The Jordan B. Peterson Podcast - 168. A Brain Divided | Iain McGilchrist

Episode Date: May 13, 2021

Iain McGilchrist is a psychiatrist, author, thinker, and lecturer. He is maybe best known for his book The Master and His Emissary: The Divided Brain and the Making of the Western WorldDr. Iain McGilc...hrist and I discussed a variety of topics relating to the bifurcated brain, how we process reality as human beings, and the downfalls of the views that have shaped western culture according to McGilchrist. Find more Iain McGilchrist on his new website channelmcgilchrist.com, and check out his new book “The Master and his Emissary”, and look for Iain's new book The Matter of Things coming in the near future.This episode was recorded on February 15th, 2021.

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Welcome to the JBP Podcast season 4 episode 21 with Ian McGillcrest. I'm Michaela Peterson. This episode was recorded on February 15th 2021. If you're not familiar with him, Dr. Ian McGillcrest is a psychiatrist, author, intellectual, and lecturer. He might be best known for his book The Master and His Emissary, the divided brain and the making of the Western world. Dr. Ian McGell-Crist and my dad discussed a variety of topics related to the bifurcated brain, how we process reality as human beings, and the downfalls of the views that have shaped Western culture according to McGell-Crist.
Starting point is 00:00:37 This episode is brought to you by Helix Sleep. I love my Helix mattress. Helix Sleep has a quiz that matches your body type and sleep preferences to the perfect mattress for you. You can also get an option where they make half of the mattress a different way so that your significant other, who likes it, super soft or rock hard, can have a different type of mattress on their side. Helix has soft, medium and firm mattresses.
Starting point is 00:01:01 Mattress is great for cooling you down if you sleep hot. And even a Helix Plus mattress for plus size sleepers. I took the Helix quiz and I was matched with the Helix midnight. I have a bit of a hard time sleeping trying to call my brain down. And the fact I have a mattress I like makes a huge difference mixed with white noise, a cold room, an eye mask or two, and a memory foam pillow I can relax a bit. If you're looking for a mattress, you take the quiz, you order the mattress that you're matched to, and the mattress comes right to
Starting point is 00:01:29 your door ship for free. Just go to helixsleep.com slash Jordan, take their two-minute sleep quiz, and they'll match you to a customized mattress that will give you the best sleep of your life. They have a 10-year warranty, and you get to try it out for a 100-night risk-free. They'll even pick it up for you if you don't love it, but you will. This is a great deal. He likes his offering up to $200 off all mattress orders and two free pillows for our listeners at helixleap.com-jordan. That's helixleap.com-jordan for up to $200 off and two free pillows. This episode is also brought to you by Relief Band.
Starting point is 00:02:05 Relief Band gives me the peace of mind not worry about nausea. Did you know that one out of three Americans regularly suffer from nausea? We've all experienced that horrible feeling, whether it's in the backseat of a car staring at your phone or reading a book, after one too many on a night out with friends, or even just the anxiety of a work day. Nausea can ruin a day, force us to change our plans, and in the most severe cases, make us unable to function.
Starting point is 00:02:28 For those of you who can relate, I've got good news. Relief Band is the number one FDA-cleared anti-Naja wristband that has been clinically proven to quickly relieve and effectively prevent Naja and vomiting associated with motion sickness, anxiety, migraines, hangovers, morning sickness, chemotherapy, and more. Relief band is 100% drug-free, non-jousy,
Starting point is 00:02:51 and provides all-naturally relief with zero side effects for as long as needed. If you know someone who deals with nausea, relief band can make a great gift. Right now, relief band has an exclusive offer, just for Jordan B. Peterson listeners. If you go to reliefband.com and use code JBP, you'll receive 20% off plus free shipping and no questions asked, 30-day money back guarantee.
Starting point is 00:03:13 So head to Relief Band, R-E-L-I-E-F-B-A-N-D, dot com, and use our promo code JBP for 20% off plus free shipping. That's promo code JBP for 20% off plus free shipping. That's promo code JBP. Enjoy this episode. Hello. If you have found the ideas I discuss interesting and useful, perhaps you might consider purchasing my recently released book Beyond Order, 12 more rules for life
Starting point is 00:03:40 available from Penguin Random House in print or audio format. You could use the links we provide below, or buy through Amazon, or at your local bookstore. This new book, Beyond Order, provides what I hope is a productive and interesting walk through ideas that are both philosophically and sometimes spiritually meaningful, as well as being immediately implementable and practical. Beyond order can be read and understood on its own, but also builds on the concepts that I developed in my previous books, 12 rules for life, and before that, maps of meaning. Thanks for listening and enjoy the podcast. I'm pleased today to be talking to Dr. Ian McGill-Christ. I met him in 2018 in London and we had the good fortune to have a relatively brief conversation, which was taped and put on YouTube, and it was very
Starting point is 00:05:02 productive. And so now I get to talk to him again and hopefully for a longer period of time. Ian is a psychiatrist, Dr. McGill-Christ is a psychologist, a psychiatrist, a former fellow of All Souls College, Oxford, an associate fellow of Green Templeton College, Oxford. He's lectured all over the world. He's published a number of scientific papers.
Starting point is 00:05:24 He's most well known for his book, The Master and his Emissary. And I think you have a copy of that. I asked you to get that so that you can show people. The Master and his Emissary, the divided brain in the making of the Western world, which is an analysis of hemispheric specialization and its philosophical and scientific significance. And he's working on a new book which I have and have started to read a long ways into it at the moment called The Matter of Things,
Starting point is 00:05:55 which will be forthcoming at some point in the future and will shape some of our discussion today. He's published broadly scientifically and publicly a study of paintings on subjects with psychotic illnesses that's coming out, I believe. I'm planning that. And a series and also forthcoming a series of essays about culture and the brain. So welcome. Thank you very much for agreeing to talk to me. That was a huge pleasure, Jordan. Thank you very much. So one of the things I was rereading the introduction to your new book this morning, and I was struck by many different topics, but I was particularly interested in your conception
Starting point is 00:06:43 of attention. And so you talk about attention as something that in some sense brings things into being. I don't think that's a misreading of your of your writing. And maybe I could get you to expound on that a bit and to tell me what you think attention is because I've had a hell of a time differentiating it from, well, from fluid intelligence, for example, or from consciousness. It's a word that makes sense when you hear it in the context of a bunch of other words, but when you extract it out from that context and try to grip it, it falls apart in your grasp. So I think one could say that attention is the way in which the individual disposes his
Starting point is 00:07:28 or her attention. It's a disposition of one's consciousness. So attention is how you dispose your consciousness towards the world. And when I discovered, when I was researching the Master in his Am his imagery, the book that's now 10 years old, I came across this fascinating thing that one of the most fundamental differences between the hemispheres is their way of attending. And it didn't entirely hit me at the time, how important it is. But we can talk about that later, but you were asking the rather sort of interesting philosophical question about how attention helps to bring things into being. And I think it does, both generally and rather particularly, very
Starting point is 00:08:19 particular sense in the left hemisphere, generally what I mean is that how you attend to the world depends, you know, on that depends what world you find. The qualities of the world that comes to your attention is determined by the quality of the attention you bring to it. And that's a very significant statement. I was talking to someone the other day who's somewhat theologically minded and he was also very interested in the role that attention played on in constituting the world. I mean, you pay attention to things that you value one way or another. And what that means is that the world tends to manifest itself in
Starting point is 00:09:09 relationship to your value structure. And that that's very troublesome idea in some sense with regards to our conceptions of the objective world because it's not easy to it's not easy to parse out what's objective because it's not easy to, it's not easy to parse out what's objective when what manifests itself to you is dependent in large part on what you value. It's very complicated to sort that all out. Well, possibly very much later, which comes as a question of what objective and subjective mean and how one can, I think it's a mistaken dichotomy. I think one can interpret the words in important ways that give them meaning. But I think to think of just being an objective world out there and a subjective world in here is one of the problems with modern wisdom philosophy. But to come back to the creation of the world,
Starting point is 00:10:05 I was going to say that not only does it sort of bring into the world, the world that you know, which is after all by definition the only world you will ever know, but it also changes who you are. So the quality of the attention you pay changes you, the attender. So it's a very profound difference. And in the first book, the Master in His Amnesty, one of the things I was expounding was how this business of attention creates a whole distinct world. So the hemispheres have evolved
Starting point is 00:10:43 to two different sets of values. You mentioned values, and it's very germane. They have different reasons for existing, and therefore have different things they respond to. And what I have tried to explain in that book is that this gives rise to a whole way of seeing the world and a whole world, which is not just for the individual, but also at times it becomes the way of looking at the world for a whole culture, because it's been as individuals never entirely distinct from our culture. We've partly created by our culture and make it what it is.
Starting point is 00:11:26 So it's a very fundamental thing. Well, you take pains in the master and his emissary to promote the idea or to call attention to the idea that something extremely mysterious is going on in relationship to hemisphere specialization. So it's a very ancient phenomenon. Many creatures or most creatures with developed nervous systems have a bifurcated brain. And the hemisphere is differ substantially in terms of their neuroanatomical structure. And the question arises, why is it necessary, assuming that that differentiation
Starting point is 00:12:07 of structure reflects some profound differentiation of function? Why is it necessary to look at the world, so to speak, in two ways? And why is it so necessary that that bifurcation is conserved across evolutionary history? You'd think that one way would be sufficient, but it doesn't seem to be. And so the first question is, why do you have to look at the world in two ways? And the next question might be, well, what are those two ways? One of the things you outline that's particularly fascinating to me is that the right hemisphere seems to be specialized,
Starting point is 00:12:43 more for what you don't know. Whereas the left hemisphere is specialized more for what you do know. And I've sort of defined knowing pragmatically. You know something. If you undertake an action and the outcome is what you specified. And you don't know something. If you undertake an action and the outcome is other than what you specified and you don't know something if you undertake an action and the outcome is other than what you specified. And that sort of reflects that novelty realization, um, division that that was Goldberg, I believe, that that originated that that the neuropsychologist.
Starting point is 00:13:18 Well, Jordan, you've raised a whole bunch of points there. So I need a little bit of time to explain that. First of all, every neural network that we know of is asymmetrical. Going down to the very most basic network that we know, and the most ancient one that we know. NEMATO STELOVEC TENSIS, a CNM-E that is 700 million years old, is already asymmetrical in its neural network, and that's the earliest neural network we know of. And it's true of insects, it's true of worms, it's true of, you know, all the way up to human beings. And the three questions that really got me
Starting point is 00:13:59 going on this was, if the brain is there for making connections and its power is largely lies in the question the connections it can make, why is it divided down the middle, warpingly divided, and most people don't realize quite how big this differentiation is, and if they haven't actually seen a brain. The second thing is why is it asymmetrical? Because if you just need to grow this brain, you'd grow it symmetrically, the skull that contains it is symmetrical. And the third thing is why is the connection, the principal connection between the two hemispheres, the base of the hemispheres, the corpus callosum? Why is it at least as much, if not more, in the service of inhibition than
Starting point is 00:14:44 facilitation? So it's as though there's something really important about keeping two things apart. Now my hypothesis, and it's just that, is that this results from something that all creatures need to do. All creatures without exception need to eat and not be eaten. They need to live and to manipulate their environment to get food, to catch something, to pick something up quickly, definitely to pick up a twig to build a nest. In other words, for all the kind of day-to-day stuff, food shelter, they need to be able to manipulate the world very precisely. But at the same time, they must pay a precisely opposite kind of attention, which is sustained, vigilant, open
Starting point is 00:15:32 without pre-supposition as to what it may find. And so on the whole, the way in which this has been addressed by evolution is that there are two neuronal masses that can direct attention at the world, and the left hemisphere tends to specialize in targeted precise attention and the right hemisphere in a much broader, vigilant kind of attention, which actually sustains the being of the world. Nothing about these tiny fragments that are isolated, disconnected, meaningless gives you any idea of their meaning. It's only when you see the broad picture and you understand that they're not actually things that go to be put together to make that broad picture. But are things that are isolated
Starting point is 00:16:15 out of an already connected picture? So that's the basis of that. I just wanted to pick up your thing about, because I don't think it's quite right to say that the left hemisphere is about what you know and the rights about what you don't know. Somebody, I can't remember who, some philosopher said that knowledge is what we're uncertain of. The things we're certain of are things that we don't really know properly.
Starting point is 00:16:44 I think there's a good deal of truth in that. The left hemisphere tends to jump to conclusions. It's much more quick and dirty than the right hemisphere. The right hemisphere is the one that says, hang on, wait a moment, you may be getting this wrong because it wants to get things quickly. Its job is to manipulate, its job is to get, its to catch, its to grab stuff, it's the one that controls the right hand that does all the grabbing. So the left hemisphere tends to prize certainty and it's very uncomfortable when there's ambivalence. Where is the right
Starting point is 00:17:18 hemisphere? You can't act. Exactly. Whereas the right hemisphere seems to appreciate the possibility that we have to hold multiple views, multiple possibilities together. And so it has a quite different take on reality. It's more interested in discovery and exploration rather than capture. The left hand is more interested in capturing a thing that it thinks it knows, often, you know, not in any deep way. It's just identified an object it needs. But to understand things, the right hemisphere is better. And the idea of the master and the emissary is, I won't go through the myth of it,
Starting point is 00:18:00 I've explained it so many times. But the basic idea is that the master, the right hemisphere, knows that it needs an chemistry to do the sort of functional administration work. So it's aware that the stuff that it mustn't get involved with and that it can't know. Whereas the left hemisphere knows everything as it were in its eyes because it only knows a tiny bit, which is explicit. And, you know, there it is in broad daylight, down in black and white, no shades of meaning, no nuances, nothing implicit about it. So it thinks it knows it. And if you like the downfall of the left hemisphere,
Starting point is 00:18:39 and therefore, I would go of the society, the civilization, as it once was, that we belong to is that the left hemisphere doesn't know what it is it doesn't know. It's, you know, the famous thing, the Dunning Kruger effect, the more you know, the more you think you don't know, and the people who know least think they know everything. It's a little, it's not quite fair, but there's something of that about it. Yeah. But can I pick up something that I type some earlier? We're talking about creating the world through attention. And I think that is, that is true. And there's a very big question there about what I mean by that. Do I mean just as you say subjectively or objectively? And maybe we should park that for the moment, but I don't mean either in a very simple way. But what is fascinating is that the right hemisphere, as I
Starting point is 00:19:33 say, knows that there are things that it's not aware of, but the left hemisphere seems to take the attitude that if it's not attending to it, it doesn't exist. This is very dramatic in clinical neurology. So people who have had a right hemisphere stroke, they not only as it were don't now pay attention to something, but they deny that that thing ever existed. There's wonderful you know, very rich accounts of patients. One of the ones that I really like is an experiment done by who was it? Bissière Canloucetti, I think, back in the late 1970s, where they got a couple of highly educated people with right hemisphere strokes. And they were in Milan at the time, and they said, you know, these people lived in Milan and they said, imagine you're standing in the Piazza del D'Ovo more in front of the cathedral and looking at
Starting point is 00:20:30 the square, describe all the buildings in the square and they would describe only the ones on the right side that the left hemisphere only pays attention to, whereas you know, but I need to say for the viewers that the right hemisphere pays attention to both halves of the world. So when you have a left hemisphere stroke, the world is relatively preserved globally. But after a right hemisphere stroke, you're only relying on this left hemisphere, it's interested in the bit of space which it can manipulate, the bit on the right. So they named the buildings down the right hand side of the view of the square they had then. And then the experiments have asked them to go to the other end of the square and look at the cathedral facade and name the buildings. And this time they named all the ones down the other side of the square, but didn't mention any of the ones they just mentioned.
Starting point is 00:21:19 So there's something it's been commented by one philosopher that it's almost like there's an ontological landslide, things come in and out of existence for the left hemisphere. And when it was pointed out to these people, what they'd missed, they became angry, irritable and frustrated, which is a typical left hemisphere emotional tone, impatient to dismiss this. So I think that's intrigued. I mean, it's just one image. I mean, the very dramatic one is to do with denying that you have parts of the body.
Starting point is 00:21:54 And this was pointed out by Chris Langeller, I think way back in the second decade of the 20th century. They don't only deny after a right hemisphere stroke that they have the left half of the body, which is not functioning because of the right hemisphere stroke. They will become very irritable if questions are asked about it or they will just go blank. I mean, they will be talking perfectly coherently and they're asked about that and they will go stumb or they will become very irritated. And if you force them to recognize that they have a body on that side, although they're perfectly intelligent people, they know they
Starting point is 00:22:30 must always have had a left half of their body, they will deny it. And he says, it's as though they never had a left half of the body. It's not only that it's not there now, it never was for them and never will be. Now that's what I mean. When I say that there are different levels of creativity of the world or the creation of the world through attention I should say. I'm in the two hemispheres. Now you're in your new book and to some degree in the master and his philosophical speculation and you're trying to solve a problem So maybe you could tell us what the problem is that and and and then we can discuss the solution Well, I suppose the problem is the one that I mentioned that there is a puzzle about why Brains are set up in this rather odd way. But you're also pointing
Starting point is 00:23:28 to a kind of philosophical malaise, right? So there's a conceptual problem, but also an emotional or broad-scale philosophical problem. And there is, there is indeed. And indeed, if I may say so, the book that I hope will come out fairly soon called The Matter With Things, which is a pun on several levels, because I think it's a critique not only of the way we think now, but of our obsession with thinking of the world as composed of things, and the only matter exists. But anyway, in that book, what I'm really trying to do is marry science and philosophy again. They never should have been separated. Science cannot properly be done by philosophy,
Starting point is 00:24:10 without philosophy. Many scientists and philosophers have commented on this over the years, and the divorce has been disastrous for them both. You get a mindless kind of science that jumps to very naive conclusions that everything is mechanical. And you get a kind of philosophy that thinks it's above dirtying its hands with science. Now I think each of these
Starting point is 00:24:32 parties can benefit from a reprachmal, which is long overdue. And it's that that I try to do in this very big book, to show how strands of neurology, philosophy, and physics, and even of world mythologies come together, to show the same very similar pictures, the same Gestaltan, the same differences between a world such as the world brought into being by the left-handers here, and a world such as is brought into being by the right hemisphere. And perhaps before going on any further, you did invite me earlier to say something about what those differences are.
Starting point is 00:25:10 Well, to try and sum it up very quickly, what I'd say is for the left hemisphere, things are known familiar. Literally things that are unfamiliar are better dealt with by the right hemisphere until the left hemisphere can go, oh, I see it's one of those and put it in the Catholic repulsed. They're more distinct, in fact, they're probably entirely fragmentary or isolated in the left hemisphere, whereas there, the right hemisphere seems that nothing is ever ultimately, um, unconnected from everything else. Things in the left hemisphere are frozen, they don't move, they don't change. Things in the right hemisphere are constantly flowing and changing, although flowing and
Starting point is 00:25:56 changing and remaining the same are not necessarily an opposition, as we know, Heraclitis, my favourite philosopher, said by changing it remains the same, which is the image of a river, which is never ever still for a moment, but the river outside my house that was there at breakfast time is probably there now. So, in that sense, it's remained. And we're all, I believe, like these rivers, all living beings, in fact, probably everything that exists as Heraclides has pointed out. So what are the things? The left hemisphere abstracts.
Starting point is 00:26:33 It tends to abstract from the body. It tends to abstract from the context. And something I learnt very early on in life was the importance of context. How it utterly changes. Anything that somebody says or any image, and this is particularly true of course of literature, which I studied and taught for a certain way when I was a young man, that once you start paraphrasing a poem and taking its sentiments out of the context, they've utterly changed themselves and no longer what they were.
Starting point is 00:27:06 The left hemisphere is more interested in categories, the right hemisphere in the unique case. So it sees that you're not just a member of a certain group, you tick certain boxes, you fall into that category, but that you're massively, complicatedly different. And I mean, this has a very real basis, you know, when you look at neurological patients, people who have right hemisphere strokes are examples to I know of that both come from the same research group in Switzerland, but involve a farmer who used to know all his cows by name and after after the right hemisphere, right parrattle stroke, he couldn't really, well, he not only couldn't tell his cows
Starting point is 00:27:50 from another, but he could hardly tell a cow from a horse. And another woman who very plaintively commented after the right hemisphere stroke, she'd spent her whole life studying the birds of Switzerland. She was an authority on them. And she said, all the birds look the same. So that's what happens in the left hemisphere world. Quality is replaced by quantity, uniqueness
Starting point is 00:28:13 is replaced by the category. And then again, the left hemisphere tends to see things as inanimate, where the right hemisphere will see them as animate. Well, a category implies in some sense that the members of that category are indistinguishable, because otherwise you don't have a category, you just have particularity.
Starting point is 00:28:41 And so you could imagine that, I mean, to understand this completely, you have have particularity. And so you could imagine that that mean to understand this completely, you have to understand to some degree what categories are for, but or what yeah, what categories are for at least in part, you put things that you can act towards the same way in the same category. And so young children might think of cats and dogs as dogs, all of them as dogs because they're cuddly, petable entities, not because they have four legs or because they have fur, but because you interact with them the same way. Then you can differentiate cats and dogs as you get a little older, but the first category, dog, which is petable things, is a perfectly reasonable category.
Starting point is 00:29:26 And you can imagine that once a categorical structure has been imposed, that it's easy just to see the category, I wrote an essay in my new book, which is called Beyond Order, about the function of artists. And I believe that part of what artists do, and I think this is maybe you can tell me, but I think it might reflect the differences that you're talking about.
Starting point is 00:29:50 When I was a kid, I lived in a small town, and I can remember all the houses on my block. I can remember the detail. They're familiar to me as individual entities, but now that I'm an adult, I live, I've lived on this street for like 20 years But the houses are indistinguishable to me. I can't see them as different entities And I think it's because I'm so familiar with the category house, which is a practical category that I can't see beyond the category And it's very efficient because I know what houses do. They sit there. You don't have to pay attention to them. And so it's really a useful perceptual shorthand
Starting point is 00:30:31 just to see the category. And what an artist will do is take you outside the category and make you see that the particulars again that you're missing, and I guess that's partly the context to remind you of what's beyond what your memory forces you now to see. And is that akin to this distinction that you're making between the left and the right? Well, it is a distinction. It's a related distinction, but not exactly the same distinction. I mean, interestingly, it relates to the difference, a very important
Starting point is 00:31:06 difference. In fact, I think probably the single most important difference of all, which doesn't at first strike people, partly because they're so used to living in a world of representations. That is the distinction between the presence of something as it comes into being for you. And your mental representation of it, which is like a caricature or a category thing or a verbal sign. So that the left hemisphere's addiction, if you'd like to understanding things via language is very important because after all, language makes everything the same. You know, as Nietzsche said, it makes the uncommon common because, you know, when you say, somebody's got brown hair or something, then you've got everything just like in a category, but when you see that person, there's something quite different about the way their hair is
Starting point is 00:32:01 and so forth. So you find that out if you painted it. You know, because you wouldn't paint it. You'd paint it with brown paint. You'd paint it with a multitude of colors if you were really looking at it. You said something you would. And you'd see that it wasn't, and I mean, artists play with,
Starting point is 00:32:15 was it Manet who painted the haystacks in, or Monet, I don't remember in multiple different times of the year? And the haystack, I mean, the shape was the same, but the haystack was completely different. And that's really, sometimes what he was portraying was the category of haystack, but the reality is, is extraordinarily complex. And, but the category, the category seems to have functional significance. So you dump things together that you can act towards the same way. And then you weren't as labeled on top of that to serve as an even further
Starting point is 00:32:54 compressed shorthand for that category. You have a complex world, it's multitudinous, and too complex to even see. And then there's a perceptual act that categorizes that into like a perceptual image, house, say, and then there's a further compression and eradication of information that enables that to be represented by a word. And now, well, the great philologist Max Mula
Starting point is 00:33:23 said, it's interesting that we read in psychopedias, we have dictionaries, we read books, and we are with all these words. But none of the things that these words represent actually exists because in the act of being represented, they are no longer the thing that was present, the very fact of representing it suddenly stops the presence of the thing. For me, this is very vivid in words with and this comes back to some of the things you were saying about the liveliness of the childhood
Starting point is 00:33:53 mind, that when he was a young man and 50 years of boy rambling in the late district, the world was very much still alive to him and coming into being for him, but that as a man, he went back there and as it were, couldn't avoid seeing the landscape as oh, a picturesque mountain, a picturesque lake, one of those, if you'd like. And this is what he means by the phrase,
Starting point is 00:34:22 the shades of the prison house, growing around the growing boy, in his intimations of immortality on reference. Yes, so, so this is from this book. There was a time when Medo Groven streamed the earth and every common sight to me did seem apparel in celestial light, the glory and the freshness of a dream. Yes, that's it. This is what I'm referring to. And this really is one of the more important
Starting point is 00:34:55 differences. I would say probably the core difference that as it were, one world is real, vibrant, unknown in part, only known and ever to a degree, and ever more coming into being, and ever more coming into knowledge for the right hemisphere. And for the left hemisphere, already cold, finished, known, dead, put in a book, stuffed on the shelf, filed away. And we now live so much in this virtual,
Starting point is 00:35:24 represented world, partly because we're very much cut off from nature, which constantly reminds you of its vivid, uncertain liveliness. It confronts you with its audacious beauty and vitality all the time, partly because we've learnt to cut our minds off from our bodies. And so we think in this enormously abstract way. And partly because of city life, partly because of technology, which means we interact with two dimensional screens rather than the three dimensional depths, which is in a room when you're with Samo, which is why, as you know, and I know, because we both help patients in our time,
Starting point is 00:36:04 that it's very important to be in the room with the patient. It's a completely different thing that happens from anything you can do on the telephone or even like this. So part of the philosophical case that you're making is, I believe, that we're, we have a terrible conundrum as human beings, we, we, we need in some sense for the purposes of efficiency to move towards the most efficient representations possible. And this has real bearing on the nature of perception itself. So I know, for example, that even in the primary visual cortex, so in principle, you know this, but I'm going to explain it for people who are listening. As the signals, as the pattern signals from your retina move back towards your brain, they move upwards, so to speak, through a hierarchy
Starting point is 00:36:58 of processing units. And even at the first stages of that processing, there's still more top-down input from other brain centers than there is input from the retinal structures themselves. And so what that implies is that even at the beginnings of perception, you know, if you think about it as being built up from perceptual elements up towards the whole Gestalt, which isn't exactly accurate, but it'll do for now, even at the beginnings of the visual process, there's more input from what's inside than there is from the external world, so to speak, or at least as much.
Starting point is 00:37:32 And then what seems to happen as we age is that perhaps, is that increasingly that perception becomes solipsistic, and so we're only seeing what we know, and that's externally efficient, because it's very hard to build up a new perception. You have to really investigate something in detail to see it a new. Whereas if it's the same old thing that you've seen 10,000 times, you can already use what you know. And it's not surprising you'd be annoyed if you were forced to jump out of that because there's a tremendous amount of work that has
Starting point is 00:38:02 to be done. If you want to see something for the first time again. Well, as we both know, people would rather deny obvious truths than let go of a cherished belief. So, that's certainly right. And we don't perceive the world as knowing a naive way, we come to the world with our history, with our vast range of experience. And so, as you say, this top down effects on the lower end of things, of the lower end of perception, from higher cognitive functions.
Starting point is 00:38:40 But that's an image of something very important to the philosophy I try to put across in the matter with things, which is that in order to understand any element in experience, it's at least as important to see what holes it goes to make up, or potentially can go to make up, as it is to see what it turns into when you break it down. In fact, often it's not very revealing to find out what happens when you break it down. In a way, every... The cause of the sufferers from that is psychology is a discipline that consists, at least in its present form, mostly of disparate experiments which demonstrate very particular things about people, about how people behave in very
Starting point is 00:39:25 particular circumstances. But there's nothing, there's very little that unites that back up to something that isn't merely fragmentary observation. And so it's very difficult to get a grip on. And there's billions of potential separate observations. And they're not that helpful in some sense. Well, it's all part of the world picture which from the philosophical point of view is the purpose of the long book which I sent to in manuscript the matter with things. Effectively, I can state quite simply that I want to give a considered response to the philosophy of our age, which is that there is only matter and that things are understood by reducing them to their parts and this doesn't change them. It's a very naive philosophy, it's simplistic and it's immoral because it changes the way
Starting point is 00:40:28 we treat the world and other people and nature. It changes our idea of who we are in a very damaging way, ruling out things that other traditions have traditionally held us very powerful. And you know coming back to your comment about how people cling on to things that they believe and it's much more difficult to try and see something in a different way, especially with age. This is why, of course, most traditions of spiritual growth
Starting point is 00:41:01 in join on the person paradoxes to see things in a completely new way that violate all the ways that they thought they knew. So I use paradox in this new book, but not in some blind way. In fact, I show that what we mean by a paradox is that the view of the left hemisphere of something, and the view taken of the right hemisphere of the same thing can never actually completely marry up. They have different qualities and if you push the comparison or the desire to make them logically come together too far, you end up with a paradox. And this started happening early on in the Greek, the ancient Greek period of philosophy with Zeno, this is where the first paradox has come from, and I have a whole chapter on paradox, which I see as generated by the desire of the left hemisphere to say it must be this
Starting point is 00:41:58 or it must be that, clinging on preferentially to the very fragmentary view of the left hemisphere. See, the left hemisphere is not good at understanding. That sounds a very blanket statement, and it is. But the whole of the first part of that book, the new book, is massively more thorough, neuropsychology than is in the Master of Neusemistry. So it's about as thorough as I could possibly make it. And what I do is I look at the ways in which we have any chance of getting an idea of what the world is, what are the portals of entry of, if you like, information about the world to us. And I take it that they, they, it depends very much on our attention, how we dispose our attention, perception, the judgments we form on the basis of perception,
Starting point is 00:42:49 the ways in which we apprehend what we're dealing with, rather than comprehend it, in other words, grasp it, as we say, with the right hand of the left hemisphere, take it, use it, how we understand it in terms of emotional intelligence, which is not a small thing. It's the whole way in which we understand everything human. By emotional, I don't mean sort of in some, you know what I mean, I'm talking about social and emotional understanding, the sort of thing that is absent in people with autism and cognitive intelligence. This may surprise people, but all these things and creativity. So creativity, intelligence of the cognitive kind, IQ kind, emotional and social intelligence, apprehension is a separate case, I come to that in a second, perception
Starting point is 00:43:47 and attention and judgment. All these are better performed by the right hemisphere. Only apprehension is better performed by the left hemisphere. So the only thing the left hemisphere is better at is getting a hold on either an idea, very precise, clear one, or on a thing that it wants to use. But all the manifold complexity, which our intelligence brings to bear in order to understand the world, all of that is better done by the right hemisphere. And I can say that on the basis, not just of experiments in normal subjects, but on seeing what happens when you have either left hemisphere damage or right hemisphere damage to summarize a vast chunk of information, which I hope will be there for people to read very, very soon. To summarize that very briefly, what one would say is
Starting point is 00:44:37 that when you have damage to the right hemisphere, your grasp of reality is the main thing that's impaired. You don't understand it, you don't connect with it. Your ability to understand what's going on disappears. Whereas when you have a left hemisphere stroke, the main things are you have difficulty speaking and using your right hand, they're practically very important, but essentially the understanding of the world, the grasp of the meaning of the world, as you see to its grasp again, but
Starting point is 00:45:11 the overall comprehension of the world is sustained by the right hemisphere. Let me ask you a question. Sure, go ahead. All right, okay. Yep. All right, okay, let me just make this point. Because I want to, people might say, well, okay, but so what, we've both got right and left hemispheres. So we're not missing anything.
Starting point is 00:45:32 So does it matter? Well, yes, it matters very importantly for my philosophical project. Because as I show in the second part of the book, where I look at the proper contributions to understanding made by reason, science, intuition, and imagination. What I can show is that in those attempts to grasp, we can see the world, we can see the signature of the right hemisphere, or the signature of the left hemisphere on a particular model. So if we have two possible models of a certain action or an aspect of reality or space or time,
Starting point is 00:46:11 which I deal with in the third part of the book, and indeed in philosophical history and in the history of physics and so on, there have tended to be opposing views of the world. Once you know how the left hemisphere sees it and how the right hemisphere tends to see things, you can see the hallmark of the left hemisphere's understanding on a certain philosophical standpoint, on a certain scientific take of the world, and you can see the hallmark and the imprint of the right hemisphere in certain other ways of reasoning and of science and philosophy. So this is very important because up till now we've never been able to judge between these two.
Starting point is 00:46:52 We've got A, we've got B, we just have to go, hmm, can't tell, we can't reconcile them, we can't do without either of them. That's true. Ultimately, that's true, but we can get a very sharp idea. I believe now of which of these is fallacious. Which one is going to lead us down a blind alley? Which one is out of touch with reality? And which one is more in touch with reality? I just wanted to say that because it's behind the whole philosophical drift of my book,
Starting point is 00:47:20 which is how do we know who we are? I ask Flotinus's question, who are we? That's effectively the question. What is the cosmos? What is nature and how do we all relate? Sorry, I had to over to you. No, no, that's good. Okay, so to do something, you have to zero in on it. So let me lay something out for you and then I'm going to ask you a specific question about it. So imagine that I'm concentrating on the computer screen, I'm attending to it, I'm writing a book. Okay, so the question might be, well what am I doing while I'm writing that book? What is it to write that book? Well, at the most focused level of my consciousness, the most focal level, that involves voluntary
Starting point is 00:48:07 control of my fingers. I'm going to be typing single letters and there are muscle movements that are associated with that, but I don't really know what the muscle movements are. I know how to move my fingers. That's the highest level of resolution I can manage. I can press T with my left hand and H with my right hand and with each with these two fingers. And so that's sort of where the pedal hits the metal in some sense. That's where my intent meets the world. Okay, so, but I'm not typing letters. Sorry, I am typing letters, but at the same time I'm typing words. And at the same time I'm typing words I'm typing phrases. And then I'm typing sentences and I'm typing paragraphs and I'm typing chapters and then I'm typing books. And then the book itself is an artifact that's nested inside higher order structures. So while I'm writing
Starting point is 00:49:01 the book, but the reason I can write the book is because I'm imagining a world within which the book is nested. And so I'm focusing on something very specific, like I can't write the book without pushing the letter T with my left index finger. But I also can't write the book without apprehending the book as a whole. And you know, when I edit, I edit not letters because I can spell, but I edit words, I substitute one word for another. I edit phrases, I edit sentences, I edit at the paragraph level, like all of these levels actually exist. Now, is it reasonable to suppose? So, sorry, I'm going to add one more level outside that. So you might ask, well, why am I writing a book? And it might be, well, because I'm a practicing scientist. And why is that important? It's, well, I'm a, I'm a, a dutiful citizen, let's say,
Starting point is 00:49:55 trying to uphold my moral responsibility. And you might say, well, why is that important? I would say, well, that's part of my, my proper moral engagement with the world. And then I can't go farther outside than that. Now, is it reasonable to suppose, if you think about that whole structure as a kind of lens that focuses us in than the world,
Starting point is 00:50:15 is it reasonable to suppose that it's the left hemisphere, so to speak, that's concentrating on the Ts in the Hs, and that as you move up that hierarchy to broader and broader levels of conceptualization, that the manner in which those higher levels are conceptualized shifts more and more to the right, or is processed more and more by the right, is that a reasonable way of looking at it? I think you could, but I'd need to sort of gloss it a bit.
Starting point is 00:50:42 What you've beautifully described is what the right hemisphere ninos, and which John summarized by saying, if you try to get hold of any one thing and pull it, you find that it bring with it the hold of the rest of the universe. Yet that sentence you're writing is informed by your personal history and the history, therefore, of your culture, and therefore, and so on and so on and so forth. So everything that has gone to make you is present in that business of the book. And so you're not, of course, at any one time, aware of more than a tiny bit, but there's a very fallacious and superficial argument that if you're not conscious of it in that sense of the word conscious then somehow you're not doing it, but of course you are the whole thing emanates from what I call the field of you. Now, suppose rather than you know we can take the example of the typing but you wouldn't be able to type at all if you were thinking about what your fingers would do. And, you know, but nonetheless, it would be stupid to say that you're unconscious while you're typing. Of course, you're not unconscious. Somebody playing a Bach few has
Starting point is 00:51:57 got to use all their fingers and their hands, you know, and their feet at the same time. And if they concentrated, they could only concentrate and focus on one finger. Of course, that would stop the whole music for a start. But they're conscious. Of course, they're just as conscious when the whole thing's happening as they are if they think about the finger and stop it. So what this illustrates is what Alfred North Whitehead
Starting point is 00:52:22 was keen to point out that as soon as we master something, we relegate it to another part of the mind that we don't any longer have to focus on and that the focusing of tension is costly. He said it's like cavalry charges in battle, it should only be done rarely, you need fresh horses and it comes at a high cost. So that's a good analogy because you build a little machine that the things that we do unconsciously are in no way inferior stuff.
Starting point is 00:52:58 So for example, the barf fugue is not inferior. When a surgeon is learning, he or she has to be very conscious of what he or she's doing, the actual business of the hand cutting. But when the surgeon is very skilled, it can hum, listen to the radio chat with colleagues and it's all happening. Similarly, a chess player, a bad chess player has to be conscious of every move, but a really good one. It's not unconscious, it's very, very highly conscious, but it's not focused. Now, my distinction to, sorry, finally answer your question is that is that focused attention, this focal attention on the detail is what the left hand is fear does. It can only take in about three degrees
Starting point is 00:53:43 of the attention, so it's incredibly limited. And as not, as Whitehead says, it can only take in about three degrees for the attention to art. So it's incredibly limited. And as Whitehead says, it has its uses in an emergency, but really it's not a satisfactory way for living. And what I think is happening is that we are now more and more saying anything that I'm not actually focusing on right now doesn't exist, all the implicit stuff, all the unconscious stuff, all the things that go to make up the richness of our both cognitive and emotional and embodied cells, isn't really important.
Starting point is 00:54:15 We've focused down on this tiny bit that the relatively unintelligent left hemisphere knows about and is aware of. So when I say conscious and unconscious, I like to say, don't think of these as two separate realms, like, you know, two tanks with perhaps a trap and things can pop up from the lower tank into the tank or whatever. But instead, think of it like a stage, and there's a spotlight, and the spotlight may just illuminate one part of the stage, but the rest of the stage hasn't gone anywhere.
Starting point is 00:54:46 It's still there. And you just need to move the spotlight. And suddenly it's there again in the middle of what you're thinking about. That's how I would see that question. Okay, so let's go back to this typing example again. Just, so when you learn to type, you're going to be paying conscious attention
Starting point is 00:55:04 to pushing the teas in the H's. As you learn to type, you start perhaps being conscious more of maybe you're attending at the level of the phrase. Like you don't have to be consciously attentive to those things you've built automated machine before. Now, they say when kids learn to read, it isn't enjoyable to begin with because it's effortful to learn to process the letters and it's effortful to learn to process the words
Starting point is 00:55:32 and it's not until they can automatize the letter and word processing and so they can read the word at a glance that they start to be able to be conscious of the phrase and the sentence and that's when they get the meaning from the text, and that's when it starts to become enjoyable, right? It's not just effortful. And then so the consciousness of a reader isn't consciousness of letters, and it's not consciousness of words. It's consciousness of something like the interplay between sentences and paragraphs. And it's like your consciousness floats above the highest level of automatization that you've been able to manage. Is that, is that seem reasonable to you? Well, I mean, when you're playing a musical piece, for example, you don't pay
Starting point is 00:56:14 attention to what you've practiced, because you've got that. You pay attention to the sequencing of what you've practiced. And the greater a musician, you are the higher up in the abstraction hierarchy, you can focus because you've, you've automated all the lower stuff. Well, and that comes back to, to what, I'm sorry. No, I was, I was, I'm trying to get the relationship between that and the hemisphere specialization. Well, I think I've done my best to point out that the, the that the right hemisphere is the one that is able to attend to the whole Gestalt.
Starting point is 00:56:50 Ultimately, it's dealing not in fragmentary entities that have to be put together, but in Gestalt and that already exist and unnested, so that you go down from one level and you find another. You know, famously you can go from the body to the organ to the tissue, to the cell, to the organelle, to the, you know, and each of these at each stage is a whole that has its own qualities and its own rules really and works in a semi-autonomous way. So there's always freedom between the levels of understanding. There's always space.
Starting point is 00:57:27 It's rather like the gaps in the structure or where the light gets in. You know, if you tighten everything up, then you've got total darkness. So what we're trying to do all the time is to know enough to be able to act, but to leave it open so that we can know more and really understand where we are and what we're doing when we're acting. So I think these are significant differences between the left hemisphere which is utterly goal directed and very direct in the goal in a wider whole. You know, the reason of typing these letters is not just to make the keys go up and down and to have a bit of paper at the end of it, but because you want to influence minds that are now unaware of this, but we'll know about it soon. I think it's the difference between this very, again, whitehead says as a civilization advances
Starting point is 00:58:25 by the number of actions that can be made or to massaging below the level. Sorry. Without thinking. Without thinking. Because thinking is very complex thing, isn't it? I mean, what is it? And a number of people have commented rather along the lines that I'm saying, that it's not so much right to say, I think as in Kogito, but in the words of Liechtenberg, the 18th century German philosopher and physicist, it's danked, it's danked in mir,
Starting point is 00:58:58 something is thinking in me. And that is the me, it's not separate and it's not unconscious in the sense of it's it has no life, it has no meaning, it has no purpose, it has no direction, absolutely not, it has all those things. And one of the things I'm trying to argue in the last part of the, may I say something just about the structure of this book, I started off on it, There's new book, and I'm just going to say a little bit more about it. Yeah. Yeah. So I've been trying to grasp something large going through it. It's not such a simple thing to do. It's not a simple thing to do. So the first part of the book I've explained, it gives one an insight into a simple fact that in terms of having access to the reality of the world, the right hemisphere is better than the left, and it has a special take, if you like, which we can recognize, so that
Starting point is 00:59:54 when we're having to choose between two opposites, we can choose one if we want to over the other. And then the second part of the book, I'm looking at the pathways to knowledge, using attention perception judgment intelligence, how do we put them to use? Well, I think the main ones are science, reason. I think most people would say they're important. I would say intuition and imagination are also vastly important. Now, none of these is infallible. None of these can say that it can deal with everything. There are proper limits to science,
Starting point is 01:00:25 otherwise you're peddling untenable naive scientism, but it is a very important thing for us to respect and to do honorably. Reason is enormously important. I use science and reason as the basis of my book, but again, reason as Pascal, famous mathematician and philosopher said, reason is poor if it cannot see its own limitations. And so it has limitations, but it can achieve a very great deal. And the same actually is true of intuition. It's had a very bad press in recent years because I think again, psychologists, I think you will, you alluded to this, they like
Starting point is 01:01:05 things that can be taken down into bits and shown that we can find the mechanisms. Intuition is a bit hard for that. And imagination has been, again, relegated to the sort of children's play box that this is something to do with fantasy, whereas in fact I argue that quite the opposite, whereas fantasy may be an interesting decoration on things that we already know and the left-handosphere can do that. Imagination is actually how we go to meet the world and understand it and we have to imagine it into being. There is no alternative. If we are not imaginatively engaged with the world, we just can't see a lot of things that are there. So we need to use all of these faculties together, not just one or two as we now do.
Starting point is 01:01:53 Sorry, Carol. Well, I wanted to comment on your discussion of imagination and the manner in which it brings the world into being. So we've already discussed the fact that the realm of your experience is dependent to some degree on your attention, and that that's associated with intent. And intent seems to me to be, it's future oriented,
Starting point is 01:02:17 to have an intent, intent means to attempt to move from one place to another. And hypothetically, it's a better place because why move otherwise? And so to act in the world with intent means that you're playing out something that's imaginative because to posit that one thing is better than another and therefore want to move towards it,
Starting point is 01:02:40 you have to have imagined up a better world. And so what that means in some senses that we're always meeting the world in a way that imposes our imaginative attempts to make it better upon us, upon the world. But that also brings the world into being. And so, and I guess I'm saying that because I'm trying to grapple with the why of your book again,
Starting point is 01:03:04 you're implying throughout, and more than implying, I guess I'm saying that because I'm trying to grapple with the why of your book again, you're implying throughout, and more than implying, that we have a positive view point that's demotivating and dangerous, and you're implying as well, that that has something to do with our obsessive concentration or utilization of left hemisphere functions. That reminds me of Heidegger, to some degree, and his claim that moderners use the world as produce, you know, that we tend to reduce everything to its functional utility in so far as it can be exploited. And like, I have some sympathy for that because we have to exploit the world to live.
Starting point is 01:03:40 But it, it, it, it it it it so let's say we do lose something by being specific. We and narrow, we gain something which is functional utility. What do we do about that? You're trying to understand it and why it is. What do we do to fight against it? Well, that's a whole separate question, but at the moment I'm trying to unpack what the problem is and I want to push. I mean, quite what we do about it is the million dollar question and we may not be in a position unless we radically alter the way in which we think about the world, understand it and feel it and experience it and interact with it. We may not have a world in which to live. So it's a pretty important topic. But so having sort of more or less as it were gone over what are
Starting point is 01:04:41 the portals to understanding what are the paths to understanding. I then in the part three, which is really, if you like, the reason we've had part one and two, you can't get to part three without them, but when you get there, we want to know, so what is the word like? And so I look at the structure of the world, the theories about it, and what we can tell from physics and from the hemisphere hypothesis what parts of it we may be perceiving with the left hemisphere and what we may be seeing with the right. And I look at the structure in the sense of the coming together of opposites and a very interesting philosophical question of the relationship between the one and the many. relationship between the one and the many. And then in the rest of the book,
Starting point is 01:05:24 I look at time, space, motion, meaning largely flow, but all kinds of motion, matter and consciousness, value, purpose, and the sense of the sacred. Now, I argue that these elements, like consciousness, are not secondary, they're not derivative. It's actually irrational, I suppose they are. Reason is on the side of the fact that they are ontological primaries. And I argue that there's a lot. Well, it would take us very long time. That. Well, I can unpack the phrase.
Starting point is 01:06:09 What I mean by ontological primary is that it can't be reduced to other terms. It can't be said that as long as you look at a brain in a certain way, you can work out what consciousness is. Consciousness is Sui generis, it is of its own kind, it is not something that is derived from anything, it has to be a primary constiction to the universe. This is not a particularly any longer controversial view, it's held by many philosophers now in the form of panpsychism, in which something like consciousness is in the cosmos,
Starting point is 01:06:48 and the cosmos perhaps exists inside consciousness, not my consciousness, but a consciousness field. And there are plenty of neuroscientists who say this too. Rhammet, Colin Blakemore, not known for being kind of away with the pharise, but they say this too, Rama Tholam Blakemore, not known for being kind of away with the fairies, but they say this too. But I would also argue and it's a perhaps harder thing to make comprehensible in a very short space, but then actually values are things that are there. They're not things we make up. They're not things that are like, hmm, I rather like that. They are built into the drive of everything. And I think that the cosmos has drives. You can
Starting point is 01:07:36 describe them in all sorts of ways. It has the fact that it changes and moves in certain ways according to quote laws, which may actually not be laws, but maybe temporary habits. We don't know. They may be evolving too. But the very fact that this thing has this energy to evolve, to differentiate, to produce differentiation within union. This is a value of a kind. You can't get behind these.
Starting point is 01:08:00 And most of our values, other than those of utility, this is good for me and I want to have more of it, which is what the left hemisphere is devoted to. Most of those other values are not reducible to share in a material greed or, you know, feathering your own nest. They're often actually the things that are vastly important, probably the whole point of the being conscious and so tall is to come to appreciate the meaning of truth, goodness and beauty, to have a sense of something awe-inspiring, which is really what we mean when we talk about the sacred, that we're humble enough to say we don't know everything, and we probably will never know. I mean why should we? That's also a totally irrational idea that our brains are so constructive that we should know know everything. I mean, our mouse might think that if it could think
Starting point is 01:08:48 that much, you would think it knows everything, but it doesn't, you know. So, and we're evolving, there may be creatures in the future who think what the hell did Homo sapiens in 2021, though, you know? So, I just want to get back into the frame that not all the things that matter to us, they are enormously important aspects of the of a university is not dead and static unless given a push, not without purpose, not without meaning, not without value. These things are in the grain, I'm certain and the job is for us to find this and most philosophers wise people, sages, what Ebylite had called them in the past have adopted a view of the cosmos which is exactly the one that one would expect the right hemisphere to hold which is one which
Starting point is 01:09:42 things are not always certain or known they they're changing, they're interconnected, but the whole thing has a meaning. It is not a heap of fragments that don't mean anything. The modern malaise. So that's really where I'm driving at, if you see. That's the philosophical goal, is to help people see something that I think they already intuit. I mean, that was a response to the the master in his emistry. And apart from people enormously movingly writing
Starting point is 01:10:13 to me saying things I never thought I would ever hear from writing a book like your book changed my life. And I'm sure you've had this too. But people saying, what you're saying, I kind of knew, I've known this, but I couldn't find any way of articulating it. Well, there's a reason why you couldn't find a way of articulating it. And that is articulation in language is controlled by the left hemisphere. It developed very good tools from mapping out the world
Starting point is 01:10:41 in a way that is very useful to its purposes. But the important things are hard to articulate in words. They're implicit meanings. All the deep things like love, religion, poetry, music, how do you say these in words? How do you say them in language? But they have extraordinary meaning and power. They're the things we live for, not for the things that we can say, put down in the notebook, you know what I mean. So what's driven you in this direction, do you think? I mean, you made a very large number of claims
Starting point is 01:11:15 in that last section of thought. Yeah. For example, you've come to the belief that value is somehow implicit in the structure of being, that's what I understood what you said, and correct me if I'm wrong. And that it's, that it's, that it's unfolding across time. And I mean, I've been thinking about this, that exact issue and awful lot. Do you, it's very difficult to formulate this question. So imagine that imagine that we're drawn towards an ideal
Starting point is 01:11:49 human beings are drawn towards an ideal and imagine that you can You can you can detect that draw By your own dissatisfaction in part is that you don't feel you're living properly or your conscience is bothering you You feel that there's something more to be attained. you're embarrassed at your insufficiencies, right? So there's this ideal that's pulling you onward and judging you at the same time. And that ideal might be, well, the ideal human being, that's one way of thinking about it. And that's partly why I got so interested in hero mythology. I mean, do you, is it a reasonable conclusion of your line of thinking that the notion of the ideal human being is somehow built into the structure of the cosmos?
Starting point is 01:12:32 I mean, because of the class, I don't know how to... No, it's not. No, I don't think that at all. I want to scotch that immediately. Okay, well, how do you scotch that if you start with your presuppositions? Like, well, because I don't, my presuppositions have nothing to do with an already conceived plan that is just being acted out. This is not why it wasn't necessarily.
Starting point is 01:12:58 All right, well, okay, but let, all right, okay, well, I'm glad you weren't. But a lot of people think that if I say these things, I must be positing an engineering god who sort of tinkers with things and makes things happen according to his purpose. I mean, that's fair. I mean, I was implying that in some sense. I mean, because the question would be, where does your insistence that values are part
Starting point is 01:13:20 of the structure of being, like where does that find its limit? Because the classic limit of that is something like, in fact, the definition of the utmost place of value in some sense is almost indistinguishable from the claim that there is a God. And so... A God is not the same as an engineering God, and I was taking enormous pains in the book. It cost me more than anything I've ever written to write the chapter called The Sense of the Sacred, in which I try to help people to a place where they can understand why people use this
Starting point is 01:13:57 extraordinarily difficult word God. You know, it's not a satisfactory term, but it's the term we have to have to name an aspect of our experience that if we don't name it disappears from our lives. And that's not to say that there isn't something there that is that merits whatever we mean when we say divine. I mean, we haven't defined, we haven't defined what we mean by divine. And we're back in the nets of language. We're trapped in the nets of language, as Shelling said. But what I'm suggesting is that, as Whitehead suggested, and come on, Whitehead was also the co-author with Russell of the Principia Mathematica. He wasn't a phantastist.
Starting point is 01:14:41 He had this, I think, incredibly deep idea that whatever one likes to call the divinity God, whatever, is the thing that the cosmos has relation with. Relation is at the core of being. I even argue that relation is prior to the Relater, prior to the things that are related. That sounds nonsense. How can you relate, how can you have a relation if there isn't anything yet to relate? But there's a wonderful image called in Indian mythology called Indra's Net, which covers the universe. And in it, the idea is that the filaments of the net exist before the net, before the crossing points, which are the things we see. And on those crossing points, there are little gems which reflect every other gem in the net, before the crossing points, which are the things we see. And on those crossing points,
Starting point is 01:15:25 there are little gems which reflect every other gem in the net. And that would take a very long time to unpack, but perhaps it can set things going, people's minds. But the idea I have- The gesture to the right hemisphere is that relation is prior to anything at all, really. And that therefore, whatever you mean by God and whatever we mean by the cosmos are in some sort of dynamic relation, which isn't evolving one, in which the outcome is excitingly not known.
Starting point is 01:15:57 If it were known, it would all be some horrible, possibly sadistic play by an almighty, all-nearing God. I mean, then look, I'm going to be talking to Rowan Williams shortly, but I don't want to go go into all that I mean by that. I don't think God is on Niscience and on Nipleton, but I don't think he's not either. Just in the same way, I don't think he's green and I don't think he's not green. I think the terms are wrong, but you know, we can go there if we want, and later or another day. But the thing, what I'm really saying is that these, that God is discovering, becoming, unfulfilling whatever God is through the relationship, which classically, in most religions, is described as love, which is after all just like a form of gravity
Starting point is 01:16:45 in the world of life and emotion rather than just in the world of the so-called inanimate. So, that therefore we are coming into being, God is coming into being, and we're necessary to one another's coming into being. It's not that God does a bit to us and then we do a bit back to God. It's like I've read a very good book, I keep mentioning it by a young microbiologist in America called Critishama, called Interdependence and she argues very importantly that it's not just that certainly it's not just that an animal or an organism molds its environment nor is it just good enough to recognize just that an animal or an organism molds its environment. Nor is it just good enough to recognize that while an animal affects and shapes its environment, the environment shapes the
Starting point is 01:17:34 animal or the organism, but that this is not a you know turn by turn process, it's not that the animal shapes the environment which should then in its turn shapes the animal. It's not that the animal shapes the environment should then in its turn shapes the the animal. It's a in entirely simultaneous process of coming into being of co-creation if you like now this idea of simultaneous coming into being is an ancient one, but I think it's a very deep one philosophically and a very important one So that account for your objection to the idea of the omniscient determining God. Absolutely, absolutely, because the God has, the God would have no creation, creation is not really just the unfolding of something that's already there. Creation. What's the name of the book?
Starting point is 01:18:18 What's the name of the book by the microbiologist? It's just called interdependence. It's by Chris Sharma. Can you spell her last name? SHARMA. It's quite a short read. Okay. I'm mentioning her quite a lot these days. Okay, so let me ask you a question. So to to now, I'm going to try to pack up what you're doing. And so again, tell me if I'm wrong. So we have these opposed viewpoints of the world, paradoxical viewpoints. They're expressed, they make the hemispheric differences necessary, or they're a consequence of the hemispheric differences.
Starting point is 01:19:00 If there wasn't a paradox, we wouldn't need the two hemispheres. We need these two different ways of looking at things. We've tilted, we're in danger of tilting too far to a left hemisphere view, and that's keeping us from what? It's keeping us from apprehension of the relationship with the sacred that you're describing, the co-creation relationship. Is that reasonable? the co-creation relationship? Is that reasonable? Well, it's ruling out so much. I mean, I can't begin to tell you, but you can imagine,
Starting point is 01:19:33 all the things that this very reduced abstract schematic bureaucratic. Essentially, it's bureaucratic. You push something, it has an action on something else., you know, we can predict the outcome, we can organize it. That's the left hemisphere's vision of the world in animate stuff that it can move about very much the industrial revolution was a kind of acting out in the outer world of the world picture of the left hemisphere in some ways. I talk about that more in the master's history. But it's phenomenally successful. It's ruling out everything, really. It's ruling out our ability to understand, to see, to see a tool. I mean, a number of very important people, one of them,
Starting point is 01:20:19 Gerta, said, you know, thinking is good. But seeing is so much better. And I think we just don't see things anymore because we don't expect them. We don't understand them. We've ruled them out from the word go because our world picture doesn't contain them. And if you stop doing that and start attending in a more flexible way, you find there's a massively complex and fascinating massively complex and fascinating, rich, nourishing response to your attention. It's the absence of that that causes the meaning crisis, which is constantly being bounded about. The Egyptians.
Starting point is 01:20:54 The Egyptians knew that, the ancient Egyptians knew that. Because they, the God, Horace is the eye and its attention. And it is Horus that revitalizes Osiris and he's the God of structure. And they saw the proper sovereignty was a combination of attention and structure, a dynamic combination of attention and structure. Well, this is very brilliant.
Starting point is 01:21:24 I quite agree, but this is where we come to, I need to make a correct, a possible misapprehension. There is absolutely nothing wrong with the view of the right hemisphere. It's in fact necessary. It's part of a, of a dialectic backwards and forwards between these two ways in which things can be built. You can't have the one in a way without the other. You're quite right. We need them both. But we live in an age which is completely obsessed with the idea of equality, a some eternal sort of truth about the cosmos.
Starting point is 01:21:59 I have, I can see no evidence for this idea. It's a lovely idea. A human-invented idea, which is a good one in society, although it can't be realized, and it may actually not be necessary or even good for it to be ultimately realized, it might lead to a horrible kind of totalitarianism as many 20th century philosophers pointed out. But the problem with the elephant is one virtue above all else. It's an interesting problem. Well, the ideological problem. No, no, the point is this. We need the left and the right, but we need the right to be in control. Now, this is very important.
Starting point is 01:22:39 This is the image of the master and his emissary. The emissary and the master are not equal. The master needs the emissary and knows he needs the emissary. The emissary being inferior doesn't know that he needs the master. So the emissary is good as long as he's under the control of the master. Now that image is extraordinarily important for understanding this picture of the cosmos. And it's actually present in ancient Chinese Navajo, not Navajo, Iroquois, mythologies and so on, this idea of the being an unequal pair, that one has to be the guardian of the other. And as long as the one that is, as it were, a potential problem, remains under the supervision of the wise on one that sees, or everything works well for everybody. And that's why,
Starting point is 01:23:32 in the Martian Museum of History, I suggest, there were three periods in the West, in early Greek civilization, in the peak of Roman civilization, and again, at the Renaissance in the West, where these were working well together. But in every case, it it slipped further to the viewpoint of the left hemisphere and in every case the civilization has crumbled and I see the evidence for that all around me now. So I'm not saying that we just need these two things, I'm saying we definitely need them both. Neither of them is bad, but what is bad is for the inferior one, the one that sees less to take control. And it's very easy for it to take control because like the less intelligent person
Starting point is 01:24:15 that thinks it knows everything, it thinks I've got it. I've understood it. There's no more. You know, we do us three more experiments and we've cracked the universe. You know, it's all just a matter of a few years of science. We'll understand that.
Starting point is 01:24:28 Let me let me let you in on some of the things I've been grappling with here. So I talked to Matt Ridley and Bjorn Lawberg recently. Their enthusiastic enlightenment rationalists, I would say, they look at what's happened as a consequence of the Industrial Revolution and the tremendous technological advances that that's produced and the immense increase in human well-being. And they say we can continue with that into the future, we can produce a world and perhaps are already producing a world where poverty is increasingly going to be a thing of the past.
Starting point is 01:25:08 And we can bring the rest of the world up to the standards of living that characterize the West. And we can continue to expand the pie. And I'm not interested in discussing whether that's possible or not, because it's possibly possible and possibly not. But it's a particular vision, right? It's a vision of the extension of material comfort to everyone.
Starting point is 01:25:30 And that comfort has been extended tremendously over the last two or three hundred years. It's absolutely amazing. And I would say in large part, it's a consequence of that left hemisphere reduction of the world to manageable bits and the manipulation of it. And this is not to say anything negative about your thesis whatsoever. But one of the things I've noticed is that that materialistic utopian vision, and I'm also not insulting Ridley or Lambert, who I admire greatly, that materialistic vision of incremental material progress has very little mode of power. Like, it isn't an exciting story for some reason.
Starting point is 01:26:08 And, you know, what do we want? Incremental improvement. When do we want it? In due time. It's not a gripping story. It's lacking something. And you, you, you seem to be pushing towards something that might be the medication for that lack. There's nothing in that vision that speaks of like a grand destiny for the individual for society.
Starting point is 01:26:36 And there are many religious traditions that insist that human beings exist in a relationship with the divine and that it's only in living out that relationship that life is imbued with the proper kind of meaning and proper means sufficient to keep you from malevolence, I suppose, sufficient to be in love with life. Now, you pause it in some of your statements a while back that when we're in this co-creation relationship with the divine, and that isn't too far off from my understanding of many classical religious propositions that human beings participate in the act of creation, whether we participate in the act of creating God
Starting point is 01:27:21 is a whole different question, but we can leave that aside. I mean, you're... So then the question is, what's the question that arises out of that? I'm still trying to drive down at exactly what the main point you're making. You've worked on this book for a tremendous amount of time. Something's driving you hard, and it's the revelation of a solution to a very important problem, and the revelation seems to be something like an attempt to explicate a higher order vision for, to explicate a higher order vision, something that we can aspire to. or division, something that we can aspire to. And the drive to this is that I don't think
Starting point is 01:28:08 that we live in an enlightened era. We live in what David Boehme called an endocrine era in which what we think is enlightening us is in fact inducing misery. The strange thing is that unarguably when people are enormously poverty-stricken and of course when needs then to define what poverty is are people who lead the lives that their ancestors have lived as hunter-gatherers, are they poor or do they only become poor when their whole world is ripped apart and they're
Starting point is 01:28:45 brought into the nearest large industrial slum and have no bearings on the life, no relationship of the world, no pride and their health suffers and they kill themselves. So what is poverty? That is a very important question first, but also remedying poverty, extreme poverty is of course enormously important. No feeling person could argue against that. But it's not enough. You know, as one rather well-known, one rather well-known historical figure said, and man cannot live by bread alone.
Starting point is 01:29:16 And the thing is that there's only bread in this story. But that leaves out everything. Peter Cook. He plays the part of a publisher who says, the trouble with your book is that it lacks everything. And I feel that these, this kind of philosophy lacks absolutely everything. It's got nothing whatever to offer. Now, let me just put that some facts to this because it may sound, you know, it's just my opinion. What do we know? Well, fortunately, going back to the 30s, school children in America in a certain system have been asked the same questions about their happiness in life at the same age, going back now 70 years.
Starting point is 01:30:00 And Jean Twenge, who has looked at this data, which avoids all sorts of problems of defining what you mean by happy and, you know, retrospective scopes and all the rest, you've got the data from the 1930s to the 40s to the 50s. And nowadays, the numbers of children that would qualify by a very well authenticated and commonly used standard as being depressed is five to eight times what it was in the 1930s when poverty was a big issue. So five or eight times, not five or 8 percent more. So there's something horrendous we're doing to ourselves. Suicide rates are going up, particularly among women who've registered much greater dissatisfaction with life now than they did 20 years ago, interestingly.
Starting point is 01:30:57 Because one might think that a number of things that would have made life hard for them have been removed. But it just shows how complicated it is knowing what works well for people. And three things overall, three things seem to be incredibly important for human fulfillment and happiness. And one of them I touched on at the end of the Master in his Emistry, which is feeling socially connected, being bound into a meaningful community of trust. That's one. The second is being in the presence of nature, just going off for half an hour into a forest and being quiet has an effect on your blood pressure, on your physical health and so on. And these effects, if practiced, are greater than those
Starting point is 01:31:41 and going to the gym. And the thing that really struck me is the Oxford, there is the Oxford handbook of religion, I think it's called, but it looks at enormous bodies of evidence about the well-being of people who are adherents to a religion and those who are not. And not only, as you very well might expect, the people who are not adher And not only, as you very well might expect, are the people who are not adherents to religion, much more prone to anxiety, depression, drug addiction, they cope less well with
Starting point is 01:32:14 crisis, they're more vulnerable generally. But they're actually physically not so well. So for example, rates of stroke of heart disease are comparably better amongst the believing groups and the unbelieving group with the difference between those who do cardio exercise, you know, for several hours a day. So an even smoking, it's a more powerful effect than smoking. Social connectedness is more impactful than smoking. Being in nature I think is more powerful than smoking and being a part of a religious community that worships and even holding spiritual or religious views to some extent is a mitigator against unhappiness and illness. So, is that any answer to your question of why I don't think that Matt Ridley's idea of,
Starting point is 01:33:12 it's totally left-hand is for an idea, we just crank out some more government departments to do some marvelous technical things and everybody gets to be living in a fantasy land of happiness. I don't believe this. Well, I don't believe this. Well, I don't think he believes that. I mean, he's a complicated person and he's more concerned with applying material resources where they could be most effectively applied. I think the question of what material comfort and plentitude needs to be embedded in is a different question. I think it's reasonable to say, well, we should do what we can to alleviate destitution and catastrophic poverty. We should
Starting point is 01:33:54 improve child nutrition. We should eradicate tuberculosis. Obviously, right. The question is, what does that have to be nested inside for it to be worthwhile? And that's, so that's what you're, that's what you're aiming at. And I believe that is what is that need, what does that need to be nested into make it worthwhile? And you've come to this conclusion. This is shocked to you over the years that you came to the conclusion that, okay, why not? I mean, the reason I'm asking is because it isn't an everyday occurrence in some sense
Starting point is 01:34:28 for a committed scientist to point out that the scientific viewpoint needs to be embedded inside a broader, what value oriented viewpoint is something I believe, but it isn't an everyday occurrence for that to be stated forthrightly. And as a scientist, it isn't necessarily the first thing that would come to mind. Have you always thought this way or have you come to this as a consequence of your thinking?
Starting point is 01:34:53 Well, I have the advantage of having come to science from a fairly thorough grounding in the humanities. So I had a philosophy of life that was based on reading, thinking, talking, and I was a relatively aged customer when I started to study medicine compared with most people who do that, at least in this country. that, at least in this country. So I brought a background which has always been my passion, an interest in philosophy, but not just a kind of forever analytic philosophy in which you it's more or less like sort of angels dancing on a pinhead and breaking everything down for the tiniest parts. This doesn't really particularly interest me, but it kind of a more human philosophy, which doesn't become theology, but which is open to the idea that there is more in the world
Starting point is 01:35:56 than we can know or understand. It does sound like it is, that it has become theology. This isn't a critique. Like, it's an attempt to observe what you're telling me. It's not a critique at all. I'm trying to understand this. And it depends what you mean by theology. You see, I don't think the most important part of the relationship with God is necessarily theology. I'm not disrespectful of theology.
Starting point is 01:36:24 But... Well, but I've read a lot of the Kabbalah. I've acquainted myself with Buddhist philosophy and I've always been very interested in Taoism, Hinduism, and the deep wisdom of these things, including as I say, North American, Native people, and even Circumpolar people. I mean, the wisdom is embodied in their mythologies. We think of these as somehow childish myths, but in fact, these myths contain, and in fact are the only way of containing or not containing, because they're not ever contained, but of transmitting, bringing into being for other
Starting point is 01:37:13 people, the things that are the deep truths. And it's that that motivates you. I mean, I'm not going to be alive much longer. I mean, I'm not in an imminent danger of dying, but I mean, we're all actually here for an extraordinary short time and there was great wisdom in the past in having a memento-mory on your desk, you know, a skull. And we're not here for very long and it behoves us to behave and to understand the world in the most fruitful way for human fulfillment and happiness. And for the greater fulfillment of whatever it is that we sense in whatever is around us,
Starting point is 01:37:52 in the being of the world, you know, the word cosmos keeps coming to mind, which also in its root means beautiful, because I think what one sees when one looks at the natural order is it is. As scientists and mathematicians are constantly saying It's outstandingly beautiful complex and orderly. Why? Where does that come from? Well, I don't pretend to have an answer But I see the difference is between people who think they know the answer and people who don't I'm one of the people who don't Think they know the answer so please don't ask me what the answer and people who don't. I'm one of the people who don't think they know the answer. So please don't ask me what the answer is. I think I would disqualify myself from having
Starting point is 01:38:28 anything worthwhile to say if I thought I had the answer. The difference is not between atheists on the one hand and and and believers on the other, but it's between fundamentalist believers and fundamentalist atheists on the one hand. And people who often call themselves honest agnostics on the other, that openness of mind, that willingness to acknowledge that one doesn't know everything that one's reaching and searching for something is far more fruitful and spiritual to me than saying, I know it's written down in this book, and these are the rules. The, I wanna go back to the, I listen to all of that, and I wanna go back to this co-creation idea,
Starting point is 01:39:16 because that's not a trivial idea. That's an overwhelmingly massive idea. And, see, I've, the audience, I've talked with, I've talked about the necessity of a vision of life that's sufficiently demanding, meaningful, to justify the trouble of existence. And it seems to me that it's necessary psychologically to be in pursuit of a noble goal. And there isn't a goal that's more noble than the one that you outlined virtually by definition, right? I mean, if you're co-creating the cosmos, but also if you're in a co-creating relationship with God, that involves you at the highest level of being, with the structure of being.
Starting point is 01:40:32 Well, I, you know, again, because we're talking inevitably in shorthand, because this is why this book is so colossally long. I mean, it's apparently as long as the Bible. The reason I had to do that is that I'm, what I'm doing is, is nothing less than this. I'm saying the whole way in which you are taught by your education, by science, popular science, not by quantum physicists who have very much more sophisticated grasp of philosophy, but by the sort of 19th century
Starting point is 01:41:09 mechanism who still dominate biology unhappily, a version of some engineering, if you ever like. This is not a good way to think. This is not even likely to be true. It doesn't answer to any aspect of experience of the world, except the most timely detail. So, for example, what I think is that the whole structure and things
Starting point is 01:41:36 is infinitely complex and has many recurring loops in it. And when you try, and it just an organism is like that, or even a cell, or even, you know, part of a cell, an organelle, is amazingly complex, that the number of interactions, the number of chemical reactions that are going on there is colossal, and they all have cascades that interact with one another. However, if you take from this very large picture, right down to the tiniest, tiniest bit of light, you can see a little chain of arrows. This leads to that, leads to that.
Starting point is 01:42:11 You can't see all kinds of other things going on. And you can identify that. And that's what we're good at doing. And we say, if I interfere in that, I can cause something to happen, which might be beneficial, for example, to somebody who has a health condition. So I'm not saying there's anything wrong with any of this. Again, I come back to,
Starting point is 01:42:31 there's nothing wrong with the left hemisphere. There's only something wrong with it when it adopts the heubristic cloak of knowing everything. And when you said, scientists are not often heard to voice the sort of things that I'm saying and that you are saying. I'd like to say that physicists very often do, but that biologists relatively less often do now, although in the early part of the century there were many great biologists, such as J.B.S. Holden, his father, John Scott Hall Dane, Conrad Haar, Wallington, you know, Ludwig von Bertelansi in Austria, who saw a very sophisticated vision
Starting point is 01:43:13 of the living world of biology. But what happened was because there were great successes in molecular engineering after the war, this vision, which as I say is technically correct that you can interfere as a detail and do something very valuable, but the mistake is extrapolate from that to say this is the structure of the whole thing we're looking at. It's not, it's not like that. The least state on nine ways in which the living being is not like a machine. So I am sorry that scientists diminish themselves by adopting this very narrow vision of what life is because after all the whole point of science
Starting point is 01:43:53 of shreddinger said, it's to answer the question, who are we? And if it's not answering that question and it's not assumable into an answer to that question, it's not really getting us to what the meaning of our life demands from us. That we have not an answer to the question, who are we? Not an answer to the question, what is the meaning of life? Of course, by definition, there isn't one.
Starting point is 01:44:14 But the very knowledge that we have to strive for it and not lose sight of it is very, very important. There's a saying in the midrash, you are not obliged to finish the work, but you are not permitted to cease from it. And I think that is, I mean, I would say that is my vision of what I do. I think it's what all seekers after truth have to do, whether they're philosophers or scientists. And, you know, I would love science to be more scientific. I, the curious thing is I honor science deeply. And I think that it has nothing to lose by making, you know, being a little bit humbler than it is by accepting that there was a lot that is deeply puzzling and that the more we know about physics, for example, the more we understand what we don't
Starting point is 01:45:04 know. And it's not scientific to rule out certain ways of thinking, to say that thinking in terms of organic holes in terms of gestaltan and so forth is somehow not scientific. No, it's just not the way that a certain very narrow form of science is practice. And I want science before I die to become more open
Starting point is 01:45:26 as science should be, to be more questing, more imaginative. In writing this book, I've had a lot to do with the story of how scientists made their discoveries, how mathematicians made their discoveries. And they very rarely, if ever, as George Gaylord Simpson himself said, one of the great mainstream molecular biologists of the last century, it's very rare that they make them by following the scientific method.
Starting point is 01:45:52 The scientific method is a retrospective thing that is fitted onto what actually happens, which is extraordinary insights of intuition, seeing shapes, testing them out, of course, which is a scientific method, of course. But it's not this kind of boring rule bound thing that it's often made out to be. Well, I think with that statement, that's a good place to end for today. It was a nice conclusion. Go on for an hour and a half or so.
Starting point is 01:46:26 And I appreciate. So right with me. Very much you talking with me. Hopefully we can do this again. I wanna get further through your book. I'll keep everybody posted as to the progress of the book and to where they can obtain it when that becomes possible.
Starting point is 01:46:43 Is there anything else that you wanted to mention today that you wanna bring up before we close? Well, no, I mean, I want to say, first of all, which I haven't really had an opportunity to say, how enormously glad I am that you are, you know, back in debate with us all, and long made that be. And I do, I think the conversations we have are good.
Starting point is 01:47:11 I hope other people may think that too. Well, we're gonna put that to the test. And I'd like to just draw people's attention to the fact that in the last six months, it's six months, I think six or seven months, we've developed something called channel McGilchris, which is a place on the internet where you can find out more about my stuff. You can see talks, lectures, podcasts, things I've done, what I'm, you know, and generally keep up to date. There's a forum there where you can enter into and discuss my work.
Starting point is 01:47:46 There's a place where you can ask questions of me. So once a month I answer four questions out of a list of things that members of the, because you can either be a non-member or a member, but if you're a member you can ask me a question which I will then spend quarter of an hour answering. So that's my attempt to try and give back something that isn't just, you know, the old book every 10 years. So, well, we'll make sure that we
Starting point is 01:48:12 put the links and all of that in the description of this video. And with any luck, we'll get a chance to talk again in the future after we've both digested this and this conversation was really good to see you again. Oh, and you. Thank you. you

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.