing, and the invention of a new sound that changed pop music forever.
Starting point is 00:54:20 And if you listen close, you can hear it all in the mix. Oh, bitch. You're a damn idiot. You're a damn idiot. Oh, bitch. From Wondery and Universal Music Room, I'm Tarrati Piense, and this Music Group. I'm Taraji P. Henson, and this is Jack. You can play, yes or something, but you can't claim.
Starting point is 00:54:48 All you gotta do is come on and sing. One, two, three. I'm a unique Jackson.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.