The Jordan B. Peterson Podcast - 242. Solving The Problem Of Human Perception | Cambridge
Episode Date: April 8, 2022In November 2021, Dr. Peterson traveled to the United Kingdom to give a series of lectures at Oxford and Cambridge. This lecture was given at Lady Mitchell Hall at the University of Cambridge on Novem...ber 23rd, 2021.Dr. Peterson gives an in-depth exploration of the problem of perception. In doing so, he touches on orienting reflexes, artificial intelligence, the infinite possibilities parsed by perception, neurophysiology, postmodernism, and the relationship between imitation, awe, and the divine, before answering questions from the audience about the direction of Western civilization, meaning, and the notion of humans as simple biological machines._______________Chapters_______________[0:00] Intro[1:30] Dr. James Orr Welcomes Dr. Peterson to Cambridge[6:30] The Problem of Perception[8:30] The Orienting Reflex[12:30] The Neuropsychology of Anxiety by Jeffrey Gray [14:30] Perception, Sokolov, & Artificial Intelligence[17:30] Infinity in Perception[22:30] Embodied AI[24:10] Utilization Behavior[27:20] Neurophysiology & Postmodernism[31:15] Dominance Hierarchies[37:40] Basis of Perception[39:30] The Problem of Perception & Wagner’s ‘Master-Singers of Nuremberg’[49:40] Cambridge, Two Chapels, & Primordial Environments[51:10] On Imitation & Awe[59:40] Divine Perception[1:00:00] Q&A: Where is Western society headed?[1:08:50] Does consciousness die with the body? Is meaning—like the universe—doomed? [1:13:00] What is meaning? And its source?[1:17:55] Existence, Hardware, & Divine Imitation#Attention #Perception #AI #Meaning #Consciousness
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to episode 242 of the JBP podcast. I'm Michaela Peterson. In November 2021,
Dad and I traveled to the UK for his series of lectures at Oxford and Cambridge and a debate
on eating meat that I took part in at Oxford Union. I put the debate online. One of the women I
faced off with literally uttered, every hamburger is served with a side of misogyny, and I think my dad died a little inside.
This podcast episodes lecture was given at Lady Mitchell Hall at the University of Cambridge.
Dad spoke about orienting reflexes, artificial intelligence, how perception narrows things from an infinite pool of possibilities,
infinite pool of possibilities, dominant hierarchies, the influence of postmodernism in neurophysiology, which was very interesting, and the relationship between imitation, awe, and the divine.
He finished the lecture with a really thought-provoking Q&A.
I hope you enjoy this podcast. What a pleasure it is to see you all. What a pleasure it is to introduce to you this afternoon.
Someone who has encouraged millions of people,
millions of young people in particular,
to probe, evaluate, ask questions
that are more fundamental than any other.
Questions with which every one of us is confronted
at some point.
Questions involving meaning, identity, relationship, dignity,
what it is to flourish as a person.
He doesn't pretend to have all the answers,
but anyone who has tried walking with him
the length of King's parade here in Cambridge
in less than half an hour
will know that he seems to be asking the right questions.
And he seems to be reaching young minds and young hearts in ways that very few other academics
in the world that I can think of come anywhere close to doing.
He's here in England for a couple of weeks and for most of those two weeks he's going
he has been and will continue to be here in Cambridge.
We are lending him to the other place for a day or two.
LAUGHTER
And as part of that visit, he has been through
what have been at least up until now.
I'll be frank with you, some pretty grueling
and critical seminars, research seminars on his work.
And he has opened himself up to criticism.
He has been receptive to it.
He has responded to it in an exemplary fashion.
He has also taken part in a public lecture, one last night,
at Gondwell and Keys,
hosted by Dr. Arif Ahmed.
Here tonight, the flagship event at the university.
He'll be speaking the Cambridge Union tomorrow,
the other union on Thursday and Westminster next week.
And in accepting this invitation to come to Cambridge, i'n gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'r gweithio i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n ffodd i'n fodd i'n 12 rules for life in 2018 that has sold many millions of copies and has topped best-sellers
lists in Brazil and Netherlands and the United States and Australia and in too many other
countries to mention.
And in the ruins of the multiversity, he is building a metaversity.
His lectures online, these long conversations with public commentators, religious leaders, yn ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymw o fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyrddio i'r fyr I hope you all know that. And it's so beautiful. I hope you all know that.
I hope you all know that.
I hope you all know that.
I hope you all know that.
I hope you all know that.
I hope you all know that.
I hope you all know that.
I hope you all know that.
I hope you all know that.
I hope you all know that.
I hope you all know that. It's such a wonderful place.
And I hope you all know that.
And it's so beautiful and so deep and so rich
and in the best possible way.
And it's been so welcoming and it's such privilege.
It's an unbelievable privilege to have that happen.
So I thank all of you and for your attendance today as well.
And so we're going to try to
work our way through a problem today. It's a problem I've been attempting to wrestle with for a very
long time. And in one way or another, whether we know it or not, we're all wrestling with it.
wrestling with it. And it's the problem of perception. Five decades ago, I suppose, the problem made itself explicitly manifest at a deeper level than it ever had before, although
philosophers had wrestled with this problem for a very long period of time, and part of the problem was, how much do we bring to the act of perception
and imagination and thought, and how much is revealed to us by what we perceive?
We thought we understood that, I would say, well enough to make practical progress after
the Second World War.
But there were doubts that bedeviled people operating in all sorts of disciplines that
became, as I said, increasingly explicit in those decades.
And I think, for me, the most remarkable revelation of the problem probably occurred in the world
of artificial intelligence.
I learned about this when I was studying models
of cognitive processes that were initially generated
by Russian investigators like Sokolov and Vinogodovar,
who were very well-known names in the Russian neuropsychological
literature, which is a very academically impressive literature.
They were students of Luria, I think both Vinogudava and Sokolov
or studies students of Luria, who is perhaps
the greatest neuropsychologist of the last mid-part
of the 20th century.
They were convinced, to some degree,
that we built internal models of the world and then compared what was going on in the world to those
models. Sokolov discovered a phenomenon called the
orienting reflex, which was an electrophysiological response to
error detection or a response to novelty. That's another way of
thinking about it. And he should have won a nail-bale prize for
that because discovering the instinctual basis
of the response to novelty, that's no small thing.
There's a lot of novelty in the world.
And Sokolov really mapped that, in some sense,
mapped that onto the body and onto the nervous system
in a way that superseded what philosophers had done
before that, because it made it much more concrete
and tied it down to the underlying neural architecture.
And so, for example, if you're walking down the road
and you, there's a loud noise behind you
and the noises of indeterminate meaning.
So perhaps a car has jumped a curb.
That's a possibility.
You'll go like that and stop and turn
and orient towards the place in the space time continuum where your stereo
vision has localized the noise.
And you do that really without thinking.
I would say it's an act that occurs outside the domain of free will.
And the reason you do that is because you might die if something unexpected happens,
right?
Something that's outside your framework of expectation.
Now, you know your framework of expectation.
You're an ignorant creature.
You don't know everything.
You don't even know that much.
And your representation of the world is actually rather shallow and low resolution.
It's good enough to get you where you want to go most of the time.
But sometimes it isn't, and sometimes it's error-ridden enough,
given the circumstances of time and place,
that the error will kill you.
And so you're equipped with instinctual mechanisms
that orient you towards the source of the revelation
of your ignorance.
And that's something very interesting to contemplate.
I would say physiologically and neurophysiologically,
but also philosophically, and I would also say
to some degree, theologically, right,
that you have an instinct that
orients you to the source of your ignorance.
And you better have an instinct like that,
because there may be a shortage of knowledge,
but there's no shortage of ignorance.
And so, and that's, you know, part of the problem of the relationship between the finite
and the infinite, right?
I mean, we're finite creatures.
We can't know everything.
We can't even know as much as we need to know.
And that means, in some sense, we have to be able to deal with the fact that we don't
know enough.
And one of what Sokolov outlined, at least in part, was the underlying neurophysiological mechanisms
that made this orienting reflex possible.
One way you can measure, for example,
is if you put a gadget, a galvanometer on someone's finger,
and you play them a sequence of tones,
when you play them the first tone, they'll show quite a response,
and then the second tone lesser, and after that lesser,
until finally, it'll flatline.
And people thought about that as habituation,
but the more sophisticated cognitive science is regarded it as
the building of an internal model of the stimulus,
and the whole of its variety of parameters.
Interesting words, stimulus.
We'll return to that.
And then if you play someone a different tone
after habituating them to that sequence,
the electrophysiological response will re-instantiate itself,
or even if you alter the spacing between the tones,
the same thing will happen.
And that's all part of this response to novelty
and then the mapping of novel territory.
This is unbelievably important this discovery,
because you don't learn anything except by encountering it
as novelty first.
So it's fundamentally the initial processes of everything
you do to learn, everything there is to learn
everywhere all the time.
And so, like I said, he should have won a Nobel Prize.
But people really understand the fundamental significance
of this discovery.
And a very influential line of English, British,
neuropsychology emerged out of that,
because gentlemen named Jeffrey Gray,
who is the most outstanding student,
I would say, of Hans Eising,
who is the most cited psychologist,
research psychologist in the world for pretty much the last half of the 20th century.
Jeffrey Gray wrote an incredibly brilliant book called The Neurose Psychology of Anxiety,
where he integrated the work the Russians had done, which very few people knew about,
apart from Gray who read absolutely everything.
The Neurose Psychology of Anxiety, I I think cited 1300 scientific, deep scientific papers,
neurophysiological papers, animal behavioral papers, like hard core psychology because there
is such a thing.
And he actually read all those papers and he actually understood them.
And then he integrated that with Norbert Wyners, who is one of the fathers of cybernetic
and computer science, he integrated that with Norbert Wynner's cybernetic theory
and with, well, a raft of animal experimental material
and laid out the neurological basis
for the establishment of the orienting reflex
and also for the establishment of memory itself.
It's a tour to force that book was written in 1982
and psychologists have really only begun
to digest the material in that book.
So I read it when I was, well, back in about 1982,
very soon, now it was 85, I guess, very soon after it
was published, it took me like six months to read it,
because, well, I had to learn animal physiology,
neuropsychopharmalology, and animal behavioral science, and cybernetic theory,
and just along the way to understand what he was talking about.
It's a profound work of philosophy, I would say.
It's probably a necessary work of modern philosophy
because we're starting to understand
the underlying physiological substructure
of many of the processes that have been discussed
in the philosophical realm for forever.
And so what does this have to do with this problem?
Well, back to the AI researchers.
So stemming from circle of work, at least in part, was the idea that there's the world
out there, and it's made out of objects objects and they're sort of self-evident.
But what we do is we build an internal model of that world and then we compute trajectories,
we make maps, we compute trajectories, we layout plans that constitute a manipulation of that
internal representation and then we act out the representation. And it's kind of an empirical
idea philosophically speaking and the idea is that sense data is in some sense given to us, and from that sense data, that given self-evident sense data, we build these models, and then that's how we think, and that's how we operate in the world, and that's kind of folk psychology, everybody thinks, well, that's right, that's how it is. It's no, no. It's not right at all. And that's why we don't have general-purpose robots.
Because what happened to the AI researchers was that they tried to build machines that and built toy environments.
So imagine you're trying to build a basic robot, at least initially. You can't model the whole world. So you build toy environments that the AI system can model,
and then you have it.
Do simple things in the toy environment.
It couldn't even get the machines to see the toy environments,
and part of what was discovered in AI
was there were no toy environments.
Well, imagine even if you just have a simple environment
that's made out of pyramids and columns, spheres,
just simple geometric forms, well, you still have the problem of variant lighting.
It's like, well, is a pyramid in the morning the same as a pyramid at midday?
What about five minutes past midday?
Or three seconds past midday, or three seconds past midday. How much illumination changes necessary before the object
isn't the same object? Well, maybe illumination doesn't have
anything to do with the object. Well, that's kind of awkward
because then you don't get to see it. And how is it that we
manage to infer the stability of an object across transformations
of illumination.
And the answer is, we don't know.
And how is it that we're able to perceive objects at all?
Because the other thing that became complicated
and you see this, if you ever use a program like Photoshop,
you can see objects in a photo in Photoshop.
And the objects appear self-evident.
They have boundaries and borders. but if you zoom in,
you can't tell where the boundaries are.
They fade into all the other images that are behind
or ahead or wherever they happen to be displayed.
And then while the image is quite different,
if it's black and white, and then you can highlight
the colors and expand them.
And so there's an endless number of things you can do
with the single image of
anything. Well, you think about what that means. There's an endless number of things you can do
with a single image of anything. Well, how in the world is perception possible then? And the answer is
we didn't know. Well, at the same time, approximately the same time,
the same problem emerged in literary criticism.
And you can see why, in some sense, right?
If you can't perceive something even
simple in some canonical manner that's self-evident,
how in the world can you derive a single reliable
canonical interpretation of a given text?
And then you could multiply that problem, you say,
well, it's bad enough for a single text,
or maybe a single paragraph, or even a sentence,
because sentences are amenable to multiple interpretations,
and complex sentences in the beginning are susceptible to an endless number of interpretations.
Really? Endless? Well, endless is a problem,. And the cognitive psychologist,
Medin and Aguilar, who worked at MIT,
they said, a finite number of objects can be grouped
in a near infinite number of ways.
Well, you think, well, imagine your books on a bookshelf.
Well, you have the books, and then you have the sequence of books.
You think that's all self-evident.
Well, how are you going to arrange those books?
Well, that's a problem if you've ever tried to sort out
your library, and if it's a big library,
it's a big problem, right?
You have to invent a very complex and sophisticated indexing
system to know where the books are.
Well, the books on your shelf, you think, well,
there's not come on.
Really, there's not a near infinite number of ways
to arrange them.
What are you talking about?
And that's only the axiomatic structure of your A prior
perceptions manifesting itself as self-evident fact
to your ignorant mind.
Because that claim is actually wrong.
So you might say, well, color, thickness, density, age. How about thickness of paper? How about the thickness of the spot exactly half an inch below the 35th page in the third chapter?
And you think, well, that's a stupid way of organizing your books. And I would say, well, how do you know it's stupid, exactly?
And not sort of, right?
Because you can't just claim self-evidence in this situation, because the self-evidence of the stupidity of that categorical structure is actually the mystery that, say, postmodernists encountered when they were trying to specify the canonical meaning of a text,
just a single text, a single paragraph, a single sentence.
There's a multiplicity of potential interpretations, even worse, there's a multiplicity of potentially valid interpretations.
And so how do you do anything that throw up your hands and say, well, there is no solution to that problem.
Or at least, and this would be better,
we have no idea how we solve this problem.
And what would even be better would be,
we have no idea how we solved it in the past.
And say that's particularly germane when you think not
even necessarily so much about the interpretation of a given text.
Let's say the Bible to take a complex problem, but the canon of texts itself.
Now the canon of texts, the fact that there's canon of texts, roughly speaking, we have agreed in the past to some degree on the boundaries of that category.
Although there's plenty of cognitive activity
around the edges trying to decide what would fit in and what wouldn't, which is exactly what happened,
for example, when people are trying to aggregate the biblical corpus across time, because the Bible
is, of course, a library of books, what's in and what's out, why is it in, why is it out? Well,
the answer to how we answer that is we don't know how we answer that. And there's this process of deliberation, let's say, that is
part and parcel of the process that gives rise to the
aggregation of a library of texts into a corpus.
But we don't really understand the mechanism.
We don't understand the mechanism at all.
And that's actually all fine except that we don't have
general purpose robots yet, although they probably are more or less around the corner
in about five years,
partly because the AI researchers solve this problem
and part of the way they solved it
was by embodying cognition,
incarnating artificial intelligence
in an embodied structure.
And the first people to really propose that that was absolutely necessary.
I don't necessarily know the first people, but I know that an MIT researcher named Rodney Brooks,
who by the way invented the Rumba, some of you may have a Rumba,
and it's kind of a laughable little object, but not really,
because it can sort of move around your house without falling down the stairs.
And year two-year-old can't do that.
So the Rumba isn't nothing, right?
And Brooks was one of the first people who really recognized
that to solve the problem of perception,
we would have to duplicate the process of evolution in hardware.
And so he started building these little machines
that pretty much all they did to begin with was scoot away from light.
And well, eventually that became the Rumba, which is a pretty useful gadget. And you see variants in some sense of what he's done with self-driving cars because they're embodied systems as well, right?
Because the car is actually a body that moves from place to place. And it turns out that a lot of our perceptions require
embodiment, and it turns out that one of the philosophical
consequences, implications of that, is that the way we
solved the problem of perception was through 3.5
billion years, or thereabouts, of evolution.
And also, that there was no other way of possibly solving it.
And so that's a testimony to the power of evolutionary thinking,
I would say, but even more a testimony to the power of the process
of evolutionary development.
And you might think that that reduces human cognition
to something sterile and mechanistic,
because many of the proponents of the notion of natural selection have adopted a fairly
reductive materialism to account for the process of natural selection.
And we're going to talk about that a little bit tonight, too.
I hope if I can manage to tie all these things together.
So no perception without embodiment.
That's pretty interesting that.
And so I could tell you a little side
in neuropsychological story.
So we tend to think that when we see the world,
while there the world is, and then we see the world,
and we see the objects, and there the objects are.
But that isn't how it works.
And I've tried to explain why because there's an infinite number of ways of
perceiving even the simplest of visual scenes.
And then there's auditory scenes and then there's the problem of smell and
there's the problem of touch.
I mean, these are hard problems.
Just why it took, you know, three billion years to solve them.
There's a condition called utilization behavior.
It's got an interesting neuropsychological condition.
And generally, if it is affected right-handed people,
that's relevant here because of lateralization,
if you have left prefrontal damage,
you sometimes will engage in utilization behavior.
And what happens if you're afflicted by this neuropsychological
condition is that you lose the ability to inhibit your motor
response to the presentation of an object.
Now, that's worth thinking about, even though it doesn't sound
like it's something that's necessarily worth thinking about,
because what do you mean motor response to an object?
Because we think object thought motor response,
but that's not how it works.
The object itself announces its utility in the perception.
And so what that means is that your eyes, which map,
let's say patterns of arrays, that's a good way
of thinking about it, they map that onto your visual system, but part of your visual system is actually your
motor output system.
And so when I look at, let's say this bottle, you think I think bottle, hand, grip, drink,
but seeing bottle is hand grip, and hand grip is drink, but seeing bottle is hand grip and hand grip is drink.
And so if you have a utilization behavior, you lose the ability to inhibit the motor
response to the object.
And so if you had this condition, I put a cup in front of you, you would pick it up and
drink from it.
And if you walk down a hallway and there's a door open, you will go through the door.
And it's not because you see the object door and think door and then
think walk through and then walk through, even though that's what you think you think.
It's that door is a walk through place. And if you lack an ambition, you can't stop acting
out the perception. And so what that implies is that at some direct level, and this is the science, not the philosophy,
you don't see objects in infer meaning.
You see meaning and infer objects,
and that's really something.
You can think about that for like 40 years,
because it looks like it's true factually.
And that's a strange thing too, right?
It's very strange,
claim to say that the fact support the notion
that the primary object of perception is meaning,
not objects.
And I actually think that's an interpretation,
though I won't go into that tonight,
that's more in keeping with evolutionary logic
than the idea that you perceive objects and infer meaning.
And so the stripping away of meaning metaphysically
from the world of perception, that's a consequence of
scientism is actually predicated on something approximating
bad science. And that's become increasingly evident,
I would say, in the neurophysiology of perception. And that's an important field,
given that the problem of perception
is the problem that has bedeviled both the sciences
and the humanities and also engineering for that matter
for the last 50 years.
And it has not only done that,
it's produced this riveting of culture
that's occurred within the universities
partly because of the postmodernist claim.
And so fundamental postmodernist claims.
Now, the first claim is that there's no canonical interpretation
that's self-evidence.
Like, okay, fair enough.
Fair enough, that might be a claim emerging from
literary departments, English literary departments,
under the influence of French continental thought, let's say.
I don't think you can take issue with that.
I think it happens to be the case, because the notion that
there is a multiplicity of potentially valid
interpretations is true.
What isn't true is the idea that we use power and domination
to solve that problem primarily.
And there's no excuse philosophically from leaping from a mystery that's utterly profound,
which is the problem of perception,
to the conclusion that some pathological, socially
constructed process is therefore at the basis of the active
perception and categorization itself.
I think that's, I think it's unforgivable, cognitively, and it's cynical beyond belief,
and it's corrosive beyond our capacity to deal with it. I mean, you think about what
that claim means is that the way you solve the, and that's like an implicit bias argument, by the way,
just to make that clear as well, that you
solve the problem of categorization
by imposing your will to power on the world,
in some zero, some winner-take-all game of dominion
and oppression.
It's like, you could not, if you tried, you could not come up with a more
cynical view of the mechanism that makes order, habitable order out of chaos. And what's
sort of delightful about that in some sense is that it's just not true. I, literally,
we could say scientifically, if we take scientifically to mean literally, we could say scientifically, if we take
scientifically to mean literally, we can say,
it's not true.
It actually turns out that even in the animal kingdom,
we know youth might think the hierarchies that we use to orient ourselves in,
that's part of the strategy that we use to guarantee our survival.
You know, we fight tooth and nail with nature red and claw
to climb the hierarchies of dominance
as a consequence of our will to power.
It's like, no, actually, that isn't how it works.
And so, we all have this image, or many of us,
I would say, have an image of chimpanzee troops,
let's say, our closest primate relative.
We split from them in evolutionary terms about 7 million
years ago, something like that.
You can calculate that quite accurately by looking at the
mutation rate of genetic material that we share with chimps
and calculate the average mutation rate and look at the
propagation of mutations and the separation of them.
You can get a pretty accurate estimate of the split.
It's a long time ago.
So we're pretty tightly related to chimpanzees.
And we know that we all know that the biggest toughest,
meanest male oppressive chimpe rules the troop.
It's like, no, wrong.
Friends to all has studied dominance hierarchies,
so-called dominance hierarchies.
And that's an interesting issue that that is the term
that's most often used, dominance hierarchy.
Because you might ask, as a very intelligent student of mine
once did, just exactly where did that term come from?
And how much political implication was loaded in that right from the beginning?
I thought about that for about five years.
I thought it was an unbelievably, I mean that.
I thought it was an unbelievably acute observation.
He was a very, is a very acute fellow.
So the brute chimpanzees, they have a pretty short
tyrannical rule.
And the reason for that is, I don't care how big and tough and
mean and dominant and oppressive you are, too slightly less
mean oppressive and dominant males can take you out pretty
easily on a bad day.
And that's a real problem.
You think about that biologically.
That's a non-trivial problem.
And so that is exactly what happens in chimpanzee troops
is that the males who rise to a position of authority
as a mere consequence of their psychopathic will to power
rule very unstable over very unstable troops
and have a very short ruling period.
Whereas the males that manage to, because in chimps,
the fundamental hierarchy is male dominated.
It's less obvious in another set of very close
primate relatives, the Bortembois.
There are more female dominated, interestingly enough.
And it isn't obvious which of those two species
were more like.
So, but I'll concentrate on the chimps now, because we're
talking a little bit about will to power.
The males who manage to maintain a relatively stable coalition,
let's say, are actually very affiliative.
In fact, they groom other males more than any other males
in the troop.
And so they're using reciprocal altruism, at least in part,
as the basis of their claim to dominion.
Now, they do have preferential mating access to females.
And that, there's some analogy in that with what happens in the human case,
but it's more complicated in the human case.
In any case, they also tend to spend a fair bit of time
attending to the females in the troop in a positive way
and to their infants.
And so the more benevolent, but still physically able males
seem to establish a stable social hierarchy
that's quite unlike the social hierarchy
that's produced by the more psychopathic,
straight raw will to power.
And well, and here's another fact for what it's worth.
If the hierarchy that we use to aggregate the canon
is fundamentally predicated on impressive power, which
is the claim, then why does psychopaths only constitute 3% of the population?
And that's actually stable.
So there are good evolutionary biology, psychology, models of psychopathy.
Its emergence has been modeled quite, emergence and stability has been modeled quite nicely in computer simulations,
and such simulations can be used as appropriate models.
If the psychopath prevalence falls below 1%,
there's so few of them that people
get complacent in relationship to the possibility
of malevolence.
And so then they can flourish a bit, right?
And their prevalence can rise in the population.
But if it gets up to 5%, it's like everybody wakes up,
and it's not such a good day for the psychopaths when they do.
And somebody has to keep the malevolent types down, and that's probably possibly, possibly
we'll say, why women like men who are less agreeable than they are.
And women have to solve this very complex mating problem in relationship to men,
which is, there are bad men.
They're really bad.
And they're a minority of the population.
And they tear their way through overly agreeable populations.
And to keep them from dominating, you need good men who are also capable of being quite terrible.
And so that's appears to be the conundrum
that women face when they're choosing male partners,
one of the conundrums.
They want someone who's strong enough to resist
what's truly terrible, but benevolent and generous enough to be productive and useful, but also
agreeable and empathic enough to share.
And you can see that that's a terribly tight line, right?
And women are always, well, young women tend to overshoot the
mark on the malevolent side.
So the psychopathic Machiavellians,
who mimic competent male behavior,
they ape that, so to speak,
are differentially attractive to young and inexperienced women,
whereas the women who've had some experience
get better at distinguishing the psychopathic Machiavellian
pretenders who often manifest a certain confidence and a certain bravado
bravado, they get good at distinguishing them from the real thing. And so it's a tricky thing for women to manage and
men can use deceptive strategies to mimic confidence, which is exactly what deceptive strategies are for, obviously.
And and that's also a pathway to short-term mating success
under some circumstances.
And it's viable enough to propagate itself in the population,
but it's not a good medium to long-term strategy.
And the question might be, well, what
is a good medium to long-term strategy?
And it looks like something, well, that is the question
that's actually at the bottom of this entire talk, because it's also the same question as what is the process that gives rise to perception itself?
This is an attempt to integrate across a whole variety of complex questions.
So you might ask yourself, this is where it gets difficult to
tile these things together. So let me see how I'm going to be able to do this.
Yes, there's no evidence as far as I can tell that the
proposition that the fundamental motivation for
categorization itself is the expression of power.
That seems wrong. It's not stable.
So then the question is, what is exactly at the base of the
process of perception and categorization?
And so that would be really right at the basis of cognition itself, perhaps the essence of consciousness itself,
and certainly the thing that acts at the interface between what is not yet known and what is going to be known, right? The active investigation that transforms what's unexpected
and potentially dangerous into what's habitable,
safe, competent, and secure.
So this opera, but you didn't think I was going there.
I saw this opera in New York City about three weeks ago,
opera written by a dead white male of the oppressive sort, Wagner.
And so he sort of weighs way up there on that list, man.
And it was the opera die, Meister Singer.
And it was really interesting.
The libretto was really interesting.
It really dovetailed in a strange way
with the sorts of things I happen to be writing about
at that point.
And I'll just run through it quickly.
And hopefully that will help tie together
of these strands that I've laid all over the place.
Now, so while music does that, it ties things together.
And great art does that.
It ties things together. And it, and it's part of the process
by which we make order out of chaos, right?
Great art, not power, great art.
And you have to be so cynical that it begs description
and so envious of what's great to reflexively identify
that with the will to power.
I mean, really, that's what you think when you enter
one of the great cathedrals or chapels
that are the grace of your campus.
You think nothing but will to power erected that?
Corrupt oppression?
And if you do think that, well, what do you think of yourself then?
You think you're nothing but the expression
of the corrupt will to power?
Or you somehow circumvented that because of your moral piety?
And, well, in that case, then what is it you're relying on to orient yourself in the world?
Is something other than that will to power?
Well, if so, then exactly what it is.
What is it?
Well, you come to university to ally yourself with the forces of great art, let's say.
And that's so much more powerful than mere power, that they're not even in the same category.
Well, back to Dimistra Singer.
So it's a very interesting opera because it's set in Nuremberg in Nuremberg in the opera.
There are these guilds of men, and they're all craftsmen.
And so one of the heroes of the story
There's two heroes and a heroine in the story
He's a cobbler and he's a really good cobbler and you think
My it's just a cobbler and it's like it was so funny because when I went to see this opera my shoes were didn't fit
I had these shoes that I hadn't really paid attention to for like three years
And my feet were just killing me.
And I was in this opera and it emphasized the absolute moral necessity of attending to
your shoes properly.
And I thought, huh, isn't that synchronous?
We'll say.
And it certainly was.
And now I have shoes that fit, although these are somewhat ugly, but they do at least fit.
So I partially solved the problem.
In any case, Dimester Singer is the master singer.
And so these men that are all extremely skilled craftsmen.
So there are people who have skilled right at the level
of the interface with the world.
They get together in guilds of their own type.
And then they also practice singing. And one of them, who's the skilled craftsman, because that's a prerequisite, is elected as a master singer.
And so each of these guilds have master singers. And now and then they elect a new master singer, and all the master singers get together to elect a new master singer. I was thinking about this little trope,
I'd already written down in the book, I'm writing.
While I was watching this, you see this,
and if you watch American sports films,
so there's a football team.
And after overcoming great odds, the quarterback,
who's probably risen above know, risen above his
suffering in some manner triumphantly, produces the victory in all the other
football players, put him up on their shoulders and lead him out of the stadium
and everybody's standing up and cheering and then he has a fair with his
girlfriend that
topped cheerleader, and everyone leaves happily ever after.
And that's the same motif as the Meister singer.
It's really interesting, you know, that the men will put that other man on their
shoulders. It's not a good long-term mating strategy that,
right, to elevate him above you in that sort of competition. But men do that all
the time, interestingly,
nothing in Dimester Singer, the man in the guilds come together
and a new entrant onto the potential master singer stage
comes into the town and he's at night and he's wanted
through nature and he's things of nature,
but he's completely undisciplined,
he doesn't know any of the rules.
And so he's known to master craftsmen like these craftsmen.
But he wants to be a master singer.
Simultaneously, one of the other leaders of the guilds
offers his entire fortune and the hand of his daughter
to the new master singer.
And he thinks, well, that's a pretty patriarchal trope.
But it's actually handled extraordinarily brilliantly,
I think, within the confines of the opera,
because she is the heroine of the story.
And although her father is offering her one of the masters,
she has the right of choice.
And it's not sort of, she has the choice a little bit.
It's clearly part of the opera that she has the choice.
And she falls in love with this night.
It's just hardly a surprise, but women are perverse like that.
But the master singers, they don't know what to do with this guy because he's unbelievably
gifted in terms of his talent, but he's not a master.
He's not a craftsman.
He hasn't gone through the disciplinary process
that would mold him into someone who's thoroughly
united right from the bottom of the craft
to the tip of the head.
And so they're thrown into disarray by this,
and also by the fact that this woman,
her instinct drives her towards him.
And so they degenerate. The men's skills degenerates into kind of an
internecine squabble. And it's complicated by the fact that the woman also kind of
like some of the other master singers, they're older, they're competent. And so
they have a shot at her hand. And so they fall into disarray under the stress of
this mating competition.
We could say that biologically.
And the cobbler, who's the paramount hero in the story,
who the woman loves, but perhaps not as much as the night.
And he's old, he's like 55, so he's the younger than me, but he's too old, you know.
This is a young woman.
And so he decides he's going to train this young knight.
And that's pretty damn interesting, you know, because he thinks
the moral thing to do here is to take myself out of the competition
and to raise this untudered but extraordinarily talented young man,
man, to a position of primacy, to unite nature and culture simultaneously in his form,
and to help him attain the status of Master Singer.
And his name is Johann Saks,
and Johann is John, and he's John the Baptist.
And I'm not just inventing that,
it's not just my patriarchal,
power-driven inference on the multiplicity
of potential meanings in the text,
because Wagner basically says that in the little bread.
And a few times, in case you don't catch it the first time.
And so in some manner, this knight is Christ.
And that's a strange thing.
But you know, you can understand it, right?
It doesn't, it's not like it doesn't make sense in some sense,
because Christ is always presented as a superordinate ideal.
And obviously, this new master's singer,
who unites the melody of nature and the discipline of culture,
is a master of his craft by divine grace because
of his talent, but also now because Han Saks, Han Saks, that's Johann, that's John the
Baptist, decides to train him.
And so now he's a model of discipline and craft, and that's allied with natural talent,
and that gives a whole new depth to his voice,
because now that natural talent has been properly brought
under a discipline structure,
which is a reflection of the guild structure
of the entire Meister Singer contest.
And suffice it to say that Saks pulls himself out of the race
and decides to sacrifice himself in some sense
for this young man.
And the woman chooses the night, and the night undergoes this disciplinary regimen,
and then he sings in the contest, and all the master singers now can, what would you say,
live comfortably with their conscience, and they elect him to the highest position,
and then, socks, and the night, and the woman are celebrated.
It's lovely, brilliant.
And then of course it's set to this remarkable music
and watch all these people who spend all these years
disciplining themselves in some unbelievably difficult manner
to play their instruments properly
and to interact with each other harmoniously
and to play while they're doing it
so that it's not just wrote and to produce this magnificent stage
in this ridiculous building,
this ridiculously impressive building,
in this unbelievably impressive city
and all these people come there
and devote your attention to watching this.
Well, why?
Why?
Why indeed?
Well, that Master, senior, spirit,
that's what solves the problem of perception.
It's not raw power.
It's not a corrupt will.
It's not a satanic force.
It's exactly the opposite of that.
And we all know that.
We all know that, although we don't know, we know it.
I was in the chapel the other day here.
It was, which one was it?
You have two remarkable chapels, well, more than two,
but two particularly remarkable chapels.
Doesn't matter, really.
Up on the pinnacle of the chapel interior, there's a picture
of Christ, and it's like a Byzantine representation.
He's in a mandorla, which is a shape that Freudians can have no
shortage of fun with.
And he's shining forth from this background, and he's placed above the sky, right, or on the sky.
And you see this even more clearly in Byzantine church architecture.
So the cathedral is a cross, and then at the central point of the cross,
so that's the point of maximal suffering.
That's what's being illustrated in the architecture.
There's a dome, and the dome is, well, how about it's the sky?
It's not that hard to figure out.
And the cathedral structure is trees.
And so it's a representation of our primordial environment,
this ancient forest, which is now recreated in stone
with this dome that's above all that's centered at the point of the cross, which is the point of
noximal suffering.
And you look up into the sky, the starry firmament,
and what do you see reflected back down to you?
You see this image of the divine word. That's what you see. You might not even know
that you're seeing that. You don't know you're in a forest. You don't know you're seeing the sunlight
filter through the branches. You know our ancestral home for millions, tens of millions of years.
You don't see the immense labor and effort that it took to erect that cathedral and to put that image in its highest place.
At least she dressed like a lobster.
That was good, actually.
And as far as protests go, relatively witty, although perhaps somewhat ill-timed.
witty, although perhaps somewhat ill-timed. So here's a way of thinking about that.
So I've been discussing a series of images with my wife, and one of them is an image of
Mary.
And it's a very, it was a renaissance image that was painted by many, many people.
And it's funny, I'm going to talk about the divine feminine
after that interruption.
Mary is often represented with her head surrounded by stars
and with her foot on a serpent on the world.
And so, well, what is that exactly, that image?
Well, it's something like, what is it
to have your head
in the stars, let's say?
Well, you know, when you,
I bought this cottage up north in,
in Northern Canada, and it's very dark up there.
And you can go on to the dark at night,
and it's dark enough so you can see the Milky Way, you know,
and it has to be pretty dark before you can see the Milky Way.
And it's very impressive, you know what that's like, if you, you know, can see the Milky Way, you know, and it has to be pretty dark before you can see the Milky Way. And it's very impressive. You know what that's like if you, you know, to see the night sky,
or maybe you see the feel the same way when you see the Grand Canyon or a remarkable waterfall or some
particularly beautiful scene, or perhaps you feel that way in a cathedral or more likely you feel that way when you're listening to music that really grips you, right?
And so all the same experience and experience of all, and it's way down low in your nervous system like the orientating
reflex. It's not a cognitive response precisely. It's a precognitive emotional response that
signifies significance. And you look up at the night sky and it fills you with a sense
of awe, but it doesn't just do that. You see, it activates
the impulse to imitate, which is a very deep motivation in human beings. We're unbelievable
mimics. We mimic each other all the time, which is why we all use the same words, let's
say. We're very good at embodying other people's embodiments. It's a particular talent
that human beings have, and we're so good at that, that we imitate all sorts
of things that aren't even human.
And so then you view this expansive night sky
and a sense of awe fills you as you confront the infinite.
It calls to something inside of you that can master the infinite, and that's a form of
imitation, right, to look into the darkest place, the most wide, experienced possible,
and to have something inside you respond that's capable of dealing with that.
That's that instinct to imitate, and that's calling the best out of you, and that's why you dealing with that. That's that instinct to imitate,
and that's calling the best out of you,
and that's why you love doing that.
And it isn't just that you love it,
is that you cannot live without it.
You cannot live without it.
And I know so many of you, atheists, or otherwise,
you can't live without music.
You think, well, why can you not live without music?
And what is it calling, too, precisely?
You know, that remarkable interplay of harmonious patterns,
because that's what music is, and that's what the world is.
It's not objects.
It's the harmonious interplay of patterns,
and music reflects that, and then you warrant yourself
in your embodied manner to those patterns,
and dance along with the world.
And that revivifies you, and if you're particularly good at, well, maybe you'll also attract a mate.
And you want a mate that doesn't detect attempt to dominate you sexually during the introductory dance, right?
You want a mate who will play along with you and match your movements to theirs so that you can see that there's a harmonious interplay between the two of you as you meet in play, soul to soul, if you can manage it.
And everyone knows that, and that capacity that's called out in the dance is the same capacity
that's called out by the night sky, and it's the same thing that's represented in those
Byzantine churches. You look deep enough into infinity and you
find your destiny. And that destiny is everything you could be. And we all know that because this is
what men and women search for each other. You know, if you're rejected by a woman, why is she
rejecting you? Well, maybe her judgment is off and that would be very, what would you say, convenient for you?
But she's rejecting you because you are not all that you could be,
and maybe not even all that you need to be.
And so that's a very painful rejection,
and it causes all sorts of tension between men and women.
But, you know, women have a lot of stake in this game.
And so they're looking for something, powerful, dominating, brutal, terrible,
no, something perhaps capable of that, but even more important, capable of mastering it,
and capable of singing despite that. We all know that's true in the shame that men feel
when they're rejected by women is precisely the shame that they feel at knowing deep in their heart that they have not lived up to
what they are capable of being. And that harsh judgment that women lay on men, which by
the way is part of our sexual evolution, because we were shaped by sexual selection, which
by the way is the operation of consciousness on the structures of matter at the most basic
possible level.
Well, it's a terrible rejection, but it's a
salutary rejection.
And that process of differentiated joins the shape
this into what we are, that action of consciousness,
wanting the best from a potential partner
and selecting at least in part on that basis.
A man participate that in that too,
and then, master singer manner.
Men are competing for dominance with each other constantly
in a what zero sum game to achieve sexual dominance.
There's an element of that, right?
Because some things are a zero-sum game,
but men are perfectly capable,
and more than willing, in fact,
to aggregate themselves into skilled groups
and to celebrate the elevation
of the most skilled above all else.
And so we see this co-opored adventure
between men and women over the longest run of possible time
in producing
some refinement of the human spirit in embodied form.
We want that from everyone.
We require that from everyone.
We're thrilled to the core of our soul when we encounter it in a conversation or in a course
or in a work of art.
It calls to us in that manner.
But we need to know this increasingly. We need to know this increasingly.
We need to know all this consciously.
We've acted it out.
We've produced images to represent it.
It's tugs at our heartstrings.
It manifests itself in our dreams and our work of art,
our literary works.
It's all lurking there in some sense.
And it's not a satanic power of corrupt oppression,
not fundamentally. That's a far weaker force than that which can overcome it.
And everything around this would be nothing but hell if that's all there was.
And everything around this is not only hell.
You know, for fragile and broken creatures ignorant to the core,
For fragile and broken creatures ignorant to the core, we don't do too badly. And people are capable of a nobility, especially under the duress of suffering that's virtually miraculous when you encounter it.
And it's so heartening to see that, and you've seen it in the people that you love when they're going through terrible trials.
You know, people become corrupt and imbitered by their catastrophes, and it's no wonder. But certainly in the main, that's not the fundamental human
response.
The fundamental human response is, keep calm and carry on,
you know?
And good on you for that.
So, well, to sum up, let's say we solve the power of perception with the divine word.
That's how it is.
And what does that mean?
Well, it means truth.
Every word, a prayer, right?
Every word, a groping to find a firm foundation to stand on
while you make your way through life.
And every time you hear a conversation of that sort or hear yourself participating in
that prayerful process, orienting yourself to this highest,
uniting good and using that to govern your utterance.
It's, it's born for the soul.
Yeah, it's love that guides that and love is the desire to work for the betterment of
all things.
And that's the proper orienting response we could say.
And it's truth nested inside of that.
And that's how it is.
And that's how it should be.
And that's how it may forever be. Thank you. I think it's a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a little bit more of a But that's where we're at now.
Yes. Very much.
So am I taking questions or is there someone pointing out?
Shall I do it?
Yes.
Okay.
Thank you so much for this passionate... for this passionate,
this passionate, defensive reason.
For those of us who are on your side, I'm just curious,
do you have any forward looking views on where we're going?
All the Bay, you know, this whole,
I mean, what you've been going through for the last few years,
every argument you've had,
do you have any sort of midterm to longterm view
and where we're going?
Or if you don't, that's fine, yeah.
Well, we're going in many directions at once.
And the question is, is the fundamental trajectory downhill
or uphill?
And I would say, that depends on you.
It Western society in particular.
What's that?
Western society.
Yes, yes, no, and more globally.
I mean, I worked on the UN Secretary General's report
on sustainable development for about two years
and read a very large number of texts
on environmental problems and opportunities
and economic development.
And what happened to me was that I got way more optimistic
than I was before I started reading those books.
I mean, so many things have happened in the last 40 years
that are so good, you just can't believe it.
I mean, we've lifted more people out of abject poverty
in the last 15 years than in the entire course
of human history, in terms of sheer numbers of people.
You know, in starvation, except for political reasons,
is now pretty much absent across the world.
There hasn't been any wars in the Western Hemisphere
for about a decade.
That's really something.
And no major wars, Blackis at the moment.
That's quite something given that there are seven billion of us.
And there's only going to be nine billion by by all appearances, it's going to peak out
at about nine billion, and my suspicions are,
in 100 years, one of the biggest problems will face
is that there's just not enough people.
And you never hear that, but I really do believe
it's likely to be the case, and we can certainly carry
nine billion people without doing the plan
and undo environmental damage, and people
who claim otherwise, I think, well, I think a lot of things about that.
But one of the things I don't think
is that that's an accurate viewpoint.
I mean, we're doing far better than we were 40 years ago,
feeding people, and we can certainly
pack in another $2 billion.
It turns out that if you want to control population,
though I wouldn't really recommend that as an occupation,
all you have to do is educate women, and that's the end of
that problem.
Then you also have educated women, and we know that's very
annoying, but it seems to be working out.
It's a great predictor of general economic development.
It's actually, I think, the best predictor of the society's
future economic development is the attitude that they hold
towards the education of women. And luckily it's in the positive direction. And so that's very cool.
And then it certainly seems to be the case that the fastest way out of a given environmental
conundrum is to make absolutely poor people richer as fast as you possibly can, because
then they do things like,
well, they don't burn wood anymore.
Maybe they burn coal, and I know coal is evil,
but it's not as evil as wood.
And I don't know if you know this,
but 1.6 million children die every year
because of the indoor pollution that wood burning causes.
It's like if the nuclear industry had a record like that,
that would be all over the newspapers,
but they're just third world children after all.
So, you know, the planet has too many people on it anyways.
And so, there's all sorts of things I see
that are so radically positive that it bangers description.
I mean, Indian, China, alone have greened an area
because of agricultural transformation,
the size of the Amazon, and partly has a consequence
of increased carbon dioxide levels, and semi-arid area.
It's either the size of the Amazon or Alaska.
I don't remember which has greened in the last 15 years.
And so these are things you never hear.
You have to fer it them out.
But as far as I can tell, if we got our act together
and actually wanted it instead of wanting to burn everything
to the ground in an orgy of guilt-ridden self-destruction,
we could set up a world in 15 years where absolutely everyone
had plenty to eat and where obesity would be the primary
problem.
It's a good problem actually. It's like, oh no, you know, we have too much food.
What are we going to do? That's a good problem.
And where everyone was educated because the cost of education is falling precipitously.
And we could do that in a way that was actually beneficial to the environment, whatever that is.
So, so I would say fundamentally I'm optimistic,
but if we want hell, we could certainly have that.
And you might say, well, you don't want hell.
It's like, yeah, really, eh?
You might want to ask your question yourself that question
real seriously, because there's a party that would wreak
that would wreak vengeance on God for the catastrophic suffering are that of a corrupt will. I think that's wrong.
I think it's wrong factually, and I think it's an appalling claim philosophically, and
it's a radically destructive claim ethically, demoralizing, terribly demoralizing claim,
and demoralizing enough to really hurt people.
And I've seen many, many people, maybe thousands of people,
maybe tens of thousands of people hurt by that claim,
hurt to the deep recesses of their soul.
But I would say, it depends on what you choose to do.
Really, it depends on what you choose to do.
I read once, I think this was an association, it's a novel,
although it might not have been, and it's certainly not his idea. It's an idea from one of the church
fathers that God is a circle who centers everywhere and whose circumference is nowhere.
Nice mathematical model of God, I like it a lot, but there's something about it that's
really true, you know. We interact
with one another as if there's a spark of the divine within us and you say, well, no, we don't.
It's like, well, if you don't, then no one likes you. So, you know, that'll be its own
punishment because we certainly do interact with the people we value when we're acting in a
manner that we regard as appropriate as if there there is something about the misof transcendent, and in some sense, eternal value. And you might say, well, you don't
believe that. It's like, well, do you believe in natural rights? Because the notion of natural
rights is predicated on that underlying presupposition or observation. And you don't believe in
natural rights. Well, then again, where are you exactly and who are you
exactly?
And those are questions very much worth posing.
And so I think that truth is more powerful than deceit by a large margin.
And I think love is more powerful than hate by a large margin.
I don't mean naive love, and I'm not naive about people.
I don't mean naive love, and I'm not naive about people. I don't mean that at all, but I think it is possible for us to rise above the resentment
of our suffering and to wish the best for all things.
And I think we can participate in that.
And you do that while by extending your hand to your enemy to the degree you're capable
of doing that, because who needs enemies, or maybe you do, but it'd be better not to have
the my think, even if they're convenient targets to defeat.
And then truth, well, that's the handmaiden of love.
And that's something everyone can practice at every moment, if they desire that.
And that's an adventure, you know.
If you're acting deceitfully, you already specify the outcomes of your actions, and you pursue
that. you already specify the outcomes of your actions, and you pursue that, but the problem with that is it's predicated on the acceptance of your own authoritarian completeness.
It's like, what the hell do you know about what you should have?
And so what do you do instead?
You just do your best to not lie and see what happens.
And what happens are wonderful things, although perhaps not at the beginning,
when there's a lot of mystic claw through.
So sorry for the lengthy answer,
but it was a complicated question.
Does consciousness die with a body
and is meaning doomed like the universe?
Well, I don't know.
I would say.
I mean, we don't understand consciousness.
We don't really understand its place in the cosmos, let's say.
But I'm not qualified to answer such questions.
I would say, though, that no one stops listening to a symphony
because they know it's going to end.
And so, I think, in some sense, our proper task is to find the meaning within the finite,
when I have clinical clients who are consumed with such questions,
because you can pick a time frame of evaluation that makes all your efforts futile.
Well, the sun is going to envelop the earth.
I think it's 4 billion years, so get ready.
It's like, well, what's the point of stopping this baby from crying when the sun is going to
develop the earth in four?
Well, yeah, you all laugh, right?
But that laughter, you see, that's a sign of wisdom.
You know, that's preposterous.
Why?
I mean, that's the existential question, right?
It's like, well, if we're all doomed to ashes and decay,
why do anything?
Well, I use the baby crying for a reason.
I mean, who in the world is going to use that argument
to not feed their baby?
Well, why feed that thing in four billion years?
The sun's going to develop the earth.
It's like wrong time frame, folks.
And so what I would say, and I did say to my clinical clients,
if you're adopting a time frame that makes what you're doing
appear trivial, the problem isn't necessarily what you're
doing, although it might be.
And you have to ask yourself that question,
because perhaps you are engaging in something that's more
trivial than you should be.
The problem is that your mind, which is capable of leaping
across evaluative frameworks, has picked a time frame
inappropriate for the task.
So quit doing that.
Instead, you could say, well, why don't you
practice adopting the time frame that
impuse your properly oriented action
with the deepest possible apprehended meaning?
And why would you not think that the fact that that meaning manifests itself with the proper choice of time frame?
Why wouldn't you accept the fact that's indication of a valid choice?
It certainly feels like it.
You know what it's like? You get engaged in something,
a deep conversation, a piece of music, a piece of art, something you love doing, someone you love being with,
you get engaged in that, you lose your sense of temporality. And you don't pop out of them and think, oh my God,
I wish I would use the time frame that made everything irrelevant because of my cognitive
brilliance, you think, hey, we could do that some more, like how about all the time?
And that's a good goal. It's like, yeah, how about that all the time? And then you've
got time right when you're engaged like that.
And I would say that's a profound neurophysiological signal
that you're in the right place at the right time,
because it's accompanied by a sense of deep well-being.
And that's literally an antidote suffering.
I mean, that literally, it's with many of my clients
who are suffering, what we would strive to do was not so much make them happy
because sometimes that was impossible.
They were so crippled in so many ways,
often physically and in pain, but something meaningful.
I would keep them going and keep them from straying
and keep them from thinking,
homicidal and genocidal thoughts, all of that,
and meaning that's the antidote to suffering.
And the question is, well, how is that best to be found?
Well, that's an empirical question.
You have to look in your own life and see where meaning glimmers
and then pursue that.
That's what Harry Potter is doing, by the way,
when he's chasing the snitch.
Just thought I'd let you know.
And you know, you win the game if you catch that thing.
So, well, that's not exactly right. You get a hundred points, but it'll do.
So, another question. How about you?
Hi, thank you very much for the lecture. You talk a lot about meaning, both from perception
and a musical arts. What do you mean by meaning? What is meaning and where is its source?
Meaning is implication for action or for reorganisation of the perceptual frames that frame action.
So there.
And is that the sole aim of what,
is that what you find in art and music?
Yes, in complex ways.
I mean, in music, you find this demand
that the music lays upon you to orient yourself
in relationship to this harmonious interplay of patterns.
And you might say, well, why is that meaningful?
Well, that's a good question.
Well, it's because you're acting out something like the adaptation of your soul to the structures of reality itself.
Now, it's done very abstractly because the patterns of music are not precisely the actual patterns of the world,
their abstractions, but it's play and representation,
and it's art.
And so you're acting out the process of optimal adaptation
at a very high level.
When you think what people are doing,
imagine a V&E's walls, so you have this unbelievably well-trained
orchestra, they're all emitting patterns like mad,
and they're playing while they're doing that,
putting little twists on the patterns
so that they're a little novel, a little interesting,
even if you've heard the music many times.
You have the conductor who's keeping
all these specialized subsections operating in harmony,
and then you have the couples dancing,
and they're trained to do that,
but they're cutting the rug, you know, in the same way. They have their dancing, and they're trained to do that, but they're cutting the rug,
you know, in the same way.
They have their moves, and they're trying to impress each other,
and there's a mating aspect of that,
and they're all doing that harmoniously,
and it's a complete vision of an ordered society, right,
from the subatomic realm, let's say,
all the way up to the cosmic realm.
That's all taking place in the dance,
and people don't know that, but, well, they do know it too,
and they know it in that they're acting it out.
And there isn't anything in some sense
that you know more deeply, or believe more deeply,
than that which you act out.
And you're not smart enough to understand
the full totality of your actions.
I mean, we're not transparent to ourselves.
We act out all sorts of things that are stunningly brilliant
without realizing it and it takes in some sense,
it takes often untold centuries for us to figure out
what we were doing and why.
And so that happens to you in your own life,
when you have a flash of insight into your own behavior.
That's why I was doing that. It's like, well, you were doing it. Why didn't you know? Well,
you're complicated. You're really complicated and certainly not transparent to yourself.
And so I have a paper called Three Types of Meaning. You could look that up if you wanted a more technical answer,
but the music answer by example is a good one.
And I think people can really relate to it,
because the only person you ever hear who says,
well, I don't really like music, is like, no,
that's just a posture.
You like music.
You just want to be kind of interestingly different
and controversial.
And it's really something, right?
And it's also interesting that music has this non-provisitional structure that's completely
opaque to rational argumentation.
I used to like to watch punk rockers, especially the ones who did Mosh Pit punk rock.
And I went to a Ramones concert once.
It was quite comical.
I was on the second floor.
It was so loud.
I couldn't, like, couldn't hear for three days after this concert,
because it was pretty little theater.
We sat about 800 people, and they had their stadium speakers in there.
It was like sonic wall of sound.
And we were above this Marshpit, and there was all these, like,
nihilistic punk rockers down there smashing into each other and throwing themselves off the stage, and I
thought, for all this talk of nihilism, there you are dancing to the harmonious patterns
of life, it's like, you know, smash this state and all of that, it's like, hmm, that's
that, we'll groove to that, yeah, it's very comical. So even among the most propositional nihilistic,
they still fall in love with music.
It might be harsh and grating to some ears,
but you start where you can.
And I like the remon.
So that was fine with me.
So maybe, yes.
Well, thank you so much for being here.
Do you have a sample of the improvisation behavior? Yes. To brain damage. And You're a sample of the humanisation behaviour.
Yes.
To brain damage.
Yes.
And heaps and heaps of neuroscience all seem to suggest that we are merely, that consciousness
is merely an app running on a biological machine.
The thing that, without being the decider really, the thing that John Height calls the elephant
and the rider.
Yeah.
And how does this, my question is, how does this grim dark materialist view of existence
square with looking for meaning, imitating the divine?
Well, I think the grim dark view and the biologically determinist view is just wrong again.
And so consciousness, consciousness is not merely an epithet dominant of matter and not
that we know what that would mean anyways.
Like, what the hell does that mean?
It's something we don't understand matter.
We might think we understand it,
but all you have to do is familiarize yourself
a little bit with quantum theory to understand
that you don't understand matter at all.
It's like, what is that stuff?
And obviously, consciousness is implicit in it,
in some sense, because here
it is and we're all conscious and we have no idea how that managed itself. And then the
thing about, but more specifically I would say, you know, when Darwin wrote his great
tracks on evolutionary theory, he stressed two elements of the selection process, natural
selection, fair enough, and you could make a determin selection process, natural selection, fair enough.
And you could make a deterministic argument
for natural selection.
It's not easy, because nature is really complicated.
And the idea that nature is selecting,
that's from a random array of potential traits,
let's say, although I'm not convinced
that that's entirely random, by the way.
But we won't get into that.
Nature selects from this random array of traits, and I think that capitalization on randomness in that manner is necessary to solve the complex process a problem of perception over a very long span of time.
But there's sexual selection. Now, it's a scandal in scientific history, as far as I'm concerned, concerned that for almost 100 years after Darwin published his
great works on sexual selection,
biologists tended to pretty much ignore it.
It's like, yeah, no, natural selection.
And that was because I think it was easier to maintain
a strict determinism by concentrating on natural selection.
The tricky thing about sexual selection is, how is that not conscious choice?
I mean, what, you don't make a conscious choice
when you select, well, maybe you don't,
if you've had enough alcohol,
I wouldn't, you know, recommend that,
as a long-term mating strategy,
but you tell me that the conscious choice
of women specifically, it's more complex in the case of men,
because we're an easier,
what would you say? We don't have as much at stake, and so we're not as choosy. Women are exceptionally choosy,
and certainly it's like a truism among evolutionary biologists that part of the reason that we had such
rapid cortical expansion is because of sexual selection. It's like, how is that not the action of consciousness
on matter?
And you might say, well, that's only
been operating since Homo sapiens,
because nothing was conscious before then.
And it's like, you ever see that BBC clip of the pufferfish
making the Mandela?
Oh, well, you could look that up.
There's little pufferfish.
He's like this long.
He's just a pufferfish.
It doesn't have any hands, which is kind of hard.
If you're hard problem, if you want to be a sculptor,
he makes this sculpture.
It's like 20 feet across.
He's this big, 20 feet across at the bottom of the ocean.
And it's a perfect circle and quite
complexly undulated and wavy.
It's not the sort of Mandela you would see in a great cathedral.
But he's just a fish, man. It's not so bad, you know?
And he spends like a week building this thing. And it's so funny watching him in the film because he goes down there and he, like maybe there's a stray piece of shell and he grabs that.
He spits it out because no shells in the damn sculpture. It has to be clean. And then he pops up and he turns one eye like a bird, and he looks at it, and then he goes down
and waves a little sand into place.
He's making these dunes that are like a foot high,
and there's like 400 of them,
and then you see an aerial shot of it.
It's this, really, it's the size of this stage.
And then this female pufferfish comes long
and you know, checks it out,
sees if he's got what it takes.
And if he does, the way they go. It it's like it isn't obvious to me at all that that puffer
fish isn't conscious and I would say say while your answer purmorphizing it's
like okay let's have that discussion so I'm pretty familiar with the animal
experimental literature and the greatest animal experimentalists especially
those that study motivation and emotion.
So they're the ones that are delving very deep
into the neurophysiological apparatus.
They're basically rule of thumb is you answer
purmorphize, except when there is a reason not to.
I think we share like 85% of our genes with yeast.
It's like rats, they're pretty complicated.
They play, they laugh.
You can tickle them.
They die without love.
You know, pufferfish, they make sculptures.
Here's a story about spiders.
This is a fun story, if you like stories about spiders.
So there's these spiders,
and the female won't mate with the male,
unless the male offers her a gift.
And so he has to find some dead fly
or something particularly delicious to a female,
and then wrap it really nicely in a web
and presents it to her.
And if she likes it and it's a good fly,
then maybe she'll date and to mate with him.
But the damn spiders, it's so funny.
Some of them will wrap up dirt and present that.
It's like they tend not to get away with it, you know, Some of them will wrap up dirt and present that.
It's like they tend not to get away with it, but sometimes they do, so that's pretty funny,
but it's also funny.
Sometimes the female will eat the fly and leave the guy.
You know, it is agitated state, let's say.
It's like, you know, those behaviors,
those are complex, man.
And it isn't obvious to me at all that consciousness doesn't exist
way down the phylogenetic chain.
I mean, maybe it emerges in some form
with a differentiated nervous system.
We don't know.
But friends to all, who's a great primatologist,
just wrote a book called, like, are we smart enough
to know how smart animals are?
And the answer to that could well be no.
I mean, octopus is, for example, man, those things are smart.
And they can do all sorts of things we can't do.
They can transform the texture of their surface,
as well as the color to match an underlying rock.
It's like they'll clamp onto a rock and then poof.
They're exactly like the rock.
It's like, that's hard.
And it's hard to imagine how something like that is
possible even without the intermediation of something
like consciousness.
And I cannot see at all how you can be a biologist and
believe in sexual selection and think that only random
factors determine evolution.
It's like, what about made choice?
Well, yeah, no, no, really. What
about made choice? Really? And you might say, well, that's not aiming at some determined
at end, and that's complicated. And that's worthy of discussion. But it's not obvious
to me at all that in the human case, it's not aiming at some idealized end.
I mean, we certainly look for something
approximating an ideal in a mate.
We want that.
And we want to encourage it if we don't have it to begin with,
unless we're bitter and resentful and jealous.
And so we are pushing towards an ideal that's at least implicit.
And it governs us at every level of our social interactions.
And so I don't think that it is a dark reduction of consciousness to an underlying, say,
ultimately real material state, and that's the final answer.
I don't think that's true.
And there's lots of people who aren't foolish who don't think that's true. And there's lots of people who aren't foolish who don't think
it's true. It isn't obvious to me that Roger Pemrose thinks it's true. You know, and he's no lightweight.
So he thinks consciousness is irreducible in some sense. And I think the biblical idea that
consciousness calls forth shape from a material substrate is there's
something to that and that's certainly not an idea that's limited in religious
texts to the biblical stories in Genesis. If you look at religious texts all over
the world there's always this insistence that there are two primal factors that
work. One is the matrix out of which things emerges and another is something
that calls forth structure
from that matrix. And it's a chicken and egg problem, you know, to use a terrible cliché,
but it's an extraordinary widespread, fundamental theological idea. So I think I have to stop.
I'm getting messages from people that I'm doing my best to ignore.
So thank you very much for the great welcome
and for attending the talk tonight.
And you have a wonderful time. Well, Jordan, thank you so much for terrific Mae'r fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn fawr yn f profes yn ffrosfi yn y universtig. Dwi'n ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r ddellunio'r delluniounio'r ddellunio'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordd o'r ffordmru'r Ysgwyrdd, ac yn ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ywch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch ymwch i'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n gwybod ymwyr i'n gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn gwybod yn, gweithio'r gweithio, gweithio'r gweithio, gweithio'r gweithio, gweithio'r
gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r
gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r
gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r
gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r
gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r
gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio'r gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn gweithio yn I am ymwch i'n ydwch i'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n fwy'n So thank you very much. you