The Jordan B. Peterson Podcast - 269. The Biology of Good and Evil | Frans de Waal

Episode Date: July 11, 2022

Frans de Waal is an acclaimed Dutch primatologist and ethologist. He's written and published numerous books, including 'Chimpanzee Cultures,' 'Are We Smart Enough to Know How Smart Animals Are?', 'Mam...'s Last Hug,' and his most recent book, 'Different: Gender Through the Eyes of a Primatologist.'In this episode, Frans de Waal and I discuss a number of things, including the instinct for reciprocal cooperation, the characteristics of sex and gender, the necessity of play, reconciliation, how we mismeasure animals, and much more. Thanks for watching. —Links— Read Frans de Waal's books: https://www.amazon.com/Frans-De-Waal/e/B000APOHE0%3Fref=dbs_a_mng_rwt_scns_share —Chapters—[0:00] Intro[6:44] A Background in Ethology[9:46] The Social Organization of Chimpanzees [12:11] Supporters Make Chimps Dominant[15:56] An Instinct for Reciprocal Cooperation[17:37] Female Choice in Sexual Selection[22:45] Biology's Victorian Beginnings [24:37] Bonobo's Collective Dominance[27:42] Characteristics of Sex and Gender[31:02] Preferences in Types of Play[33:26] The Origin of Antisocial Behavior [35:43] The Necessity of Play[37:13] How Play Teaches Self Control[41:01] Self Socialization[46:18] Interference in Boys' Development[51:34] The Behavior of Reconciliation[57:29] Differences in Male and Female Aggression [59:35] Peace Making vs. Peace Keeping[1:01:59] The Conundrum of Compassion[1:03:11] Competitiveness In Males and Females[1:06:20] Disliking the Facts of Sex Differences[1:11:36] How We Mismeasure Animals[1:21:24] Anthropomorphizing Animals[1:23:26] Consciousness in Animals[1:27:15] Sentience [1:31:16] Self Consciousness and Embellishment [1:35:50] Unconscious Olfaction [1:38:53] Problems with Virtualizing the World[1:40:41] Frans de Waal's Intellectual Heroes[1:42:20] Closing Comments#fransdewaal #reciprocity #primatology #jordanpeterson #animalbehavior #dominance// SUPPORT THIS CHANNEL // Newsletter: https://mailchi.mp/jordanbpeterson.co... Donations: https://jordanbpeterson.com/donate // COURSES // Discovering Personality: https://jordanbpeterson.com/personality Self Authoring Suite: https://selfauthoring.com Understand Myself (personality test): https://understandmyself.com // BOOKS // Beyond Order: 12 More Rules for Life: https://jordanbpeterson.com/Beyond-Order 12 Rules for Life: An Antidote to Chaos: https://jordanbpeterson.com/12-rules-... Maps of Meaning: The Architecture of Belief: https://jordanbpeterson.com/maps-of-m... // LINKS // Website: https://jordanbpeterson.com Events: https://jordanbpeterson.com/events Blog: https://jordanbpeterson.com/blog Podcast: https://jordanbpeterson.com/podcast // SOCIAL // Twitter: https://twitter.com/jordanbpeterson Instagram: https://instagram.com/jordan.b.peterson Facebook: https://facebook.com/drjordanpeterson Telegram: https://t.me/DrJordanPeterson All socials: https://linktr.ee/drjordanbpeterson

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Welcome to episode 269 of the Jordan B. Peterson podcast. I'm McCarrot Peterson. In this episode, Dad spoke with Franz DeWall, a Dutch primatologist and the ethylologist. Throughout his career, DeWall has published numerous books about the complexity of animal behavior. His latest book, Different, published in 2022, looks at sex differences in humans, chimpanzees, and bano bones. Dad was really excited to chat with friends to all because he's had a great influence on his thought. Together, the two of them discussed the instinct
Starting point is 00:00:32 for reciprocal cooperation, the necessity of play, reconciliation, how we miss-measure animals, and more. Thank you for listening. I hope you enjoyed this episode. Hello everyone. I am really thrilled, even more than usual, because I'm usually thrilled with my guests to be talking to Dr. brands to wall. Dr. to wall is a Dutch American biologist and primatologist known for his work
Starting point is 00:01:13 on the behavior and social intelligence of our closest biological relatives. He is C.H. Canva Professor E. Maritus at Emory University and Distinguished Professor E. Maritus at Utrecht University. His scientific work has been published in hundreds of technical articles in journals such as Science, Nature, Scientific American,
Starting point is 00:01:34 and Outlet Specialized in Animal Behavior. And I should point out that generally speaking in a scientist's career, even a single publication in a journal such as Science or Nature can be the pinnacle of a career. a scientist's career, even a single publication in a journal such as Science or Nature, can be the pinnacle of a career, and to do that multiple times is pretty rare. And so, Dr. DeWal is a scientist who's in many ways in the league of his own. His popular books translated into more than 20 languages have made him one of the world's
Starting point is 00:02:01 most visible primatologists and scientists, I would say, his latest two books of many, and I'll mention some others, are Mama's last hug, Norton 2019, and different gender through the eyes of her primatologist, which was published by Norton in 2022. He's been elected to the U.S. National Academy of Sciences, as well as the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences. In 2007, time declared him one of the world's 100 most influential people today. Now, I would also say that I'm particularly thrilled to be talking to Tarte, through D. Wall, because his work has had a real influence on my thought. Now, I'm not so happy, he might be about that, but he is one of the world's deepest thinkers on a variety of important issues, perhaps most importantly, on the biological basis of morality,
Starting point is 00:02:54 the development of morality, and his work on the development of the moral sentiments, you might say, in chimpanzees and moral behavior, and his analysis of hierarchical behavior in chimpanzees and moral behavior and his analysis of hierarchical behavior in chimpanzees and bonobos is I think revolutionary not only biologically but also philosophically and that's something he has delved into. I would say equally that's the case for his work on play and his work on gender. DeWol is one of the few people who have made a really solid case for a specifically sophisticated view of the construction, let's say, of hierarchies in primates, which are often pilloried, say, with regards to chimpanzees, as predicated on something like brute force and power. And the fact that DeWall has
Starting point is 00:03:44 indicated quite clearly that that is insufficient to say the least view of the complexity of such hierarchical organization is work of tremendous importance partly because it allows for the union in some real sense of is and ought because that's an old philosophical conundrum. Can we derive an ought from it is? And the answer to that is not in any simple way, but the fact does remain that there are That's an old philosophical conundrum. Can we derive an ought from it is? And the answer to that is not in any simple way, but the fact does remain that there are elements of social organization in our closest primate relatives that do shine some light on the biological foundations of ethics itself.
Starting point is 00:04:20 His work on peacemaking among primates is also signally important in my estimation. I've read about seven of his books peacemaking among primates. I thought that was a great book. That was 1989. chimpanzee cultures which he edited with Richard Rangham was another guest on my show, another great primatologist. Good nature, the origins of right and wrong and humans and other animals, another great book. Bonoble, The Forgotten Eight, an analysis of another very close relative of ours, biologically speaking, genetically speaking, who but as a chimpsub type in some sense, or a napesub type, that organizes its social community quite differently than chimpanzees. He wrote,
Starting point is 00:05:04 Mama's last hug, as I mentioned before, animal emotions and what they tell us about ourselves. Are we smart enough to know how smart animals are, which is another great book, and most recently, different gender through the eyes of a primatologist. I would highly recommend to all of those who are watching and listening that you pick up any or all of that you pick up any or all of Dr. DeWall's books. They're extremely, they're very straightforward. They're easy to read. They're deep. They're well grounded in a multi-dimensional, in the scientific literature in a multi-dimensional manner. I think he's one of the world's most important psychologists, even though that's not his primary field. His books are a delight to read. And he certainly influenced my thinking more than anyone else with the
Starting point is 00:05:50 hand, exception of a handful of people like Yacht Panks, who's done great work on similar grounds with rats in the realm of biology. So I'm so thrilled to have you on today. So thanks for a quick gushing and we'll get to it. Thanks for all the praise. Yeah, well, I mean, it's, it wasn't praise, man. It's, uh, it's, uh, it's the truth. Uh, your work has been un, of the unbelievable interest to me, especially on the, as I said, especially on the front of the development of morality. So maybe we could, you want to start with just, maybe a little bit of bio and just tell everyone,
Starting point is 00:06:30 what you do and how you developed what you do. You said, for example, that you were influenced by Nico Tinberg and then maybe we could just go through the whole ethylogy background and make it personal, then we'll get down to brass tacks after that. Well, Nico Tindberg was a Dutch etologist. Ethologists are biologists who study behavior of animals, mostly naturalistic behavior. So that was a big difference with skin, let's say, in the psychology who put rats in a box and let them press levels. Ethologists wanted to have natural behavior that they looked at.
Starting point is 00:07:04 the etologies wanted to have natural behavior that they looked at. And Tinberg was a Dutchman. I'm a Dutchman. I'm from the same school, basically, even though I'm not a direct student of his. And I was trained to observe animals. That's mostly in the beginning of my career. That's what I did. I observed animals, observed chimpanzees, bonobos, other animals.
Starting point is 00:07:24 And then later, when I moved to the US, observed animals, observed chimpanzees, bonobos, other animals. And then later, when I moved to the US, in the 1980s, I moved first to Wisconsin and worked with monkeys there, and then I moved to Emery, much 10 years later. I started to do experiments, behavioral experiments, not invasive studies, just a chim would come out of a group into a room and do something on a touchscreen or a tool or whatever the experiment was. And so later I started to do
Starting point is 00:07:52 more experimental approaches and got interested in very different behaviors such as reconciliation after fights or empathy, how they have responded to the distress of others. And so I developed ideas about cooperation and empathy. In a time that people were still quite a bit focused on competition and aggression and violence, which was the early focus of mythology, really. Right. So there was this assumption that that was held long, I would say, across animal species that who were organized themselves into a social community, that the social hierarchy, which is almost an inevitable consequence of a community, was predicated on something like dominance. Hence the word dominance hierarchy.
Starting point is 00:08:39 I had a graduate student, or he's a colleague of mine now, but about seven years ago, he told me that I should stop using the word dominance hierarchy. graduate student, or he's a colleague of mine now, but about seven years ago, he told me that I should stop using the word dominance hierarchy. And I asked him, why? Because I used that phrase a lot. And he said, well, dominance is when you put a chain around someone's neck and their naked and you can lead them around and you can get them to do anything you want. And our hierarchies, our functional hierarchies are not based on dominance. And then he said, and I think that
Starting point is 00:09:07 that's that idea is a consequence of the invasion of Marxist derived ideas implicitly into the biological domain. And like it really took me back because I had used that term a lot. And I thought, oh, he's really onto something there, because I think our hierarchies, when they're functional, are predicated on competence and reciprocity. And it's only when they become pathological that they're based on power. And then I came across your work, which, well, it's before that too. And you, well, I want you to talk about that. So let's talk about the hierarchical, the social organization of chimpanzees on the male
Starting point is 00:09:44 and female side and what you've observed it. Yeah, so the dominant relationship is a two-way street, you know, so it's very hard to dominate a bunch of people who don't want to be dominated or don't want to be led. So it's always, you have the followers and the ones who are dominant, but the idea that dominance is purely based on coercion and power is, I think, simplistic. You need a party who wants to be dominated. And in order to be dominated, you need to give them certain things also. So the dominant is not purely coercive.
Starting point is 00:10:24 It does happen. I call them usually bullies, and we have them in human society too. It does happen also in Chippewa City society that a male is just big and strong and orders others around. That kind of males are not very popular, and the group is basically waiting for a challenger, and if a challenger comes up and they come to support the challenger, do you want to get rid of a meal like that? Yeah, well, one of the things that's so revolutionary about that, I believe, on the philosophical front, is that I think your work, as well as panxeps with rats, has shown that that kind of coercion and power actually constitutes a very unstable basis for social organization.
Starting point is 00:11:06 And that if you engage in that, as you said, you'll gain enemies. And that even happens at the animal level in some real sophisticated sense. And the probability that as you've also described, when you're a bully, and you have an off day, two of your subordinates that you've bullied can do a pretty good job of tearing you into pieces. Yeah, yeah, yeah. So the stable alpha male and most alpha males that I've known in chimpanzees, they break up fights, they keep the peace,
Starting point is 00:11:42 they defend the underdog, so they defend a juvenile against an adult or a female against a male. They are empathic actually, they reassure individuals who are distressed and they can become extremely popular. So people always imagine that the alpha male must be frightening and that everyone is scared of them. That is a certain respect for them, so there is a certain fear involved, but they can be extremely popular. Yes, and that's that indication of stability, because so this is one of the things that's so key. Do you know about Yacht Pankseps work on with rats?
Starting point is 00:12:21 I know Pankseps, I knew him. Not a couple of years ago,, I knew him. Yeah. Okay. So, it had a couple of years ago, but I knew him, yeah, sure. So you know that he showed that if you repeatedly put juvenile males together to play, that the larger rat can dominate the smaller rat with no problem. But if the larger rat doesn't let the little rat win 30% of the time across repeated play bouts, then the little rat will stop playing. And so it's a surprise me.
Starting point is 00:12:47 Yeah. Well, it's so cool, you know, to see that alongside your work, because it's one of these indications as far as I'm concerned that the rules that govern a single interaction, which might be that I can win by dominance, are not the same rules that govern repeated interactions within a social context. And your work has done a lovely job of fleshing out the sophisticated complexity of behavior and attitude necessary, even at the chimpanzee level, so that the troop remains stable because you also described how when there's violence between two males, say,
Starting point is 00:13:28 how when there's violence between two males, say, buying for a status shift and a better position, that the entire troop gets agitated and that part of the regulation of that male violence that can spiral out of control is actually distributed into the social community. Yes. So what happens is that it's also in the way you become dominant. How do you reach the top position, often in a chimps society, that is with the support of others? And so I can become the alpha male, but I may have one or two male supporters. I may need female support.
Starting point is 00:14:01 And as a result, also, I need to be nice to my support is because if I'm not nice to them, they will stop supporting me. And my position is based on them. As a result, you can also get the smallest male can be the alpha male that happens sometimes in a in a frequency group is the smallest male is the dominant male. And you wonder how that happens, but that's because he has supporters. It also means that a female can be very powerful. Like I described in my previous book, Mama's last hug.
Starting point is 00:14:30 Mama, the alpha female, who was alpha for 40 years. And so she saw a lot of males come and go. And she was crucial in the alpha male business. And so if a male wanted to be the dominant male, he needed her support, because he had the support of all the females. And so it's almost like a democratic system in that sense, is that you need the support of others and you need to keep your supporters happy. So it's a very different, people have this one-sided view, which comes from the early
Starting point is 00:14:59 baboon studies, I think. They have this view of the male who dominates everyone and orders everyone around. But that's really a simplistic view and that applies maybe to these baboons, but certainly not to chimps and babos. Yeah, well, that, that, that, that your work also highlights the crucial importance and multi-species importance of something like the principle of reciprocity, right? And you use words like nice and empathic, which, and then you justify that, we can talk about that so-called answer to morphism as we go along. But the idea that a stable society, and also the idea that the individuals that make
Starting point is 00:15:42 up that society are psychologically stable, and that would be not too stressed, let's say, that that depends intrinsically on a degree of mutuality and reciprocity. That's really a revolutionary idea to ground that in biology. Yeah, reciprocity is very important because the alpha male, if he has a supporter who keeps him in that position, then he needs to give that supporter a lot of things. Otherwise, what would be in it for the supporter, you know? Well, what kind of things do you see the alpha doing in this more reciprocal sense?
Starting point is 00:16:15 Well, the typical situation is that the alpha male is dependent on another male. He will let the other male mate with females, which is what dominance is really all about among the males is that access to females. And so he may be intolerant to every other male and keep them away from females, but his buddy who made him alpha, he let them mate with females.
Starting point is 00:16:38 Because if he doesn't, and I've seen this happen, if he doesn't, then his buddy will revolt and will stop supporting him. Right, so that also indicates, so you might ask yourself why the instinct to allow for reciprocal dominance exists in human beings,
Starting point is 00:16:57 given that access to mating privileges is such a crucial element of reproduction. But if part of that is, is that if we can make coalitions with our superordinates, say, make coalitions, even though we're not dominant, and that ability to form coalitions upward is associated with sexual access, because the dominant male chimps, they chase other males away from the females, right? The females will mate, this is something that makes them different than human females,
Starting point is 00:17:29 the female chimps will mate with pretty much any male in their nestress if I understand it properly. But the more dominant males will know, is that wrong? Yeah, the females, there's a big issue with female choice, as we call it. Females have preferences for certain males, and these preferences don't need to correspond with the male hierarchy. And so we used to think that the dominant male would sigh our most offspring
Starting point is 00:17:53 because we saw them, the dominant males made more often than other males, but females do all sorts of things behind the bushes and at night. And so we now know from paternity testing that the dominant male is not always the one who has the most offspring. And that is because females have preferences, different preferences. Right. Right. Is the, would you say, okay, so, so let me, let me update myself in relationship to that. I understood my understanding of the primatology literature and insofar as it extended to humans was that human females
Starting point is 00:18:28 were in some sense unique in the degree to which they exercised sexual choice with concealed ovulation and so forth. And that that was part of perhaps what drove our rapid departure away from chimpanzees, let's say, on the cognitive front. But you're saying as well that choice in chimpanzee females, female choice is plays a more important role than might have originally been predicted. I think it started with the birds.
Starting point is 00:18:55 We have monogamous birds, songbirds, male female, and they have a nestless egg. And we've always assumed that the male was the father of all these fertilized all the eggs. But now since paternity testing, they started in the 1970s. We know that if you look at the eggs, you very often find extra pair of males in there.
Starting point is 00:19:19 So different males who have made it with the female. Initially, the biologists assumed, very Victorian type of assumption. They assumed that, of course, the female was probably raped by other males or something like that. But now we know that the females actively look for sex with other males. And so in the birth literature, it's very well known. And we distinguish social monogamy from genetic monogamy. And most of the monogamy that we see is social is not necessarily genetic in the sense that
Starting point is 00:19:52 the male is the father of all the offspring. And since then we have now lots of studies on other animals, including the primates that we know, for example, a female chimpanzee, she mates with many more males than you would, then would be necessary to be fertilized. She mates with lots of males. And she has certain preferences that we now learn about, that it's called female choice, and that's a big literature on female behavior. So females have very often preferences that deviate from the male hierarchy. And yeah, so let me push you on that a bit. So I looked at human studies and the correlation between sexual success and socio-economic status
Starting point is 00:20:34 for men in relationship to sexual success with women is about somewhere between 0.6 and 0.7, which is an unbelievably stunningly high correlation. Yeah, yeah. It's like it's higher than the correlation between intelligence and academic performance, for example, which is maybe the second highest correlation of that sort. We know. And so, but, but that shows in human beings that the female preference and the male hierarchy are pretty tightly aligned.
Starting point is 00:21:00 Do you, how close are they aligned in chimps? And if they're not aligned, what are the females looking for that isn't signaled by the hierarchical structure of the males? In humans, of course, the female preference has also to do with status and income in the sense of resources, which is not so much the case in chimpanzees, I would say, because you don't have a nuclear family structure, where they don't obviously gather resources to say.
Starting point is 00:21:32 Yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah. So we humans have a nuclear family structure, so the males are involved in the family and in caring for offspring, which is not really the case in but over in chimpanzees. And so the human female has a more complex picture in front of her, which includes maybe sexual preference, which may be based on the body or the appearance and so on,
Starting point is 00:21:56 but it also is based on, she also looks for resources. So I think it's a more complex picture probably. So I'm curious about that too, because tell me what you think of this. It seems to me that although socioeconomic status and mating success in human males is very tightly associated, that human females aren't looking so much for status or even socioeconomic position, they're using those as markers for the ability to generate resources and social status.
Starting point is 00:22:26 And so because really what they want, and I think this is tied with your work on reciprocity and social skill in chimpanzees, what the human females really want is competence and generosity, and they use social status and resource acquisition as a marker for that. That's very well possible. I'm not sure if familiar with that human literature, but there is a big field now of female initiative, female choice, female sexuality, biology started out pretty Victorian in the sense that female sexuality didn't exist, females were passive sexually, males were active and had a sex drive, but females basically didn't need it, that's how the biology thought about it, and all that thinking has
Starting point is 00:23:17 changed, and in my book on gender I talk quite a bit about female sexuality and the size of the clitoris and so on, which is related to that, seeking pleasure and so on. And I think it's highly significant that we see that female chimpanzees and certainly female bonobos, they have quite a bit more sex than is necessary for strictly for reproduction. So they are very adventurous. And the female bonobo has a big clitor little, bigger than the human female, and the biggest one is in the dolphin, which is also a sexually adventurous species. So people are not paying attention to that, whereas we went through a long
Starting point is 00:23:56 period where female sexuality will basically ignore it, was irrelevant. So in the Bonobos particularly, the social organization differs from that of the chimp quite markedly, despite the close genetic association between the species. And the bonobos have often been portrayed as, well, let's say, hippie, hippie chimps, because it's free-lobs sex, and in some sense, a more matriarchal social structure. So why do you think those differences emerged between those two subspecies? And I know you've drawn lessons from both, are they species or subspecies they can mate?
Starting point is 00:24:36 So how are they technically defined? Well, we do call them species. They belong to the same genus. But those are female dominated. And the female dominance is a collective dominance. A single female cannot dominate a single male. If you see it's zoos that happens sometimes that you see one male and one female benabou, which is of course very atypical, but then the male is dominant. As soon as you add a second female, the females become dominant.
Starting point is 00:25:05 It's a collective dominance that they have, high level of female solidarity, serviced by a lot of sex and grooming between the females. And why they have this different society, I think it's made possible by the fact that the females can travel together. There is enough food in their forests and they don't have competition from gorillas. They don't live together with gorillas in the forest. That they have more food available that allows the females to travel together. Actually, you mentioned Richard Rangham. Some of these are these ecological ideas come from Richard Rangham. Is that the female binobo, they can stay together as a group and that allows them to have this very powerful sisterhood. Whereas the female chimpanzees, in therefore as they need to spread out in order to find
Starting point is 00:25:51 enough food and so that kind of bonding that happens in the Bonobo is not really possible for them. So this is, sorry, go ahead. Yeah, and in captivity, because I've worked, of course, mostly in captivity, in large chimpanzee colonies in zoo settings, then the female chimps, they are actually very powerful. Because then they're altogether, they're forced to live together, of course, and then they develop some sort of sisterhood. It's not as well developed as in the buddhabo, but the power difference between males and females becomes very different than captivity.
Starting point is 00:26:27 Okay, well, this is a good segue into the sex and gender discussion. So in your book, the difference, your new book, you distinguish as do the more radical political types now, sex and gender. And I was interested in that for a variety of reasons. So when I look at that issue, I look at it from the perspective of a personality psychologist. So I the notion that sex itself is binary is fine with me, but the notion that there is something that might be conceptualized as gender, although I think that's a bad term, is also, I think, reasonable because
Starting point is 00:27:06 although I think that's a bad term is also, I think, reasonable because human beings very substantially from individual to individual in terms of personality on the five cardinal dimensions of personality. And there are plenty of men who have a typical female personality structure and plenty of females that have a typical male personality structure. And then if you add to that variation in creativity, which gives you a kind of fluidity of identity, it's sort of a hallmark of creativity, then the notion that there's a gender that's separate from sex starts to take on some validity. But I like to approach it from the personality dimension. I mean, men and women overlap quite a
Starting point is 00:27:42 lot in their personality. So that's why. Yeah, I think that's entirely correct. I would say sex is divided, biological sex is divided in mostly male and female. And there is in between category, but sex is based on genetics on chromosomes, on genitals, on hormones, that's what sex is. Gender is divided, better divided in masculine and feminine than in male and female. So I would say gender is masculine and feminine and everything in between all sorts of combinations are possible. And gender is a much more flexible concept. That was introduced.
Starting point is 00:28:28 The origin of gender is John Monty, the sexologist, who introduced the term, because he had noticed that there were people who were born with one sex, but in the course of their life, felt a belong to another sex. And so he felt he needed to have a term for that. And they only at the time they only had negative labels for these people, like weird, queer, and abnormal, and whatever. And he wanted to have a more scientific and a friendly label for them. And that's why he came up with the word gender. He was also the first one to set up a gender clinic. So the word gender relates more to the cultural side,
Starting point is 00:29:08 how we expect men and women to behave, the social norms that we have, the education that we give them. And so that's the gender side, and the gender side is obviously much more flexible and much more fluent than that. So I wanna ask you about that too, because you said that the gender side is more socialized.
Starting point is 00:29:27 And I'm not so sure about that. I think this separation, I mean, I'm not unsure about it either, because culture obviously matters. But a lot of the differences that you see from individual to individual are at least 60% biologically determined. So if you're born, let's say as a male with a female temperament, so you're high in agreeableness, empathy, and you're low in negative emotion, because that's the typical female pattern, about 60% of the variance in that is attributable to genetic factors. So we could have a sex and
Starting point is 00:30:02 gender split as the political types insist that's also grounded in biological differences without making a categorical distinction. You know, say, yeah, so absolutely. They always remain connected. The reason that we have genders and to have a duality of gender is because we have sexes. So people who say that gender is purely in our heads, is purely a society product, that's an impossibility. I think if we were a cloning species, so we had no sexual reproduction, where we would all be identical, no one would have come up
Starting point is 00:30:42 with the concept of gender. So the concept of gender. So the concept of gender is related to sex. There is a certain independence between the, between the nebate also remain joined at some point. And you're right. It is never purely cultural. Well, and you show that, you show that in your book because, and I want to go into it in quite a bit of detail. So for example, let's start with this, toy preference and differences in nurturing behavior. And so I'd like you to lay that out and then we can talk about that psychologically for a bit. Yes, it's interesting. If you look at the behavior
Starting point is 00:31:18 of the young primates and children, human children, you see a lot of similarity there. And often people, of course, think that the toy preferences are socialized, that we see, but I think there's too much similarity with what we see in other primates. So young female primates, they are very fond of infants.
Starting point is 00:31:41 They wanna hold infants as soon as there's a newborn baby and the mother arrives with it. They wanna, fond of infants. They want to hold infants as soon as there's a newborn baby and the mother arrives with it. They want to, they surround her. There's very few males who are interested in that, but the young females want to get their hands on the, on the, on the infant. And in, in nature, we also know that they pick up logs and rocks and chimpanzees do and, and the young females, they carry them around on their back on their belly
Starting point is 00:32:05 as if they have a doll. If in captivity you give them a doll like a teddy bear, they will walk around with it and take care of it and be friendly with it. Whereas young males they have a tendency to take them apart basically, yeah, look what's inside. And so the females have this very strong urge to hold infants. And the same thing has been found in human children that girls are more interested in infants than boys are. And if you look at the young males, the young males have a higher energy level. This is also true for boys versus girls. They have a higher energy level. And what they love to do is rough housing, mock fighting. That's what they do the whole day, basically. They have a higher energy level and what they love to do is rough housing, mock fighting. That's what they do the whole day basically. They like to mock fight with each other. Sometimes we're adolescent males to just to test their
Starting point is 00:32:54 strengths on them. And so a rough and tumble play also in human children, much more typical of boys and of girls. And the segregation that we see in the playground, between boys playing together and girls playing together, probably comes to a large degree from the fact that the girls don't like all these rough housing that the boys are doing. That's not their type of play, and so they stay away from them because it's too rough for them. So I spent a lot of time looking at the play literature in children. So partly, I worked with a team of investigators at Montreal who were interested in the origin of anti-social behavior. And this is partly what gave me a foray into your work, by the way. So the lead
Starting point is 00:33:40 researcher there was Richard Tromblay, who's done a lot of great work on. I know his work. Yeah, you know his work. Yeah, Tromblay is great, man. And he was on this podcast, by the way, and I worked with him for 15 years, something like that. Didn't he work with Fred Strayer, who was also there. Yeah, yeah. So, so he was interested in the origins of anti-social behavior and criminality, mostly. And while we worked together, we kept pushing back the origin time in some sense of anti-social behavior until the age of two. And he had put a bunch of interventions in place to try to modulate anti-social behavior, but it's very, very persistent once it's established, very difficult to to ameliorate. And so there's a subset of two-year-olds, about 5% of them of the males,
Starting point is 00:34:29 almost all male, who bite, hit, kick, and steal at the age of two. Now most two-year-olds don't, but a percentage do. And most of them are socialized by the age of four. And so that becomes either probably integrated into their personality rather than repressed. So maybe they're more disagreeable from a personality perspective, highly probable. They're more competitive, they're blunter, but they get socialized. And one of the key questions is, you know, who socializes them? And my suspicions are, in many cases, it's a father or a father equivalent. In any case, the ones who don't get socialized, they aren't popular with their peers. And humans get socialized primarily by their peers after age four. So they get to be little outcasts
Starting point is 00:35:19 and then there are bullies and then there's juvenile delinquents and then there are criminals. That's where the life course persistent criminals, that's the population from which they're derived. And so then we looked at what was socializing them. And the answer to that, which I thought was so cool, and so great, was play. Both pretend play and rough and humble play. And except it's done lovely work on rats with rough and humble play. Yeah, that's a point that I make in my book is that the play behavior of the boys, enough of young male primates, it's partly developing fighting skills. So it's partly just like in
Starting point is 00:35:57 the females, their interest in infants is a preparation for adult life when they will have offspring. And for the males, the play fighting is in the preparation for adult life in when they will have offspring and for the males the play fighting is in the preparation for adult life in which they will have a lot of competition going. But another aspect very important is that they learn how to control their strengths, they learn how to be nice to others, they learn to release pressure once the other cannot stand it. So if you look at an adult male gorilla, for example, this is big fist on a baby gorilla, he could just, there's a little pressure, he could kill it, because he's incredibly strong. But you know, adult male gorillas, they play with infants and they do very well with them. And that is because he has learned over a lifetime to control his physical strengths.
Starting point is 00:36:43 Right. And that's, that rough and tumble play is so important physical strengths. Yes. Right. And that rough and tumble play is so important for that. Yeah. It's embodied, right? So one of the things, because I really got interested in rough and tumble play as a primary source of socialization of aggression, is that, and also more than that, right? Because it also underlies the spirit of reciprocity, I think, which is manifested in pure, in its pure sense, in the spirit of reciprocity, I think which is manifest in its pure sense in the spirit of play,
Starting point is 00:37:05 because play is something that the participants have to engage in voluntarily. Yeah, and they have to enjoy it. So this also relates very much to the gender relationship, is that because we have a lot of trouble, of course, with men abusing women, men need to learn about their strengths, need to learn how to control it, need to learn when to hold back and when to use it. And play is an important factor in that. And so when I now hear, I hear sometimes from people who have children that at schools there is less recess time and there is less opportunity for physical contact because the teacher said
Starting point is 00:37:46 that she shouldn't be touching each other. But they forbid it. They forbid it entirely. Any like my, at my school, my kids school, the boys and the girls were forbidden to even pick up snow on the off chance that they would make a snowball. And so they just stopped the rough and tumble play
Starting point is 00:38:02 of the males, 100%. And Panks have showed if you deprive male routes of play, juveniles, they would play hyperactively when you gave them the chance and that you could use methylphenidate to suppress that. Oh, yeah, yeah, yeah. That's a rough thing. But the Ruff and Tumble play is absolutely essential. It's not just for developing fighting skills, which is part of it, But the rough and tumble play is absolutely essential.
Starting point is 00:38:25 It's not just for developing fighting skills, which is part of it, but it's also how males control themselves, how they, because they become physically stronger than females, they need to have full control over their body. Right, and they, well, I think they also learn, I think through rough and tumble play, kids also learn what hurts,
Starting point is 00:38:44 and what is, what hurts them, really. And so, because when you rough and tumble play. Kids also learn what hurts and what is what hurts them really. And so because when you rough and tumble I rough and tumble played with my kids a lot. I set up kind of a little wrestling ring made out of two couches, pushed together. And one of the and they love it, right? They're so excited by it. But you tend to push the kids to their physical limits, right? Because you twist their arms and you sort of toss them in the air and you and what you're engaged in this dance that's an exploration of physical limitation. And so I think one of the things that play does and and Panks' episode, there was a student of his Tiffany Fields who massaged premature infants in in the incubator and showed that she could rapidly radically facilitate their development. I think play also regulates emotion because it's soothing and it shows you. It allows you to make
Starting point is 00:39:31 a clear distinction between real threats and false threats and between physical situations that actually hurt and those that don't. And that has to be done in an embodied sense. So the funny thing that happened to me is that when I was a student, I worked with two young chimpanzees who loved to play with me. So they were only five years old, but they were already much stronger than I was. You know, it develops much physically, much stronger
Starting point is 00:40:00 than even though they're smaller, that's physically stronger than I. And so I would often trouble play with them and I was, you know, I would always lose because they have four hands and they can put you in a knot in a way that is impossible for me to get out of. And each time I would protest, I would say, oh, oh, and let them know that this was, they were going too far with me. They would be very worried.
Starting point is 00:40:26 They would come around and they would look at my face and they would be looking very worried and a sort of like surprise that this big fellow was so weak in their opinion, you know. And then they would slow down the play and they would be very gentle for 10 minutes or so. They would be very gentle with me. And calibrate it.
Starting point is 00:40:46 Yeah, and that's how play goes. Also in dogs, play is a, is constantly measuring what is painful, what is not painful. And of course, the goal is to have fun. That's the main goal. So that calibration is going on the whole time. Right, right, right. Well, and that's, well, so you know, P.A.J., who wrote a great jump, P.A.J. wrote an absolutely
Starting point is 00:41:10 stellar book on the development of morality out of the spirit of play. It's a great book. And, you know, he showed, so he also, and this is germane to your work. He made a technical case that any organization that's based on the spirit of voluntary reciprocity will over a reasonable period of time outcompete any organization based on the spirit of compulsion. Because if you use force and compulsion, you have to waste energy and resources on the compulsion. Plus plus you don't optimally motivate. And so that's another pointer to an intrinsic ethic of reciprocal cooperation. And I think that best, so one of the things I've been thinking about lately is the best
Starting point is 00:41:53 rejoinder to the sort of postmodern and neo-Marxist insistence that its power and dominance above all is that no, it's not. Is that an aberration from the spirit of play because if it if a society and any social interaction is optimally structured then it moves out of it moves into the domain of voluntary play is mutually enjoyable stays on the side of positive emotion and it also involves this continual mutual collaboration which which children start to seriously negotiate between the ages of two and four. Yeah, and so what you mentioned is that socialization is mostly done by children by the peers, you know, that's a very interesting thought, because in my discussion of gender, I noticed that
Starting point is 00:42:41 many people saying that socialization is a one-way street. The parents socialize their children. And I think parents overestimate their influence on children. I think children socialize themselves to a large degree. I call it self-socialization. They look for adult models. It can be the mother, can be the father, can be somebody else. They look for adult models, can also be imaginary models that they see on TV basically. For that purpose of fiction, as far as I can tell, fiction puts up ritual models for emulation.
Starting point is 00:43:15 And so the children socialize themselves, and then in addition, they have all the peer influences on them. And so I think parents totally overestimate their effect on this. Yeah, our research at Montreal basically indicated that the proper, so imagine that the the averaged parent, which would be sort of the consequence of a monogamous relationship, right? So two half insane people unite and produce one moderately sane person. And then that sane person is a analog of the general social environment. And then the purpose of that sane person on the socialization front is to help the child manage his or her behavior so that they become optimally
Starting point is 00:43:58 socially acceptable by the age of four when their peers take over. And so that's the, yeah, that's how it seems to be. And then, you know, you talked about this self-socialization. You say, well, how can kids self-socialize? And part of the answer to that is, if you don't socialize your children in a manner they find enjoyable, let's say, in the spirit of play. I know some discipline is necessary, but in the spirit of play, the children will vociferously and continually object, right? They'll cry.
Starting point is 00:44:28 They'll be uncooperative. They certainly won't be enthusiastic. And so there is a dynamic dance there right from the beginning that the child and I think this is also why, by the way, you know, there's all this study showing that there's a fair genetic influence on temperament and there's a pretty decent influence of non-shared environment. But there's hardly any influence of shared environment. And I think the reason for that isn't so much that parents are irrelevant, but that good parents particularize the environment so much in response to their child's temperament
Starting point is 00:45:01 that there isn't a lot of overlap. Yeah. So in the other primates, we see the same thing. We see that young females, they imitate a mother more than young males. Young males, so for example, a recent study on orangutans in the first, found that daughters eat exactly the same foods as the moms. Sons, they have a more variety in their diet. There's hundreds and hundreds of plants around and fruits around. They learn their diet from others. And so the sons, they learn it mostly from males and other individuals that they see around. There's other studies on tool use, for example, that daughters copy exactly the tool technology that
Starting point is 00:45:46 Hermann uses. Sons, they have, they're on their own with that and they develop their own tool technologies. And so in the primates we see, because they, you know, a chimpanzee built when he's 16, so there's a very long-learning period. We see that they self-socialize by emulating individuals of their sex. And that means also that the concept of gender is applicable to them, because they are also culturally influenced, they're influenced by the behavior of adults around them. And that's why I use the word gender also for the other primates. Okay, so I want to read something to you from your book and then I want to discuss that for a second
Starting point is 00:46:26 This is from Deborah Bloom you you you cite her My son Marcus passionately covet toy weaponry Denied even so much as one lousy plastic pistol by his gun in tolerant mother He is compensated by building armaments out of everything from clay to kitchen utensils. I watched him charge after the cat, shouting, shooting with a toothbrush, and I found myself mentally throwing up my hands. Now, to me, that's kind of amusing, but I didn't really find it amusing.
Starting point is 00:47:00 I actually found it really dark because the line of work that you're pursuing, which shows quite clearly, as does the personality literature on these topics, that these toy preferences and behavioral differences are deeply grounded in biology. And they may also be fostered culturally, but fundamentally it's the manifestation of an intrinsic mode of being, an instinctual pattern. And this woman, this author says, you know, he's she's throwing up her hands because she can't stop her son from being a boy. I don't find that a music. I find that actually quite horrifying.
Starting point is 00:47:37 And, you know, the idea that has to do with the desire of many parents to influence their children and to make them sort of gender neutral and to have boys play with dolls and girls play with tricks. And of course, there are a few who will do that, but they're trying to go against whatever the tendencies are. You are. And they punish those tendencies at least implicitly. And now, you know, if male ambition serves power and oppression, then you can understand why early
Starting point is 00:48:11 manifestations of that might be worth punishing, especially if that was culturally a cultural consequence. But if the fundamental male drive is something like reciprocity and competence and say, aim, because I think that's relevant to the use of weapons, then the punishing of that, the viewing it as only serving power and oppression instead of competence and reciprocity, that's an awful thing. And I think, I'd like to know what you think about this, but you know, boys are not doing that well in school. And they're all the way through kindergarten through university. They're dropping out, and they're also increasingly and interested, less interested in sexual
Starting point is 00:48:51 activity of all sorts, even masturbation. And I think that it's a consequence of this attitude, it's like they're punished for a male typical pattern of behavior that's improperly associated with dominance right from day one. So yeah, boy boys are more often kicked out of school That's a that's a big problem and that has probably to do with the tendency of rough housing and and mox fighting which they do all the time and yeah The way I look at that is that that's a primate pattern. All the male primates like to do that, mock fighting. And we at our schools in the west, we have decided
Starting point is 00:49:32 to stop that and to to say you shouldn't be touching. Yeah, you know, I worked with this guy named Dan Always, and he wrote a great book called bullying, what it is and what we can do about a very straightforward guy, kind of like a 50s engineer. And he reduced the bullying incidents and associated alcoholism and criminality in Scandinavia, a whole, in a number of countries by 50%. And he went into schools, he was really careful because he defined bullying very particularly. He said, he didn't associate it with rough and tumble play, for example, because he actually
Starting point is 00:50:06 had a clue. He said, you mentioned earlier that the reciprocal male, Elfa, whose competent breaks up unfair fights. Okay, so that's what all of you just did. He went into schools and he said, look, you can intervene in an incident of aggression when there's a clear and inequitable power balance. So if a 12-year-old's beating up an eight-year-old or if two 10-year-olds are picking on a 10-year-old
Starting point is 00:50:32 and then he taught the kids, you can go tattle to a teacher, which is otherwise forbidden, as all children know, and most adults should remember, it's your ethical responsibility to inform a person in authority if you see violence with power imbalance. And then he taught the teachers and the parents the same thing. It's very narrow, narrow focus on a very specific form of aggressive behavior that wasn't play. And no one's petty in the attention to it in North America, like even though it's a stunning body of research.
Starting point is 00:51:04 And the only body of research I know that shows a positive effect of the attempts to socially ameliorate anti-social behavior. But instead we get this over-reaction that includes all elements of play, rough and tumble play in particular, and it's also why boys are medicated. If Banksep is right, they manifest this boisterous, rough and tumble play desire, and it's disruptive in classrooms, and the best quick trip out of that is an amphetamine, which suppresses play behavior. That's what it does technically and biochemically. Yeah, it makes me think of, I did a lot of studies of reconciliation behavior. So what happens after a fight? Who reconciles with whom and so on?
Starting point is 00:51:51 And I actually, males are quite good. Males are good at fighting, but also with a reconciling after fights. And in those studies of reconciliation behavior, there were some developmental psychologists who started looking at children. We did the studies on the primates who kiss and embrace after a fight or sometimes have sex after a fight, things like that. But they did studies in human cultures. What they found is that Japanese children reconcile a lot more than Western children after fights and are much better at conflict resolution. And the reason they gave, this is not my study, but the reason they gave is that in Japan, the teachers don't break up fights. Well, of course, accepted killing it sort of, but they usually don't break up fights. They let the kids fight. And if they think it needs to be interrupted or calm down, they sent another child, an older child, they sent an other child to mediate
Starting point is 00:52:47 between them. So that's so smart because otherwise too, you know, the kid that's getting beat up is now vilified because a teacher had to intervene on his behalf. So now he was a bully victim to begin with, let's say, and now he can't even stand up for himself and a teacher has to come to his aid. It's a terrible status defeat for him. So in the primates, of course, there is a lot of reconciliation.
Starting point is 00:53:12 There's also mediation in reconciliation. So all the females who bring parties together sometimes. But they all learn in their lifetime how to fight and how to reconcile after fights. And I'm from a family of six boys. I may maybe my interest in conflict resolution was partly because as a boy in a boy family, you learn how to do these things. Yeah, well, you know, okay.
Starting point is 00:53:35 So one of the things that I call just like Jonathan Height has commented on is this culture of fragility that we've produced and emotional overreaction. And I able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able to be able So, imagine the typical human family wouldn't have had one child, would have had multiple, so they'd have to be strategizing and jockeying for attention in a way that didn't cause undue tyranny and aggression. So, first of all, we have lots of single child families or two child families, but single often, and then also to much older parents. And so, if the children are actually being socialized in large part, let's say by their siblings
Starting point is 00:54:28 in a more naturalistic human community, part of the reason for this emergent fragility could well be that they haven't had to engage and say that Japanese style of conflict that isn't mediated by parents. And they haven't learned to reconcile. I want to point out to the viewers and listeners too that Dr. DeWol's work on reconciliation is also of groundbreaking importance as far as I'm concerned,
Starting point is 00:54:51 because he put the other, what the other shoe dropped with the work on reconciliation, because you can think, well, conflict is a terrible thing, aggression is a terrible thing, and the aggressive males are power-hungry and demonic and dominant. But you showed very clearly that the aggressive males are power hungry and demonic and dominant. But you showed very clearly that the sophisticated males are experts at reconciliation, and sometimes that expertise even exceeds that of the females. And all of that needs to be learned. So you need to provide learning opportunities. And if it's not in a family, if the family has just one or two children, then you need to bring children together somehow in your neighborhood or in a school,
Starting point is 00:55:28 or you need to give them the opportunity to learn these things. And I think we are very good at removing these opportunities. And that's partly, or forbidding them even. Yeah, that's partly a fear of competition. So for example, I've read in the psychology literature that they say that women have very good friendships, wonderful friendships, deep friendships and so on, but men are too competitive, men are hierarchical and competitive. That's hard to rubbish.
Starting point is 00:55:58 Yeah, and that's what they say, even though many men have friends and enjoy the company of other men. But I think they're confusing competition between men, this friendship. It's like you can only own a moment. Yeah, they don't know how to integrate, I mean, male friendships that are optimized are competitive in the extreme often, often on the basis of humor. Yeah. in the extreme often, often on the basis of humor. Yeah, so they confuse, they think it's either competitive or friends and the combination of the two they cannot see. And I think for man for sure, that combination exists.
Starting point is 00:56:39 Man can be rivals and competitors, and but half an hour later they're laughing, Mrs. Sattal, let me tell you a funny story. There was once a swimming trainer in the Netherlands who trained girls for swimming competition. And she switched to boys. And then I saw this interview with her on the newspaper, why did you switch to boys?
Starting point is 00:57:01 And she said, well, if the girls on the team have a fight that starts in the early season, they will have that fight going for the whole season. It will not stop. If the boys have a fight, they drink in the evening, they drink a beer, and the next day, they don't even remember the fight anymore. She says. Or they have a fight. And then they reconcile, and the fight is over. So okay. So I want to use that as a segue into something else you wrote so we can talk about it. So I want to read something again that from different from the introduction. And I don't know if you agreed with this or if you
Starting point is 00:57:38 were just if you were saying it as a summary of someone else's thought, but you said, it's easy to see why male and female patterns of aggression are valued so differently. Only the first creates trouble in societies. And so, let me riff on that for a second and turn it over to you. So, I think this observation you made about the swim teams is extremely interesting, because we studied male pattern aggressive behavior, which tends to involve physical violence, and female pattern aggressive behavior in humans. And what the females do is gossip back bite and destroy reputations.
Starting point is 00:58:15 And the female anti-social types are really, really good at that, and they're vicious. And the thing that differentiates those right now, so in such a cardinal manner, in my my estimation is that female aggression scales on the internet and male aggression does not. And so, you know, we know that the online world is vicious in a very particular way, and I think a big part of it is that it actually can't be settled. So part of the reason that women have difficulty in conflict is because men can resort to fighting. And that will produce an alteration in the, it'll produce a cessation of the conflict in a very real sense, one way or another, right? But when women are embroiled in a conflict, well, they can't fight physically. And, and so they do get caught in this conundrum.
Starting point is 00:59:08 So one of the problems that our society really wrestles with at the moment is that it's very difficult to control untrammeled female pattern aggression. And so I think it is causing a tremendous amount of trouble. And that reputation damaging, that a huge, you just see that constantly on social media platforms. And so I don't know if that was your thought or if it was a thought that you were summarizing, but I'm kind of curious about your thoughts about that. Yeah, so I make a distinction in my book between two ways of keeping the piece. One is peace making, the other one is peace keeping.
Starting point is 00:59:50 The males in chimpanzees, the males are very good at peacemaking. They have a fight and then they get together, they kiss and embrace and then they groom each other and they cycle through this all the time. It's a very easy process for them. And they have more fights than females. And I think what the females do, the females reconcile less when they have a fight, less often than the males, because they have more trouble with that, I think.
Starting point is 01:00:18 And what the females do is stay away from your rivals. Stay away from fights. Stay away from individuals that you're likely to, whatever I call that peacekeeping, is that they suppress aggression and they stay away from those. All right, yeah. So let's look at it in the human case.
Starting point is 01:00:33 So one of the biological temperamental differences that you see between men and women is that women are higher in negative emotion that starts in puberty, as boys and girls are the same and it doesn't change throughout the life course and women are more agreeable. And so that combination, so agreeable people are conflict avoidant. Now agreeableness is empathy and that's supposed to be a cardinal virtue but the downside of
Starting point is 01:01:00 agreeableness and empathy is conflict avoidance. And so, and if you add conflict avoidance is emotionally taxing. So I think it's also practically a problem because it means that if someone pushes on you that you have to negotiate with, you have to avoid or you won't push back. And what the hell are you going to do on the negotiating front? If that's the case, say at your workplace, you can't avoid your boss. But the interesting thing is that both strategies are actually quite successful. We did a study of human behavior in the hospitals. We looked at operating rooms and we looked at how well men and women work together.
Starting point is 01:01:37 And female cooperation is really highly developed. So female teams, not all female teams, but females on a team, works very well. So I think both sexes have a good strategy. One is to suppress conflict. The other one is to reconcile very easily. So they're both good strategies and they're both quite effective, but they're totally different. So let me push you on that just for a second. So let's take an optimal female cooperating group. See, the conundrum I think they run into, and I think this is the conundrum of compassion in general, is that that's just fine until you throw a predator in.
Starting point is 01:02:17 And so a group like that that's actually made out of pure cooperators can work just fine. But if you throw in a woman who has anti-social personality, which would be manifested, say, as borderline personality disorder, which looks like the clinical equivalent of ASP and females, or if you throw in a real predator, the co-op, like, what are, if the cooperators can't avoid and they can't oppose, all they have left is to fall victim to the predator. And that's why I think that men, women are in this weird evolutionary conundrum because they have to find men who are disagreeable enough to stave off the real predators
Starting point is 01:02:54 but agreeable enough to be generous with their productive competence right and so it's this knife edge you, because a little too far in either direction is not good. So is there a flaw in my reasoning there with regards to the Achilles heel, let's say, of female cooperative groups? No, I think it is true that we have always underestimated female competitiveness. So, you know, in the psychological literature, males were always called hierarchical and competitive and not females. And now we know that, there's plenty of female competition, and there's plenty of female hierarchies. Actually, the word pecking order comes from hence, not from roosters. And in the animal kingdom, lots of females have hierarchies, have alpha females and so on. So I think it's important to point out all, is that female competition is maybe less physical,
Starting point is 01:03:55 but it's not absent. Well, I probably also not competition about exactly the same things. In your work on animal cognition, you use this term, umfelt, which is like the implicit motivational environment in some sense that an animal might inhabit. And I'd like to talk about that in a bit. But men and women don't compete within their own sexes for status in the same way.
Starting point is 01:04:22 And so it's easy for men to look at women and think, well, they're not that competitive. It's well, they're not engaged in rough and tumble player or violent confrontation. But that doesn't mean that the competition isn't there. It's just that, and it also may be that men aren't subtle enough in some sense to see it. Now, let me tell you a funny story on that.
Starting point is 01:04:41 This was a scientist, a woman scientist in Finland who studied children at play in the playground. And she said, if you watch these children at the end of the day, you say that the boys had five times as many or six times as many fights as the girls. And that's just from watching them. If she asked the kids at the end of the day, did you have a fight today? The boys and the girls had equal numbers, which means that the girl fights are just not visible She and as he said if a girl walks up to another girl and that girl turns around the walks away They consider that a fight right well, but that that's very interesting, but of course boys will boys will not consider that a fight
Starting point is 01:05:23 It more needs to happen. Yeah, no the boys will not consider that a fight. More needs to happen. Yeah. No, the boys are not, the boys are not even as stooped enough in some sense to notice that. The thing that the thing an anti-social girl will do is like a non-familiar girl or even a familiar girl will approach a playgroup that's already in formation, say. And the alpha female of the playgroup will say, we don't have to play with you. And then, and that's devastating, right? Because it's a real like cardinal inclusion, but there's no physical aggression associated with it.
Starting point is 01:05:56 And the girl who's rejected will walk away crushed because she's been put down as unacceptable socially. And that all can happen, especially if the girl bully is sophisticated, because then an adult can come in there and call her on it and she'll say, oh well, she's just over-sensitive. I didn't really mean that at all. And with a perfect facade of angelic innocence as well,
Starting point is 01:06:21 which the anti-social female types are, sure. These differences are so interesting. These sex differences that we see in human behavior, but they're not well-documented. So recently, I received a handbook on, I think it's developmental psychology or something like that. And I looked for gender in there because I was writing on gender. And there was almost nothing on gender.
Starting point is 01:06:43 So the play differences between boys and girls were not mentioned, but also the conflict differences that you now mentioned were not mentioned. It's as if the psychologists have decided that that's a two two sensitive as a topic to get into the sex differences of I think part of it is that they actually don't like the facts. So for example, when I've been looking at personality differences between men and women, and we did some of the cardinal work on that with the big five aspect scale. So the truth of the matter is, is that temperamental differences between men and women as so gender differences, as measured by personality scales,
Starting point is 01:07:23 which is the right way to measure it, they maximize in Scandinavia. So the more egalitarian the country, the bigger the gender differences, which is exactly the opposite of what the social constructionists would have predicted. Now some gender differences decrease, so men and women are more likely to be in the workplace
Starting point is 01:07:41 now than 40 years ago. But if you make any egalitarian society, you actually maximize gender differences, which is no one expected that, and it just flies in the face of this sort of Margaret Mead, early Margaret Mead, like social constructionism. And then you add to that a couple, so that accounts that plus the difference in interest, which is relevant to your work. So girls are reliably more interested in people, and boys reliably more interested in things.
Starting point is 01:08:08 So that's, say, dolls and cars or toy cars. And that also maximizes in the Scandinavian countries. It's one standard deviation, the difference. It's the biggest personality difference between men and women that we know. And it accounts for a huge amount of the variance in occupational choice, like between, say, engineering and nursing, which would be the cardinal examples. And so the psychologists look at that and they think, oh my god, that isn't how we want it to be. And so they don't talk about your work. And they don't talk
Starting point is 01:08:38 about toy preference differences. And they don't talk about temperamental differences. It's all like shunted under the carpet. So you mentioned Margaret Mead, who is often mentioned in the context of cultural construction of gender differences. But I reread her book, Her Book, Mail and Female. And she has a whole section in there about what she calls universal differences between men and women. So she, in a later edition of her book,
Starting point is 01:09:07 she said, if she had to write it today, this was in the 1960s, her book was, I think, published in the 50s. She said, if I would write it today, I would bring in more biology, the biology of the differences between men and women. And so one of the differences that she mentions is that
Starting point is 01:09:27 men always want to accomplish something. They want to be better at something than other men or women. They feel that's their goal in life, so to speak. And she mentioned that as a universal saying in man, she mentions also, of course, the childbearing capacities of women and the interest in children of women and the interest of women in the well-being of others and of themselves and their family. And so she mentions in that chapter quite a few things that I think are universal sect differences. Well, so she was more open-minded, I would say, than her followers in that regard.
Starting point is 01:10:07 Well, so this desire to be better at something. So let's take that apart a bit in light of our discussion. If you're cynical about that, you can say, well, that desire to be better at something is nothing but a manifestation of that power and dominance drive. And so it's to be competitive, urges to be discouraged because it's zero-sum game, and one person always gains at the expense of another. But if you look at the, even the animal literature in a sophisticated way, you think, wait a minute, competence is associated with hierarchical status. It's very sophisticated. It's a competition for competence, even among animals, and even more so among human beings. And so that ambition that young boys manifest is, can easily be manifested in the service
Starting point is 01:10:52 of social goods. And furthermore, and this is the thing that's so awful about it for me, is that that's actually the primary thing that male humans have to offer females, which is, well, look, of course, I want to be better at something because I want to be differentiated economically and to be paid for something, you have to be better at it than others. Otherwise, no one will pay you. They'll pay someone else. And so, what I see happening to boys in our culture, and I've seen this with thousands of people, is that their ambition is being quelled, because it's associated unthinkingly with power and that undermines their entire commitment to the social and the sexual and interpersonal
Starting point is 01:11:30 enterprise. Yeah, I think there is a bit of suppression of those tendencies. Yeah. So, okay, so maybe we could move to, if you don't mind, to the animal cognitive front, you talked about cognitive etiology. And I'd like to get your thoughts on that too. You wrote a book whose title, I don't remember precisely, but it's something like, are we smart enough to know how smart animals are? It's exactly the same. Oh, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no,
Starting point is 01:12:07 no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, no, example of gibbons, for example. And the fact that octopi and elephants and primates can be intelligent in a particular way, because they have manipulable appendages, unlike say, don't have. So let me give an example, how we mismeasured the animals. So elephants, there was the question,
Starting point is 01:12:41 do elephants use tools? So what people did is they take an elephant at a zoo, and they put some food outside of the question, do elephants use tools? So what people did is they take an elephant at the zoo, they put some food outside of the cage, they give the elephant sticks, and the elephant doesn't do anything with the sticks. If you do the same experiment with a chimpanzee, of course he takes the stick and reaches the food and brings it close to him and then eats the food.
Starting point is 01:13:01 So the temple used the stick to get the food. The elephant didn't do that. And the conclusion of that study was that elephants are not tool users. Then a couple of years later, another research team, they did a smart thing. They hung some food very high so that the elephant couldn't reach it. And they put in his enclosure a bunch of boxes, wooden boxes. And what the elephant did is he would grab a box, bring it close to under the foot, put it under the foot, and then stand on top of the box and reach the food. So he was using a tool clearly. The conclusion of that research team was that an elephant, the trunk, we think it's like a hand, but the trunk is also a nose, of course.
Starting point is 01:13:46 And if the trunk reaches for food, it needs to close itself, when it grabs the food, and the elephant was not ready to close his nose while he was grabbing food. But if he could stand on boxes, that would work for him. And so we need to think in terms of the animal and its physical features and
Starting point is 01:14:07 that trunk is not an arm, it's not the same saying. We need to start thinking like the animals and then we can solve a lot of these problems of their intelligence. Our appendages in some sense are primary tools. And so if you don't give an animal a problem that it can solve with its primary set of tools, it's obviously not going to be able to solve it. And so that also brings us into the issue of embodied perception, because I read a great book called Ecological Approach to Visual Perception, which Jay-Jay Gibson, which is, man, that's a classic book, it's something brilliant. And which is man, that's a classic book, it's something brilliant. And, you know, his premise, fundamentally, is that we see with our hands in some real sense, is that when we look at the world, we basically construe it as a set of gripable objects. Now,
Starting point is 01:14:58 it's more complex because we see walkable surfaces and climbable, you know, what would you call, climbable slopes and so forth. So it's not just hands, but that our eyes function to map our hands onto the world. Yeah, I think it's brilliant book. And that's why we understand a chimpanzee much better than an elephant. Or a dolphin. Yeah, a chimpanzee has hands and has binocular vision. So we are completely in tune with the chimpancy and not in tune with elephant or giraffe or whatever it is. Right. And that understanding is a consequence, not so much of mutual misunderstanding. So to speak, it's much more profound than that.
Starting point is 01:15:39 It's because our cognitive and perceptual architectures actually scaffolded off our morphology. If the morphology is different enough, we wouldn't be able to understand the animal even if we could communicate because none of the presuppositions would be the same. I think E.O. Wilson, I wasn't Wilson who said, if we could speak with a lion, we'd have nothing to say to him, something like that, or he might have said it about ants. I think of us Wittgenstein. I know a number of people made similar comments, but you also detail out the developing understanding of the Gibbon Hand and species-specific face identification.
Starting point is 01:16:19 Yeah, so the Gibbon Hand is interesting because the Gibbon has almost no summer. So the given hand is interesting because the given has almost no sum. So it's more like a hook hangs by the hand. And so when they trusted tool use on the given, the given's were not using tools at all. They never did anything until a smart investigator lived to tools up a little because the given cannot pick up something from the ground. And since the given does live on the ground, it lives up in the trees. It doesn't matter for the given that he cannot do that. But in the experimental situations, they couldn't pick up the tools. So yeah, you always have to test animals and that becomes ever more complex. For example, the elephant has a hundred times better
Starting point is 01:17:01 smell, all-faction than the dog. The dog probably, and the dog probably, a hundred times better than us. So you can imagine the elephant, you need to test them probably on all-faction. So recently, just Platnik, a former student of mine, he did an experiment with elephants in Thailand, where he would give them two buckets. In one bucket, he would beat sunflower seeds and the other one was empty. And
Starting point is 01:17:26 the elephant of course, with his trunk would, he could not look into it, but with the trunk would smell and would pick the bucket with the sunflower seeds. Then he started to vary the quantities and he found that elephants can distinguish a bucket with 120 seats from a bucket with a hundred seats So they can count with their nose so to speak they have they're so sensitive no animal can do that I think but the elephant can do that So they can estimate quantity Well fact orally. Yeah, yeah, yeah, wow Wow, and has it has the limits of that discrimination being tested? Yeah, well, I think if you look at that paper that he hasn't been testing by making the difference a smaller and smaller. Yeah
Starting point is 01:18:10 Hmm. Oh, that's really something. Well, yeah, we have no idea how an animal most animals brains are Arrange to round old faction not vision. We're really weird in that regard. It's like us in birds of prey You know, and maybe some close primate relatives. That's it. The rest of the animal kingdom is almost all smell. And we can hardly even imagine what a world like that is like. So their brain business is interesting, because at some point, five years, six years ago, people said, we should not look at brain size, not even a brain size relative to the body. We should just count a number of neurons. And that will give us an index of the intelligence of an animal.
Starting point is 01:18:49 And of course, everyone must convince that at that time that humans have the most neurons. We have 85 billion neurons or something like that. Until a couple of years later, it was found that the elephant has three times more neurons than the human. And then that theory was abandoned because humans need to stay on top. Yeah. Well, you know, we radically underestimate the relationship between intelligence and morphology. You know, I thought about this in particular with regards to dolphins who have quite a remarkably
Starting point is 01:19:21 large brain and a large brain in relation to their body size. And they, well, dolphins are intelligent. It's like, well, it's possible that they're intelligent. In some way, we don't really understand, but they don't have hands. And so they can't build anything. And I would, our prefrontal cortex emerged in large part out of the motor cortex across the course of evolution. And it looks like what we do with our prefrontal cortex is primarily map out action and manipulation before implementing it. And so again, we think of the world as a place we can grip and move. And when we think about problem solving cognitively, we're really looking at at elaborations
Starting point is 01:20:00 of what we do with our hands. So it's not obvious at all what dolphins do if they think because what can they do with it? That's even reflected in our language. Can you grasp the problem? Exactly. Exactly that. Can you get a grip on it? Can you handle that? That's all questions about competence. So, if you look at the animal kingdom, we judge animals by our standards. We are good at language and we're good at two news, and that's our standards. But if you look at, for example, echolocation by bats, it's a very complex cognitive skill. Ask any engineer who designs radar system for airplanes, how complex that is. You
Starting point is 01:20:47 are moving object and you need to calculate and recalculate and so on. So echolocation is a very complex skill and you can dark this room, make it completely dark and release an insect and the bat can catch the insect. It's just incredible that they can do that. But we're not impressed by it. We're not impressed by the bat because we don't do these things. We're not echolocators. And so bat intelligence is not on our radar. So to speak, we always go for language until use. Well, in Dolphin's echolocate too. So that could be a huge part of what they're called in the process is devoted towards. And not only know what they're doing linguistically.
Starting point is 01:21:27 So let's talk about one of the things I liked about the book on animal cognition was because I thought this for a long time especially in relationship to rat studies. So there's this idea that was drummed into my head as an undergraduate is that you shouldn't answer purmorphized animals. And then as I got to be a more sophisticated viewer of the animal literature, I thought, no, no, no, that's exactly backwards. What you should do is presume continuity of all function unless there's compelling reason to not. And so that may be interested in the issue of animal consciousness because conscious and so you touch on consciousness in that book and so when when first so I'd like to ask you why do you make of consciousness like how would you define it and how far down the animal chain I'll tell you story about a wasps because I've watched wasps and those bloody things, man, those things are sophisticated.
Starting point is 01:22:27 Let me say one thing about antipromorphism. Okay. Okay. We need to respect homologies. Homologies are similarities between species that come from a common ancestor. So my hand is homologous with the hand of a chimpanzee. And so, but the chimpanzee has, we don't call it a paw or a claw or never call it a hand,
Starting point is 01:22:52 because we use the same terminology for homologous traits. The same is true for behavior. If I laugh, my laughing expression is very similar to that of a chimpanzee who has been tickled, we need to both call it laugh. When I was younger, people would say, why don't you call it vocalized painting? They wanted us to get away from anthropomorphism, but I think anthropomorphism is useful, especially for species that have homologous traits.
Starting point is 01:23:18 Yeah, right. Well, that's where it should be the assumption rather than the exception. And Pankseps showed that rats laugh too when you tickle them. They just grow elseersonically. So that homology is extremely deep. Okay, so this was, it was buzzing around my head and I nailed it with my hand and hard. You know, I kind of snapped at it. And so that's a hell of a fast gesture, that forward movement back. And so I nailed the was with my finger. And I knocked
Starting point is 01:23:46 it probably 15 feet, which is a hell of a GeForce blow for an insect. And that bloody thing righted itself, an inch from the ground and dove directly at my face. And I thought, oh my God, like I don't. And then so I watched that. And I thought, wow, that's something, man. That to do that is something. And then I read also about these wolf spiders who are predatory, and if they, if they're on a plant here, and they see a prey animal or insect over here, they will walk down the plant
Starting point is 01:24:16 across the ground and jump on it from behind. It's like, okay, okay, that's a spider, and it has like no brain. And yet that is so unbelievably complicated. Navigation of unfamiliar territory. It's like learning. Yes, it certainly does. But I mean, neuronal,
Starting point is 01:24:35 so neuronal, it's very low. But I think are they are wasps conscious? Are spiders conscious? It's like, and you can take, you know, if you take the whole cortex, the cortex and the limbic system off a cat, and you leave it with nothing but it's hypothalamus and it's spinal cord, that cat is hyper exploratory. And that's bloody weird, right? You take the whole brain off a cat except for like 2%. And it doesn't just
Starting point is 01:25:04 lay there and do nothing. What it does instead is engage in almost all its normal activities, but it can't remember anything. So it's hyper curious. And so, so let me, let me give you my take on consciousness, because no one has a good definition of it. So, and if we have no definition, how am I going to measure it? But there are certain things that we humans cannot do without being conscious. So for example, you cannot plan a party for tomorrow without thinking about what you should buy, what kind of music you're going to have, which friends you're going to invite, what time it's going to start, you have to be conscious of the process before you organize your party. Now, we have evidence, we have very good evidence nowadays of planning in animals. So, for example,
Starting point is 01:25:53 chimpanzees in the wild, they will pick up tools and they're going to walk two miles with these tools and then they're going to use them on a termite hill or whatever, which means that 20 minutes before they started the activity, they were already planning the activity. And so we also have experiments now, they have all sorts of experiments where you can give tools to animals that they can use only the next day, so to speak. So we have good evidence for planning now in animals. Now, if we cannot plan without being conscious, it would be unlikely that these animals can plan without being conscious,
Starting point is 01:26:35 because of course the process is gonna be very similar to what we do. Okay, so I talked to Dr. Roger Penrose about consciousness and he believes it's non-computational and non-algorithmic. And the reason he believes that in part is because the horizon of the future differs unpredictably from the past. That's the problem of induction.
Starting point is 01:26:55 And so he thinks that consciousness is a tool that we use to solve the problem of true unpredictability. And so your association of consciousness with future planning seems to be both dead on and then keeping with that idea. And so, so then, okay, how far down the chain of animal existence do you think that consciousness extends? So consciousness is, I think, a subcategory. The bigger category that we use usually is sentience. You're Mr. Frodova.
Starting point is 01:27:29 Sentience is the ability to experience positive or negative experiences. And sentience probably goes back all the way to insects. And we do that emotional sentience as well because it's associated with approaching avoidance, right? So it's really old. So maybe as soon as you get a nervous system that can represent embodied movement, you get something akin to the emotions of approaching avoidance and then maybe something akin to sentience at that point. Yeah, sentience, for the longest time people debate it, do fish feel pain, which was a sentience question, do fish feel pain, which was a sentence question, do fish feel pain. And then experiment as they found that if you shock fish or, or perhaps,
Starting point is 01:28:12 you know, in a laboratory, you bring them in a situation where they can hide themselves in, in crevices and you shocked them in these places, they're going to avoid the places where they were shocked. Right. So they have anxiety to them, because that avoidances anxiety. So they have pain and they can abstract from that. So they have pain and they remember the pain and they change their behavior as a result of that. And so that was used as a conclusion to conclude
Starting point is 01:28:39 that these animals have sentience because they have experiences. So sentience is the bigger issue. Also, a morally, a very important issue, of course. Do animals have sentience? And consciousness, I never know exactly what it is, because philosophers talk a lot about consciousness, but they never tell me how to measure it.
Starting point is 01:29:01 Well, you said that you know, you associated sentience with the capacity to experience. And then I think, well, there's no difference between the capacity to experience and experience. And there's actually no difference between experience and being because it's very, very difficult to understand being itself in the absence of any experience of being. I mean, what is it? there's not even nothing, right? Because nothing is only a concept that's something that's capable of apprehending something could ever come up with. So I don't see consciousness in being as being separable
Starting point is 01:29:38 concepts in some sense. And okay, okay. So you chased that all the way down to two insects. Okay. So so sentience new is now is recognized also in vertebrates. And it used to be a debate about fish, but I think that debate has been settled. Well, it certainly seems to be the case for octopi.
Starting point is 01:29:56 Those things are so smart and so flexible in your behavior that it's just, it's beyond common. Did you see my octopus teacher? Yeah, yeah. Their capacity to mimic and camouflage's just, it's beyond common. Did you see my octopus teacher? Yeah, yeah, yeah. Their capacity to mimic and camouflage is just, to see that octopus aggregate all those shells and turn itself into a rock, I mean, especially given they have what about a three-year lifespan?
Starting point is 01:30:17 So the octopus is interesting, that movie, the octopus teacher. Many people were very moved by the octopus embracing the person who approached. Octopi are not particularly social. As you say, they live maybe one year, maybe two years. They are very solitary creatures. I don't think they have love for each other or the capacity to love a human.
Starting point is 01:30:42 But maybe that octopus was very curious. And maybe the octopus also felt safe in the presence of a human, but maybe that octopus was very curious and maybe And maybe the octopus also felt safe in the presence of a human because these human kept predators away I'm sure so it was maybe a good situation for the octopus to be in yeah and curiosity could easily be the reason because the octopus is so damn smart it probably has to make sense cognitive capacity like a way yeah But but people then often projects yeah, they feel, they think the octopus is loving the human. I'm not sure about the loving part. I think anything that isn't cuddly is probably a little low on the love end of things, you know. So, so, okay. So, so let's talk about self-consciousness then, because that's the consciousness,
Starting point is 01:31:25 that's a sentience that involves at least a reflexive distinction between self and other, broadly speaking. And I've been interested in, but also skeptical of the animal's self-conscious experiments, because mirror recognition isn't nothing, but humans have an unbelievably elaborated self-consciousness. I mean, we're fully aware of our span of life.
Starting point is 01:31:48 We know we're going to die. We can really distinctly apprehend the future, and we have a really complex, explicit model of ourselves. And animals, well, many animals can recognize themselves in the mirror, and that's not nothing, but it's certainly... No, it's not. There's not many animals. There themselves in the mirror, and that's not nothing, but it's certainly... No, it's not many animals, there's a few. There's only a few who do that. But we do have evidence in chimpanzees
Starting point is 01:32:13 and in orangutans that they embellish themselves in front of the mirror. So the observations, they're mostly anecdotal of apes and captivity who put something on the head and then look in the mirror or in human context, they may put lipstick and put the look themselves in the mirror. So if you embellish yourself and then look in the mirror, it's sort of interesting addition to the pictures.
Starting point is 01:32:39 Right, definitely, definitely. Because yeah, well, it also sees that you view yourself as a Modifiable entity in the eyes of others in all likelihood. Yeah, so we also have evidence from behavior that female apes female chimpanzees and female betoeuvos They do more self-adornment and self-embellishment than the males So the females they will hang things around themselves, put things on the head, walk around with them, much more so than the males. The males also use tools to enhance themselves, but that's more to be more intimidated. Like beating on a beating on a banging
Starting point is 01:33:18 or something, banging things or whatever. Well, when my daughter was young, I watched my kids quite closely when they were little. And there was a variety of differences in them on the sex difference front. My son was much more disagreeable and tough-minded than my daughter, for example. But I also saw remarkably early in my daughter this instinct for self-adornment. Now, my wife doesn't wear much jewelry or much makeup. She's kind of a minimalist in both regards. And so she didn't even have much of a jewelry collection. But my daughter was really entranced by jewelry and makeup from as long far back as I can remember. And I thought, well, maybe that's a socialized difference. But I can't see how because
Starting point is 01:33:59 we didn't she didn't have makeup dolls. And it wasn't part of what she was being fed she didn't watch TV and so but she was She was really really interested in jewelry and endorsement and I think that's that's actually being a stable part of her character because as an adult She's been much more interested in self-adornment than my wife was and it wasn't obvious to me at all that she learned that I thought well that that so was. And it wasn't obvious to me at all that she learned that. And I thought, well, that that so. So in the in the apes that's also a super stimulus thing like the stick stick or backs. That it's more a female thing. And I think in the apes, of course, the females have a genital swelling, which fortunately, I think humans should be happy that we don't
Starting point is 01:34:42 have that. But they have this when they're fertile, their genitals swell up. And I think humans should be happy that we don't have that, but they have this, when they're fertile, their genitals swell up. And I think the female apes, that's an adornment for sure, and they have to it. They may have to it. Now, we know from human women that in the time of ovulation, they adorn themselves more. So our studies on that is that women dress differently at the time of ovulation and they expose more skin and they fancy a clothes on usually at that time. That's also when they prefer the same photos of men with a square jaw than a narrower
Starting point is 01:35:18 jaw at the peak of ovulation. All of this is unconsciously because in that study, what they did is they photographed women in the regular clothes and then they asked them about a menstrual cycle. That's how they figured out how they were dressed relative to the psyche. Right. That would manifest itself in that umfelt, which would be something like, I really feel like dressing up today. There'd be no, and that's a spontaneous motivation that's drifting up from hormonal influence and unconscious in that sense and tuning by hormones, tuning of perception and emotion by hormonal influence. To me, it's always very interesting the things that we do unconsciously and that we are not
Starting point is 01:36:00 really aware of. So there was a study, for example, looking at handshakes. And I think it was an Israeli study. Man shaking hands of man, man shaking hands of women and the other way around. And what they found is that people bring their hand to their nose, so they touch the face, they bring their hand closer to their nose, when they have shaken hands with same sex partners. Not with that right, eh? Not with the opposite sex. And they think it has to do with testing anxiety or testing dominance or testing, right? God only well God only knows what sort of information we're picking up on the old factory front. Like a lot of that could be unconscious. I mean, I know for example that if you give women t-shirts, longer t-shirts have been wore one day by men, and you ask them to
Starting point is 01:36:52 rate the older preference, the women prefer symmetrical, the t-shirts that's symmetrical men wore. So, and that's obviously not in any sense a conscious olfactory preference for like the concept of symmetry. It's just built right into the olfaction. So, we've seen now, during the COVID crisis, that now that we're coming out of the crisis, everyone is very happy that we have physical interaction with others. And I think one of the things we have missed is probably smell. So you can talk with your grandparents over Zoom, but that's not the same as visiting them and seeing them and smelling them. I once...
Starting point is 01:37:33 Yeah, there's a deep sense of familiarity and memory that's associated without, you know. My wife, when my mother died, my wife took one of her scarves and she kept it in a little jar. And now and then she'd take it out and she could detect the smell of her mother on that. And then about five years later, she said, I can't smell my mother on this anymore. And she just got rid of it. But that was a, you know, that smell is so evocative, right? And it can produce a kind of a longing and also a sense of deep comfort.
Starting point is 01:38:09 Yeah, and but it is also, you know, I was once on a flight before COVID, on a flight to Tokyo, I was sitting next to a businessman and he was going to Tokyo for a meeting of a couple of hours and then he would fly back. And I said, you really feel that you need to fly to Tokyo for that. You could do this virtually. He said, no, no, it's a multi-million
Starting point is 01:38:31 dollar deal. And I want to see these people. I want to smell these people. I want to see them up close. He felt that this all this bodily interaction was awfully important to him. And that is how we are. We have remained primates in that regard. And all faction is largely unconscious. We don't even have words for it. We cannot even express what's happening, but it's largely unconscious, but it's extremely important to us. Yeah, well, we, yeah, that's the problem with virtualizing the world to some degree, is we don't know, well, like we talked about earlier does me does female pattern anti social behavior scale on social media and if the answer to that is yes it's like well that's revolutionary because it's never scaled before
Starting point is 01:39:16 and so we have no idea what these virtual virtual environments are doing to us by interfering in unexpected ways with our embodiment. But, you know, I just recently saw that on Twitter, the far fewer women than men. It's mostly men who do this Twittering and Facebook and so on. Yeah, well, I think that what's happened on Twitter, you know, this might be accused of tormenting my theory past its justification. I think that the fact that there is no direct to face-to-face confrontation between men on social media platforms facilitates a female style of anti-social behavior between men on social media platforms. Yeah, yeah, because you see this reputation savaging and it's like, well, you don't do that in face-to-face contact with a man because if I've seen people repeatedly, hundreds of times,
Starting point is 01:40:10 not the same person, but say something on Twitter that no one would ever say face-to-face to another adult male even once in their life, because it would immediately cause, well, a movement towards physical aggression, if not an immediate punch, but that's not there. And so then you think, well, the fact that that's not there, what does that do to the communication pattern?
Starting point is 01:40:35 And the answer is, we have no idea. God only knows what it does. So maybe I could close with a couple of general questions, unless there's something else that you would like to, okay, okay, so I'm always curious about people's intellectual heroes, like I mentioned some of the people who've been really influential to me, my viewers know Freud, Jung, Piaget,
Starting point is 01:41:00 Pankstep, Jeffrey Gray, like a handful of, while you, a handful of animal experimentalists, some philosophers like Nietzsche, Solzhenitz, and for you, like, who's been signaling your intellectual development and really shaped your thinking? Well, the founders of etology, which were Nikutenberg and Convert Lawrence, they were very important, I think, they're very different characters, but they were very important. I think they're very different characters, but they were very important for my intellectual development at Wilson. I think was very important. Yeah, I think Wilson, and you know, I was also influenced in the beginning by Desmond Morris' book, The Naked Eight. Yeah, even though now, I can easily mention quite a few things
Starting point is 01:41:49 that are not correct in that book in terms of theories, but the way he wrote it, and sort of irreverent way of writing about the human species and sort of also, and also making fun. Just the idea that this comparison between species was interesting ethically and morally and emotionally. That was kind of revolutionary. He did a good job of that.
Starting point is 01:42:12 Yeah, his book was extremely successful, and partly because he had this tone of mocking humans a little bit, you know? And I think he did a real good job of that. So yeah, those were my early influences, I think. Yeah, one of the things that's so interesting about your writing, I think, is apart from its remarkable approachability, which I really think is a gift, is that it's also non-naively positive and optimistic, which is so cool to see, because that does, that combination
Starting point is 01:42:46 doesn't occur that often, often people who are optimistic are kind of naive, but you're a hardcore biologist, sort of in the scientific trenches in a real sense. And yet, I always come away from your books with a fundamentally positive, they have positive consequences on me. And the fact that you, I think your work on the biological basis of moral development is, I think it's an un of what would you say? It's kind of epoch level, philosophical significance. You get such a question. I'm an optimist by nature. And even though I started studying violence and aggression, because that was the topic at the time,
Starting point is 01:43:28 everyone was studying aggressive behavior, I very soon switched to conflict resolution, because I found it actually more interesting than the aggression itself. Well, that's so extremely cool, you know, that you got in through the darkness, so to speak. But the consequence of that was that you actually learned that Aggression and violence weren't the fundamental basis of social interaction and that
Starting point is 01:43:51 Reconciliation and competence and empathy and long-term planning all of that actually turns out to be more Fundamental and important. Yeah, that's so optimistic that that could possibly be true And I must say I don't enjoy violence. So I have colleagues who will tell you a horrible story about two chimps killing it sort of, or people who watch sports games where there's a lot of violence. And I've seen a lot of violence among the primates, I can tell you. I know, but I don't enjoy it. It's, I prefer when they get together afterwards and groom each other. Right, right.
Starting point is 01:44:30 Well, that's interesting too, because that actually made you capable of seeing that when you did see it. You know, and that's really something. Yeah, well, your work and your work has really been a cardinal influence on me and I've been able to talk about it with all these people I talked to, you know, 150,000 people publicly in the last three or four months and making this case that the fundamental basis of human social organization is much better construed as reciprocity and the spirit of play and the ability to reconcile and and and that's such a people just don't hear that and to know that that's grounded in a solid solid scientific literature as well as you know being the target of ethical conceptualization it's it's so helpful to put that other
Starting point is 01:45:21 foundation underneath the claims that you know there's real reasons to be optimistic about the nature of social organization and individual motivation and your work on gender differences and your support in some sense for male ambition in that regard. It's also, I think, unbelievably important. So I really appreciated you talking to me today. It's a pleasure to meet you. Yeah, let's talk on your Zoom. I really appreciated you talking to me today.
Starting point is 01:45:44 It's pleasure to meet you. Yeah, let's talk when you're soon.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.