The Jordan B. Peterson Podcast - 562. What Do We Actually Know About Autism? | Sir Simon Baron-Cohen
Episode Date: July 14, 2025Dr. Jordan B. Peterson speaks with psychologist and autism researcher Dr. Simon Baron-Cohen about the nature of empathy, systemizing, and the neurological basis of autism. This needed conversation exp...lores how humans develop theory of mind, the differences between cognitive and affective empathy, and why some individuals gravitate toward systems over social interaction. They discuss the evolutionary roots of invention, gender differences in cognition, and how autistic traits relate to creativity and pattern recognition. Unflinchingly, they also discuss the darker side of empathy deficits—including psychopathy and cruelty—raising urgent questions about compassion, human development, and our capacity for evil. Professor of Psychology and Psychiatry at the University of Cambridge, Simon Baron-Cohen is Director of the Autism Research Centre and a global leader in autism research. He pioneered the 'mindblindness' and 'empathizing-systemizing' theories, has authored over 770 scientific papers, and was knighted in 2021 for his contributions to autism understanding. His work spans clinical practice, public education, and cutting-edge research on cognitive sex differences, empathy, and neurodiversity. This episode was filmed on July 5th, 2025 | Links | For Simon Baron-Cohen: On X https://x.com/sbaroncohen?lang=enRead Dr. Baron-Cohen’s most recent book, as discussed in the show, “The Pattern Seekers: How Autism Drives Human Invention” https://a.co/d/9hsG5MR
Transcript
Discussion (0)
One of the things we might want to do is lay out the cardinal features of autism.
How would you characterize autism?
Autism isn't just a single thing.
There are multiple dimensions to autism, multiple factors.
The fact that it's a multidimensional disorder risks obscuring its central features.
If we just focus on the things that they have challenges with, it's like a deficit model.
If we recognize that brains develop differently
and that some brains focus more on systems
than they do on people,
there's growing evidence that autistic people
are better than non-autistic people
at understanding systems, at pattern recognition,
which is obviously a great asset
in a lot of different environments.
recognition, which is obviously a great asset in a lot of different environments.
Hello everybody. My guest today, Professor Simon Baron Cohen, is a world-renowned clinical psychologist and director of the Autism Research Center at Cambridge. He's done groundbreaking work on autism, empathy, systemizing, and the extreme male brain.
In doing so, he's reshaped our understanding of neurodiversity. He's the author of numerous books including his latest, The Pattern Seekers,
How Autism Drives Human Invention. He spoke to us at length today about tool use, about theory of
mind, and most controversially perhaps the differences in male and female approaches to
the world and male and female neurological structure.
Join us for that.
Well, Dr. Baron Cohen, I have been looking forward
to talking to you for a long time.
I followed your research, I don't know, for how long?
15 years, 20 years, a long time.
And there's a lot of things that we,
there's a lot of interests that we share. a lot of interest that we share so I thought what I do to begin with is outline
Your main domains of interest and you can correct me and make sure that I've got that
Formulated properly because I would like to walk through them
Sure, you know which in some relatively systematic and empathic manner. So
So let tell me what you think of this breakdown. You're very
interested in how people adopt the mindset of other people, how we
understand other people, and I really want to talk to you about that. So I want
to throw some ideas at you and see how your vision of mutual understanding and
emotional alignment differs and maybe is
similar. You're very interested in gender differences. Let's call them sex
differences just to be politically incorrect. That shades off into your
concern with systematizing versus empathizing, which is a, I suppose, a
dimensional analysis of interest, although it shades into temperament.
You're quite curious about empathy and evil, and you're very interested in pattern seeking.
And so, are there other major domains that might be worth delving into, or does that
give us a reasonable rubric? That's a great framework and I just want to start by saying I'm honored to be in
conversation with you. I've been looking forward to talking to you for a long time too. And actually
this is going to sound funny but I was sitting right at the back when you spoke at the O2 Center in London. And that was probably one of your largest gigs.
And I was way at the back row.
So you were quite small on stage,
but it was fun listening to you.
And I'm looking forward to our conversation.
In terms of the topics,
I guess there's one other thing to mention,
which is that I'm the director
of the Autism Research Center in Cambridge.
And on the long list of topics that you mapped out for us, I guess we'll probably touch on
the field of autism and autistic people.
Yeah, yeah, I guess I would have segued into that through systematizing and empathizing,
but it is good to highlight it as a major,
obviously it's good to highlight it as a major concern
since it is a major research area of yours.
Okay, well that's good.
Maybe we'll start with understanding others.
And so let me,
I'm gonna ask a relatively complicated question
and then I'd like you to indicate your agreements and disagreements, if you would.
So I was very influenced in my understanding of social perception by J.J. Gibson and his theory of affordances,
and also by Jeffrey Gray and his essentially cybernetic neuroscience theory. And so this is how I'm understanding it at the moment.
And it's relevant to an understanding of stories and also an understanding of mind, I think,
is that we shape our perceptions around a goal, a destination, let's say.
Our perceptions are guides to navigation. And when we occupy the conceptual space of someone else,
we adopt their goal that sinks our perceptions,
and it also sinks our emotions,
because we experience emotions in relationship to a goal.
And I was also influenced in that notion by Piaget, you know, the
developmental psychologist who was interested in how children establish
shared frames of reference in games. So anyways, I'm curious, start with that if
you would. It's interesting to hear your influences because mine are a little bit different. So for me, the major influence in how I think about
other people's minds and the whole question of how do we
imagine what someone else is thinking
and what they might be feeling,
the influence came from the philosopher Daniel Dennett.
And I don't know if you know his work, but he published a-
Yes, I interviewed Dennett.
Okay.
And I know his work, yes.
Yeah, so he published a really, I think,
really important book called The Intentional Stance,
probably in the late 70s or early 80s.
And the idea of the intentional stance
is that when we look around at the world, like
the world of objects, we don't particularly attribute mental states.
But when we look at people, what humans do, and some people would say uniquely, is that
we take this intentional stance.
That's to say we try to imagine what's going through their mind.
And mental states, he argued, cover not just emotions
and goals, you picked out those two,
but importantly also epistemic states, so beliefs,
what people know.
So in every conversation, in every interaction,
what most people are doing is that they're monitoring
the other person's state of mind.
What does the other person know?
What do they think?
What do they want?
What are they feeling?
And in my terminology, more recently,
I call that cognitive empathy.
But Dennett called it the intentional stance, taking the intentional stance.
So the word intentional is meant to cover the whole range of mental states
that another person might have.
And he gives this wonderful example in his book of how important this is,
that every time we venture out on the highway,
we are making assumptions about other people's mental states.
For example, that other people want to stay alive,
so they're gonna stay in their lane
and not swerve into our lane.
And that other people can see us
and that they know that we can see that they know that
we can see them.
So many different kind of unconscious sort of processing of what other people are thinking,
what they believe, what they know, what they want.
And that's happening in every conversation, or at least it should be.
So the intentional stance, I think that that idea was influenced by Husserl and Heidegger, maybe
Heidegger more particularly, because he insisted that our fundamental attitude towards the
world was one of care.
And that's a good way, I think, of uniting perception with emotion.
If we perceive our destinations, I guess you're doing that on the highway with other people
too, what if the destinations would be to arrive alive, for example?
So you're inhabiting a shared structure of value and that highlights certain perceptions
and foregrounds certain perceptions and hides others.
Okay. So,
so I think you've, you've just introduced an extra element, which is,
do we care about another person?
And I would say that that's an extra element because you could adopt the intentional stance.
You could think about other people's thoughts without really caring about them.
And you know,
about other people's thoughts without really caring about them. And you know, if you think about a psychopath or somebody with antisocial personality disorder,
they can model what someone else is thinking, but they may not care about the person's well-being.
They may not care about their feelings.
So you know, a psychopath, for example, can, you know, they can deceive you, they can manipulate
you to believe that something's true when it's not.
They turn up at your front door and they pretend that they are the gas man coming to read the
gas meter.
And then once they're in your house, they might mug you.
So that kind, so they clearly don't care.
But most people don't operate with the lack of care.
So that's why I kind of think there's a difference
between the simple act of can you imagine
what someone's thinking and do you care about
another person's feelings
and whether they're going to suffer or not.
Right, well, is it fair to say that,
is it fair to say perhaps that that's a matter of
value prioritization? I mean, I've increasingly been
conceptualizing antisocial behavior, even psychopathy, as a form of delayed
immaturity. I mean, we know that two-year-olds are
relatively egocentric, and we also know that there's
a subset of two-year-olds who are willing to use aggression to pursue their ends.
It's about five percent of boys seem to be like that temperamentally.
Most of them are socialized by the time they're four, by the way, but the ones that aren't
tend to be life-term persistent offenders. So imagine that the initial stages of maturity with regard to intention are narrowly self-focused,
so they might be hypothalamic even, you know, they're basic drive focused and they don't
take anyone else into account. They don't take
reciprocal interactions into account and then as you mature, it seems to me that you prioritize
the relationship over immediate gratification because you learn, well you learn that's the
pathway to friendship for example because friendship is, Go ahead. So I would take a slightly different view.
I mean, people talk about the terrible twos, you know,
and when my kids were two years old, I didn't find them terrible actually.
I mean, sometimes they have their own wishes
and they get upset if they're frustrated that they can't have what they want.
But I don't think most two-year-olds are just driven by self-interest
and purely in the pursuit of their own goal.
Some two-year-olds maybe.
But there are these studies, you brought in Piaget,
but also you're really referencing
the field of developmental psychology.
But there are these tests that I'm sure you're aware of
where the mother might be playing with a toy hammer
and she acts as if she's hit her thumb with the hammer
and shows some pain.
And even two-year-olds will look up at the mother's face
when she is in pain, they recognize that.
And they may come over and console her,
or give her a kiss, or give her a hug or a cuddle,
because she's acting as if she's in distress.
So I don't see, I don't think even the typical two-year-old
is oblivious to another person's feelings,
and nor are they kind of like detached,
I don't know, psychopaths where they just don't care
that somebody else is in pain.
They actually have the natural reaction
of wanting to alleviate another person's suffering.
And of course, there may be differences in maturation,
I think that's what you're saying.
We might get onto the controversial area
of gender differences and whether girls on average
might be developing faster in terms of being able
to recognize other people's feelings
and respond with an appropriate emotion.
The way I started talking about this earlier
was to talk about cognitive empathy, which
in my framework is the recognition element, recognizing what someone is thinking or feeling.
But the flip side of the coin, the other fraction of empathy I call affective empathy, and that's
having an appropriate emotion, an appropriate emotional response
to somebody else's state of mind.
And of course, for most people, most interactions combine the two.
The cognitive element, I might be reading your face, I might be reading your body language,
or I might be inferring what you're thinking or feeling from the context,
what I know about you. But the affective part should be kind of kicking in very, very quickly,
because if I see that you're bored or if I see that you're a little bit upset or angry,
I should be responding in real time with an appropriate emotion. So I wonder if there's a way of bridging the gap between the approaches that we took.
I mean, it seems to me likely that empathy, broadly speaking, is sufficiently crucial to
social creatures like us, that it might be represented at multiple levels of neurological function.
And so those affective responses, emotional responses that you described as characteristic
of two-year-olds, which are obviously there, and which vary with the child's temperament
to some degree, I wonder if those are a more primordial and immediate form of social cognition contrasted
with, and maybe this is related to your notion of cognitive empathy, with the understanding
of long-term reciprocity that develops when children learn to play complex multiplayer
games and extend them across time, right? So, tell me again,
that affect of empathy, is that more instinctual, would you say?
Yeah, I mean, I guess it begs the question, what do we mean by an instinct? But I'm sure
it's very rapid. But what I liked about something you said earlier was about the developmental kind of
timetable or the unfolding of these things in early childhood.
Because I used to work in this field, and the field is broadly called theory of mind.
When does a child develop a theory of mind?
When is a child able to appreciate what someone
else knows and believes and thinks?
And the consensus back then, and I think the literature still supports this, is that it's
not till about the age of three or four that kids can appreciate that somebody else may
have a false belief, you know, that Jordan thinks it's Tuesday,
but actually it's Wednesday.
You know, so it takes till the age of about three or four
before kids can know what is true
and understand that somebody else
might hold a false belief about the world.
And then it takes a little bit longer,
not till the age of five or six,
before they can do what's called second order false belief.
You know, that I think that you believe
that somebody else believes something false.
That's a kind of, so it's kind of like at a meta level.
And these, you know, I can easily see
that these are kind of orders of magnitude of complexity
that kids have to master.
If we bring this back to autistic kids,
there's quite a lot of evidence that they show delays
in mastering some of these kind of social cognitive
or levels of theory of mind reasoning.
Let's diverge slightly into the realm of autism briefly. I want to ask you some specific
questions there. So I know that the classification of autistic spectrum disorders has widened
tremendously since the original formulation of the concept. And I suppose that
segues or shades into your work on systematizing and empathizing, which is maybe even a broader conceptual framework.
It seems to me, I mean it may maybe depends on the severity of autism, but it seems to me that
the notion that
relatively severely or even moderately autistic people are impaired in
their theory of mind is appropriate. But the initial symptoms of severe autism seem to me to be so
neurologically primary that they point to something more fundamental, that maybe that theory of mind
deficits are a secondary consequence, because they don't, severely autistic children,
they're often very delayed in their language development, and that's not true of, it's not
particularly true of people with Down syndrome who are also very cognitively impaired.
They still develop language quite fluidly, let's say.
But also, this is the strangest thing, I think, and you can tell me what you think about this,
is that while autistic children often don't like to be touched, and that's really something because mammals like to be touched. And so
whatever's gone wrong is so primordial that it's part of a system that we share with all
mammals. So tell me how you reconcile that with the theory of mind hypothesis.
So I guess the theory of mind hypothesis, I would criticize it as being too simplistic
in the sense that autism isn't just a single thing.
There are multiple dimensions to autism, multiple factors.
And theory of mind may be just one of those,
but if you just focus on that,
you may be missing many other aspects to autism.
I'm just gonna pick you up on a couple of things.
One is the word severe.
You talked about severe autism.
And I wonder how we're gonna define that,
because I think the way you are starting
to define it is if a child's not talking.
So if a child is not only autistic but also has language delay, that makes it more severe.
And I think you were also hinting that if a child not only is autistic but also has
a learning disability or in the US it's called an intellectual disability.
So their IQ is below the average range.
Again, that would make it more severe.
And I think what we've come to understand
is that there are many different types of autism.
Although we've only got a single word,
in the US it's called autism spectrum disorder,
ASD.
I personally don't like that terminology because the word disorder is quite stigmatizing.
But nevertheless, so I just use the word autism as a kind of umbrella concept.
But you can have autism with language delay or without language delay, with intellectual disability or without,
with ADHD or without.
So many different kind of combinations,
co-occurring conditions.
But whether you'd say that one is more severe than another,
it's not clear to me.
You could have somebody,
and I'll give you an example of an autistic man
who didn't speak until he was 11,
didn't read or write until he was 18,
but he's now a professor in my university.
So at what point in his development would you say
his autism was either severe or his
autism as a lifelong condition was severe?
There's a lot of change, a lot, you know, and even just taking that one indicator of
language delay wouldn't have told us what his intellectual level was.
And that's something that I've certainly learned,
that you can have autistic people
who have no expressive language, no speech.
Maybe they need to communicate through devices,
but cognitively or intellectually, they may be very smart.
So it's very easy to kind of underestimate somebody
just because they can't speak.
Well, let that'll, we can use that differentiation as a
springboard to getting to the heart of two matters, I would
say. So what I would, in an attempt to answer the question
with regard to severity, one of the things we might want to do
is lay out the cardinal features of autism because as a multi-dimensional disorder, I mean, the fact that it's a multi-dimensional
disorder risks obscuring its central features.
And I tried to highlight language delay and more importantly, antipathy towards touch,
which I think is the most, I mean, that's just so incomprehensible, that particular.
So how would you characterize the autism at its core?
And also maybe we can use that to move into a discussion
of systematizing versus empathizing
if those two things are intelligibly related.
Yeah, yeah, yeah.
So I'll come on to the definition of the core characteristics of autism in just one second,
but I realize I didn't pick up on your quite interesting observation
that some autistic people don't like to be touched.
And again, I'm just being careful to not generalize it,
because it may not be true for all autistic people.
But the way I interpret that, and you're absolutely right
that most humans enjoy intimate touch.
You know, it calms them down.
It makes them feel emotionally closer to the other person
if it's in the context, for example,
of a loving relationship, like between a parent and a child.
So why would an autistic child or some autistic kids
not like to be touched?
And for me, that may come down to not understanding
the other person's state of mind.
What does the other person want when they touch you?
It may come down to the unpredictability of it.
If somebody doesn't announce that they're going to touch you
and they just touch you out of the blue,
we know that autistic people don't, in general,
don't like things that are unpredictable.
And it's going to kind of segue into this whole concept of systemizing, because if you
can systemize the world, you can predict it.
And autistic people, so this kind of brings us into maybe some of the areas of how you
define autism, that autistic people have a hard time with unexpected change.
If things are predictable, they do fine.
And in the world of music, where you can play the same song
over and over again, or watching your favorite video,
and you can watch the same episode over and over again,
the world is beautifully predictable.
And some autistic kids really gravitate towards domains which
are predictable, where there's the opportunity for repetition, so that there's nothing that
catches them out.
And clinically, like you, I'm a clinical psychologist.
You know, what I see and what I hear autistic people tell me is that when unexpected things happen,
they get very stressed.
And that could be just going into the classroom at school,
but where instead of being able to sit
in your favorite chair or the chair you've always sat in,
somebody else is sitting there
and you're expected to sit somewhere new.
Or maybe there's even like a change in the wallpaper.
It could be a small change, you know, it could be that your mum has given you a different
t-shirt to wear and the texture of that t-shirt is different to your favourite t-shirt.
So it might be at a very sensory level, but it's a change. And so dealing with unexpected change,
you know, seems to be a cause of challenge
for many autistic people.
But now if we just go back to your main question,
how do I define autism?
I don't call it a disorder.
I've already mentioned that.
I see autism as a disability
that's really important to acknowledge, and
the disability is in social relationships and communication, and also being able to
cope with unexpected change. But I also see autism as a difference, and that word really
comes from the relatively recent framework of neurodiversity.
The autistic people, right from birth and actually before birth,
their brains are developing differently, not necessarily disordered, simply different,
so that when they emerge into the world, they may not be looking, for example, at faces the way a non-autistic child would
be doing so, because faces move unpredictably, especially if you can't imagine what's behind
the movement of someone else's face or somebody else's eye.
If I suddenly look to my left, you can make sense of that because you may be thinking
Simon has seen something that he's interested in, but you're already attributing a perceptual
state, he's seen something, and an epistemic state, he's interested in something, maybe
a volitional state, he's seen something that he wants.
Young kids can make all of these attributions so that faces and facial expressions
and eye movements are not confusing.
They're actually, they're very easy to interpret.
Autistic kids may be a bit delayed
in reading another person's body language
and facial expression and tone of voice
because they may have trouble with cognitive empathy
or theory of mind.
So are you thinking that, and I'm thinking, I interviewed Temple Grandin,
and so I'm going to talk about some of the things she said here in a moment,
but are you hypothesizing perhaps as a unifying rubric that the autistic condition is characterized by a relative inability to infer a uniting, it's like a uniting narrative or a uniting concept,
because what you implied was that if I'm watching your face, which is changing, unlike the background, let's say, which is very stable, that the reason that doesn't trigger anomaly detection
anxiety in me is because I understand the thread
of our conversation and that thread is a unifying
reference point and I can assume that all the manifestations
of your differences are a variation of that single
thing.
So Temple Grandin told me that she can't, or she had real difficulty abstracting in
a very particular way.
So she said, for example, and you see this with some autistic drawings, she can't bring
to mind a generic church.
You know how children draw a house with a triangle for the roof and a square for the
house and a door and two windows?
No house looks like that, but all houses are like that, right?
It's more like a hieroglyphic or an abstraction.
The same with those stick figures, right?
They're kind of quasi-linguistic abstractions.
And Grandin said when she thinks of any phenomenon,
she doesn't think of a generalization.
She thinks of a concrete exemplar
and can't abstract upward.
I wonder if that's the same issue
as this lack of theory of mind.
So, first of all, again, sorry to pick you up on that word lack, but it's a kind of,
it's relative degrees of disability.
You know, a lack of theory of mind is the extreme point.
And I wrote a book back in 1995
called Mind Blindness, which kind of conveys this idea
that some people might be totally blind
to someone else's state of mind,
but that would be the extreme case.
For many people, and this is a spectrum
of individual differences, but for many autistic people,
it may not be a complete
lack of theory of mind, it might just take them longer to infer what someone is thinking
or feeling. And by the way, you've used the word narrative in the narrative of our conversation.
That assumes you've got linguistic information to go on. But actually actually for a non-autistic person, you don't necessarily need language.
You know, you and I could have a playful interaction
without words, and you know, parents do this
with their infants long before the infant has language.
Or if you're watching a mime artist, like Marcel Marceau,
you know, you can infer what the mime artist is thinking
or what they're trying to communicate
just through facial expression,
through eye movements, through gesture.
So words of course give you another set of information
like a printout of what some,
what might be going on in someone else's mind.
But the basic ability of just imagining
what another person is thinking or feeling,
or wanting or intending or hoping,
all of that could be done without words.
And that's why you can have, for example,
two deaf people communicating perfectly well
without speech, just reading each other's hand movements
or finger movements or facial expressions.
Right, can you infer a narrative merely through drama?
I mean, that seems to me to be what you're pointing to,
is you can act it out.
Like, it seems to me that what a narrative is
is a verbal description of a drama, right?
And the drama is embodied in the way that you described so that a mime artist can manage it or people can can act it out.
I would say that the fundamentals, whether you're going to call it a drama, the fundamentals of communication is that two people, at least two people, establish a topic.
is that two people, at least two people, establish a topic.
And right now we're having a conversation
and we're both recognizing
what is the topic of conversation.
And that's an incredible thing
when you stop to think about it.
Because if you look at two individuals
from any other species,
if you think of two sheep in the field,
or two dogs that might be in your home,
the animals are not establishing a topic
that they're then going to communicate about.
And yet in humans, if you think about a typical toddler,
they use the pointing gesture.
So, you know, an 18 month old, even younger,
14 month old child will point to something,
look back at their parent to see if the parent
is looking at what they're pointing at.
So they've established a topic of conversation,
all without words.
And autistic kids often are delayed
in the ability to produce the pointing gesture.
Oh, okay.
Okay.
That's crucial.
Yeah.
Or if you suddenly look out the window, I'll turn to look at what you're looking at.
Right.
And a 14-month-old child will do that.
That was the work of Jerome Bruner, a developmental psychologist way back in the 70s.
He called it joint attention or shared attention.
And of course you can elaborate it into a narrative
because when we're in the audience
and we're watching a play in a theater,
we're not only modeling what is the actor trying to convey,
what's going on in the actor's mind,
but we're keeping track of how do all the sequences in the narrative fit together into a narrative arc. So you can make it as complex
as you like, but the simple building blocks are being able to imagine what someone else
is thinking or attending to. But can I bring us back to something that you said about Temple
Grandin? is that okay?
Absolutely.
Yeah, because I said that autism was a difference, not just a disability.
And some of the differences include excellent attention to detail.
Temple Grandin talks about this in some of her books where she notices things that other
people miss, just small details in the environment.
So if she was looking in the church,
she'd be looking at the shape of the windows
or the number of the windows or the texture of the bricks,
all the small detail.
So excellent attention to detail,
often excellent memory for detail.
And then something that I introduced in my recent book,
The Pattern Seekers, is excellent pattern recognition.
And this kind of relates to what you were saying,
that for non-autistic people,
we seem to move very quickly to the general.
Although we look at specific things
like this specific church,
we very rapidly slotted into a category called churches.
And then we abstract so that if we're asked
to draw the church, as you gave the example,
we don't produce a very lifelike,
a photographic representation of this particular church.
No, icon.
Yeah, I mean, it's almost like,
it's almost like, you know, our drawing
or our memory of the object is filtered through
what are the general properties of churches.
Or if we have to draw a picture of a dog,
we might have in our mind kind of the general
prototype of a dog, and that's what we draw.
Whereas for an autistic person, the details matter.
And actually they seem to be very, they have a cognitive style where they prefer to go,
to stay in the specifics rather than to generalize very quickly.
And that brings us onto the topic of systemizing.
Because when you systemize, what you're trying to do
is understand a system that's in front of you.
Could be any kind of system.
It could be the light switch in the house
where you're trying to understand what happens
if the switch is up or down.
You know, is it turning on
this light or that light? It could be the water faucet or tap, as we say in the UK.
You know, if I turn the tap, you know, clockwise the water comes out faster. So you're looking
at these little systems in the world. It could be the keyboard in your house, where you kind
of learn that if I play this note
or this combination of notes, I get this particular sound.
But it's the detail that matters
because if you start moving to the abstraction
or to the general, you're not going to understand
the system in front of you.
And I see this as an asset.
You know, in the old days, they used to say
that autistic people have difficulty generalizing.
I actually see this as an asset rather than a disability.
Because if you really want to understand,
let's say the iPhone 10 rather than the iPhone 12,
there's a world of difference in the operating systems
between these two devices.
And if you're gonna become an expert in this device,
you know, don't rush to try to understand what do all iPhones share.
Just try and understand this particular system in front of you.
And that could be true for anything you're trying to systemize.
And you know, you might be trying to understand the wings of a butterfly, of
this particular butterfly, or you know, the structure of a snowflake, or you know, how
birds are able to fly, but not all birds, how this particular bird is able to fly. That's a natural system.
You know, the iPhone is more of a digital or, you know,
electronic system.
Music, we talked about earlier,
is a kind of, it's a set of notes.
But what systems share is that they are rule governed.
So when you systemize, you're trying to analyze the rules
that govern the system in order to predict
how the system works or to understand the system
in great depth.
So this is kind of where I see the strengths
of autistic people.
And this kind of fits into this whole
neurodiversity framework.
If we just focus on the things
that they have challenges with,
it's like a deficit model.
But if we recognize that brains develop differently
and that some brains in some humans in the world
focus more on systems than they do on people,
because systems are ultimately predictable if you focus on the
rules.
I call these if-and-then rules.
If I take something and I do something to it, then I get a particular result.
So it's kind of a Boolean logic trying to understand how a system works.
And bringing this back to autism, there's growing evidence that
autistic people are better than non-autistic people at understanding
systems, at pattern recognition, which is obviously a great asset in a lot of
different environments. So do you see... Okay, so two things. Is there a dimension of systematizing or systemizing
and a dimension of empathizing?
Because I've seen your model as systemizing on one pole
and empathizing on the other, but...
Okay, but no, but it's two distributions.
Okay, one other question too, just to cram it in there
and we'll get both of them at the same time maybe. Well, I couldn't help but think while you were talking that,
I guess this sort of came out of my, to some degree, out of my study of mythology, you know,
in the story of Genesis, it's engineers that build the Tower of Babel. It's the people we
would call engineers, and they're
inclined to a kind of technological worship. But there's something deeper underneath that,
I think, that might be relevant to the discussion that we just had, which is that human beings
are staggeringly remarkable in their ability to use tools. And is that systemizing tendency,
natural variation in the proclivity
to see the world as a place of tools?
You know, in the Gibson model that I've elaborated,
so once you specify a target of a destination,
your perception orients itself to show you a pathway. Okay, now destination,
pathway, tools, obstacles. Okay, but there's this, this now Gibson didn't talk
about this, but there's a social equivalent,
hey, because friends and foes are like the social analog of tools and obstacles.
And then I'm wondering if, you know, the empathizing types construe the world as a place of social
relations in relationship to a goal and the systemizers see tools and obstacles.
You said the autistic proclivity is also to hyperreact to the unexpected.
The unexpected is a very particular form of obstacle, right?
It's when the system doesn't do what it's supposed to do.
And you could imagine that if you're system oriented,
that part of that might be sensitivity to deviation
because it indicates a flaw in your understanding of this.
So is it associated with tool use, do you think?
Is this something that fundamental?
Sorry, is what associated with tool use?
Systemizing, are the neurological variations Is this something that fundamental? Sorry, is what associated with tool use? Autism.
Systemizing.
Are the neurological variations you're describing
variations in tool use proclivity?
Absolutely.
So my last book, The Pattern Seekers,
the subtitle was,
How Autism Drives Invention.
And the book is really a theory of invention,
by which I mean how humans, homo sapiens,
uniquely have the capacity for generative invention.
We don't just invent one tool and leave it at that.
What seems to be unique about our species is that we invent
unstoppably. And in the book what I do is I go back through the archaeological evidence,
which suggests that humans have been inventing complex tools for at least 70 to 100,000 years.
And I kind of take the reader through, for example, the first bow and arrow,
which the evidence is there in archeology
was about 70,000 years ago.
It's a much more complex tool
than the kind of tools that other species use.
You know, a monkey or an ape that picks up a rock to use it as a hammer to crack a nut
is using a tool, but it's a very simple tool.
And they could even just be achieving the outcome, getting the juicy fruit through associative
learning that using the rock to crack the nut gets you the juicy
fruit, it gets you the reward and so they repeat that and many species
have been repeating that kind of simple tool use for millions of years.
There's no innovation.
There's no innovation and there's no complexity to the system. Whereas a bow and arrow uses this if and then Boolean logic
that if I take a stretchy fiber and I attach an arrow to it
and release the tension, then the arrow will fly.
So it's if and then.
And the first musical instrument, again,
it's a complex tool, was actually from 40,000 years ago.
It was the hollow bone of a bird,
which one of our ancestors had drilled holes into it.
So it's a flute, the first musical instrument
that's been found.
But the if and then algorithm that drives human invention
is if I blow down the bone and I cover one hole,
then I get a particular note.
But if I blow down the hollow bone and cover two holes,
I get a different note.
So the ancestor who invented this flute
had created a complex tool
and also had created a complex tool and also had created a complex system, which is music itself.
Music follows rules or patterns. You know, you can have different notes that sit in relation to each other.
You can have musical keys. You can have musical phrases that repeat, etc.
So just like any other system, it's rule governed.
And back to your point,
is systemizing a dimension of individual differences?
Absolutely.
We can all systemize to some level.
So, you know, if something goes wrong at home,
you can try and fix it by understanding the system.
You know, if your door handle won't open,
that's the problem, the challenge that you're trying to solve.
And you might try and understand,
well, what's actually stopping the handle from turning?
But you're using that-
You decompose it into parts.
Yeah, and what Temple Grandin was kind of telling us
and what autistic people do when they approach a problem is they try
and take the problem apart.
They want to see what are the individual components,
the if, the and, and the then.
And what drives human invention is that
once we've understood the pattern, the if and then,
we can then play with the if,
because that's one parameter,
or we could play with the if, because that's one parameter, or we could play with the and,
which is the operation.
Engineers call it input-operation-output,
but I call it if-and-then.
And, you know, and some of us systemize
better than others.
Autistic people may systemize nonstop,
that they're looking around the world,
and they're fascinated by how is
that table made? How is the leg of the table actually joined to the table surface itself?
Or why is that table leg a little bit wonky? So they're trying to understand the system.
Or could you take an existing system and make a change to it
so that you've invented a new system?
And we can think of autistic people
who have used systemizing to their advantage.
And one example would be Elon Musk.
Elon Musk came out as autistic a few years ago. You know, he's an engineer and we can take this back
to the Tower of Babel.
You know, when the Old Testament was being written,
you know, that may have only been 3,000 or 5,000 years ago.
Humans have been doing this systemizing
for at least 70 to 100,000 years,
building things as complex as the Tower of Babel.
But the engineers, the systemizers,
needed to make sure that the tower was going to stand up
and was going to be able to withstand the wind,
was made from the right materials,
how all the pieces were going to lock together,
just like when we drive over,
I don't know, the bridge in San Francisco. Those were the engineers and the systemizers who produced
that incredible invention. But the empathizers might be, you know, again, that's another bell
curve of individual differences. Some people have a greater interest or drive to empathize and they're the ones who
might be kind of much more focused on communicating and checking how other people in the community are,
checking in with their friends, with their family, making sure nobody's left out. They're not so
focused on, you know, why is the door sticking or, you know, how is this radio made? But
they're more interested in, you know, can I communicate? Can I create relationships
and can I look after relationships?
Now you have associated those differences with sex differences as well. And I know among chimpanzees, there are marked sex differences in play toy preference
in juvenile chimps.
So if you give a male juvenile something that approximates a doll, he's likely to tear it
apart, whereas a female is likely to take even a block of wood and treat it as if it's
an infant, let's say.
And those, like, chimpanzee juvenile males prefer wheeled toys to infant-like toys,
which is quite remarkable given that they don't really have any...
What? They don't... Now, I'm wondering, you know, if there's an association between that and hunting, for example, and
also I'm curious about what you've made of the sex differences.
We kind of hypothesized already that a perceptual landscape could be differentiated systematically, so you're looking for tools and obstacles, or it could be differentiated systematically. So you're looking for
tools and obstacles or it could be differentiated socially. I mean,
obviously your relationships with people are another means to joint ends or
individual ends. I mean, so you could imagine the world lays itself out to
people as a place of things or as a place of social interactions.
And it also sheds some light on maybe the hyper-reactivity to novelty that is characteristic of the systematizer.
Because if one of the potential pitfalls of paying attention to detail would be sensitivity to
variation in detail, whereas if your proclivity is to perceive higher up the
abstraction chain, those differences are going to vanish in the generalization.
Yeah, but as soon as you move to the general, you're going to lose a lot of
information. Right, right, of course. That's part of the advantage of moving to the general.
Yeah, so if I call them hypersystemizers,
a hypersystemizer, which may characterize
some or many autistic people,
they care more about the particular system in front of them,
to understand this particular system.
So what would be an example?
particular system. So what would be an example? So let's say that the child is building a tower out of Lego bricks. Many autistic kids love Lego, you know, and they often focus
for long periods, you know, sometimes it's called obsessions, but I think that's a kind
of pejorative way of describing what they're doing.
They're locked into the detail
of how do all the bricks fit together
and what they're trying to build.
But they may not be looking at other children's faces
in the room or at adults who are trying to interact
with them because they're so focused on the system
that they're trying to build.
They're not trying to think of,
what are all the different possible systems I could build?
They're just trying to build this particular system.
Once they've done that, they may then,
under their own control, make a change to the system.
So it's not that they're averse to all change,
but they want change to be under their control.
And I guess what's challenging for autistic kids,
or for a systemizer, a strong systemizer,
is in the social world, in the playground, for example,
you've got kids running around,
you've got lots of people talking to you at once.
Some of the verbal
communication may not be literal, so you're trying to track the meaning of words when
someone's using sarcasm or using metaphor or other kinds of figurative language. You've
got not just focusing one-to-one, like in our conversation it's a little bit easier
because it's one to one, but imagine
if we were in the pub and there are six people talking at once and you're trying to keep
track of what does one person think about what someone else has just said or what someone
else is thinking. So it's these multiple levels of employing a theory of mind in real time
and being expected to react with a socially appropriate response.
All of that is too many moving parts.
Whereas taking apart a transistor radio and putting it back together again, or taking
apart, I'm going to use kind of a nice domestic example.
Imagine a three-year-old child who takes the toaster in the kitchen, takes it apart to understand
what happens inside when I push the lever down
and when the lever comes up and the toast pops out.
That's a child who's gonna be a future engineer,
a future systemizer.
And I think for a long time,
because I've been working in the field of autism
for 40 years, in the bad old days,
we would have tried to discourage autistic kids
and autistic people from their repetitive behavior,
sometimes called obsessional behavior.
We thought that was bad for them
because they're never gonna learn about the real world
if all they do for hours and hours
is take the toaster apart and put it back together again.
But actually that's their learning style
for many autistic kids.
And if we see that not as a bad thing,
but as a good thing,
you know, that they take a 1,000 piece jigsaw puzzle
and they want to put it together again in a complete way You know, they take a 1,000-piece jigsaw puzzle
and they want to put it together again in a complete way
to understand how every piece fits into the whole.
You hear of these anecdotes of autistic kids
who can do that, maybe not even using the picture
on the sort of the right side of the jigsaw puzzle.
They can do it face down.
So they're using the shape
and understanding how every shape locks
in a unique way with every other shape.
But they can do this very rapidly.
So it's kind of, we should focus on
the strengths of autistic people.
And that could then become their stepping stone
into flourishing in education and flourishing
in the place of work.
But if we're expecting all kids to be hyper-social, checking each other's faces, checking each
other's relationships and body language and reading between the lines, picking up hints,
autistic kids may feel, they may appear to be struggling.
Whereas if we focus on their strengths,
we can say, well, this kid's got a gift.
Let's not worry about whether the child
is making eye contact or not.
Let's just let the child learn in their own way
and see how far they can go.
And parents who recognize their child's autism
is neurodivergence,
that we don't have to expect all kids to learn the same way,
to process information in the same way,
just let the kid follow what they're interested in.
Those kids might decide
I want to do mathematics all day.
And sure enough, and they end up in university
and they're on the fast track into advanced mathematics.
Even if along the way, they haven't really managed
to learn how to write an essay with a beginning, a middle,
and an end, which communicates to a reader,
because that's not their interest.
Their interest is how numbers fit together,
just like how pieces of the jigsaw fit together.
Let's talk about the relationship between
this hyper-focus on tool use and systemizing,
as opposed to functioning in the social world,
and sex differences, and also the consequences of that in the education system.
And I want to add another dimension, too.
Just before I stopped being a university professor,
I was starting to take a look at the relationship
between your scales, systemizing and empathizing,
and the big five personality dimensions,
because empathizing sounds an awful lot like agreeableness,
right, at a trait level.
And I'm wondering if, I should know this, but I don't, if there's been some
well-conducted research assessing the relationship between these interest patterns. I mean,
there are studies of interest patterns as well, the Ryasek model, I can't remember who developed
that. I mean, there is a taxonomy of interests, right,
that's been quite well documented.
But I was curious about it temperamentally.
So let's talk about the sex differences,
the implications of that in education,
and then the temperamental issue.
Sure.
So I should have really responded to your question
about sex or gender differences earlier,
but let's bite the bullet.
So we've found over many years actually that in the general population, that's to say the non-autistic population,
there are these on average sex or gender differences, both in empathy and in systemizing.
So what we find in small scale studies
is that girls and women on average score higher
on tests of empathy and on measures of empathy,
and boys and men on average score higher on tests of systemizing and measures
of systemizing.
I say small scale because back in the day we would be testing a few hundred people,
but now you can do online research, you can really do research at scale.
So we published a study back in 2018 which was 600,000 people taking the empathy quotient,
the EQ, and the systemizing quotient, the SQ.
They did it online, so that's how we could achieve
such large sample sizes.
And we found, sure enough, replicating the smaller studies,
that women on average score higher on the EQ
and men on average score higher on the SQ. How big a difference is it in
standard deviation terms? It's a great question. It's a
significant difference. I'd need to look up the effect size and I could, you know,
contact you after the conversation, but it's a significant difference.
And especially when you have these large samples,
they jump out at you.
They were robust gender differences.
We've also used a performance test of empathy
called the eyes test.
It's also called the reading the mind in the eyes test,
where you look at photographs
of people's eyes. And the test is whether you can infer what someone might be thinking
or feeling just from looking at the emotional expression around the eyes.
Because the eyes
So that's a gestalt.
It's a gestalt. Yeah, I mean, it is. It's a, if you imagine like a letter box,
you've only got this much information,
but it turns out to be very rich in information
about another person's state of mind.
And women on average score higher on the,
it's called the reading the mind in the eyes test
compared to men.
And just a few years ago, 2022,
we published a study showing across 57 different countries.
So this was kind of independent of culture.
You see this female, excuse me,
female superiority on the test.
Are there tests that are that straightforward
on the systemizing side that show the male advantage?
There are. I mean, one quite old one, which you may remember, is called the embedded figures test,
where you have to find a target as quickly as you can that's embedded in a larger design,
and men are faster and more accurate in finding the part within the whole. So that might be a systemizing test.
We've just completed the development of another test,
which is more about mechanical reasoning,
trying to, you know, asking people whether a lever will move up or down
depending on the cogwheels that are moving in different directions.
So you're having to kind of,
it's kind of like looking inside an old fashioned watch
to understand the drivers,
the cog wheels driving movements of levers.
And males perform faster and more, you know, higher,
they score higher on average on that test.
So these are controversial areas as you know,
because the field of sex difference research
or gender differences research
understandably gets a lot of pushback.
We all want a society which is where there is gender equality.
But that's more about our social values.
It's not necessarily a scientific question
about whether male and female children and adults,
whether their minds work in the same way on average.
And I keep using those two words on average
because the best we can do as scientists
is compare groups of females and groups of males
on these different measures and see if we find a difference.
In the temperament literature,
the differences in agreeableness,
which I think are most closely related to empathizing,
they maximize in gender neutral societies.
They don't minimize, right?
So yeah, so in this case, this is not controversial research from the technical perspective because
it's been unbelievably well established.
So you can rank order countries by the degree to which they've implemented sexual, say, parity in the law and in society on sexual grounds.
And then you can look at the differences in magnitude of temperament differences between men and women.
And what you find is clearly the case that, well, for example, differences in agreeableness and neuroticism
are maximized in the Scandinavian countries.
So if you flatten out the environmental variation, you seem to maximize the genetic variation.
And I'm curious with regards to empathizing and systemizing, if you rank order, if you
did the same analysis, do you know if there's any evidence that...
It's a great question.
I haven't seen any research on it. But first of all, going back to your question about,
is empathy closest to agreeableness
if we think about the big five personality dimensions?
That makes sense to me.
And there may be some data out there
that confirms a correlation.
But agreeableness, I would say,
and you're the personality psychologist here
between the two of us, so you tell me,
but agreeableness to me involves keeping track
of how someone else is feeling
and what they might be thinking.
You and I are having a very polite conversation,
so we're being very agreeable,
probably because we don't want to insult each other because we don't want to insult each other.
We don't want to hurt each other's feelings.
So we're kind of exercising our agreeableness, but that is really what I was calling empathy
earlier on.
If you don't care about the other person's feelings, you can be as rude as you like.
Driving your point home in a debate,
you could be very blunt, you could, you know,
you could be very direct, you could be very, yeah, insulting.
There's all kinds of, you know, different ways
that you could use language, which would not be agreeable,
even if you're making a very logical point.
So, you know, I think I'd agree with you not be agreeable, even if you're making a very logical point.
So I think I'd agree with you on that kind of overlap
between these dimensions of individual differences.
Well, the most relevant element of that, I would say,
is not so much the somewhat obvious point
that agreeableness and empathizing
might be associated.
I would say what's more
interesting from a scientific and social perspective is whether low agreeableness is
associated with systemizing. Because one of the things that's peculiar about the personality and
psychopathology research is that there's a tremendous number of positive trait names and identifiers on the empathizing side,
on the agreeable side, but there's very little positive about the disagreeable side.
And that's a big mistake because it's a normal distribution for a reason,
and there's just as many reasons to be disagreeable as there are to be agreeable.
The question is, like, and you know, this
is a comic trope with regard to engineers because they tend to be somewhat parodied
as blunt and disagreeable in their prioritization of systemizing over-empathizing. You ever
watch the Big Bang Theory?
Of course.
Yes. Well, so, I mean, all of the humor in the Big Bang Theory is systemizing over-empathizing,
pretty much.
Yeah, and that character, what's the name of the main character in the Big Bang Theory?
Yeah, the gangly physicist.
Is it Steven?
Simon?
No, neither of those.
It'll come back to both of us in a second.
But some people wonder whether he's playing the role
of an autistic person, because he's fascinated by science
and the world of systems and technology,
but he may not be paying equal attention
to other people's feelings in the room.
You know, we have this phrase, read the room.
You know, autistic people and maybe some engineers
may be less focused on reading the room
because they're focused on understanding the system
and making sure it's working.
You know, right now you and I are surrounded by mechanics
and engineers making sure that the cameras are working fine,
the sound systems are working fine, and that's their job.
You know, that's their talent.
And they may be kind of less focused on,
are we both comfortable or are we communicating,
are we on the same topic?
You know, they're more kind of focusing on the engineering
of our interaction or of the situation.
But coming back to gender, just because I know that,
you know, this is again a topic of mutual interest.
The reason I say on average gender differences
is you can find individuals
who are atypical for their gender.
When we do those big studies,
we've classified the population into people
who lean more towards empathy than systemizing,
or those who lean more towards systemizing than empathy.
So those are two brain types.
We call them type E and type S.
And then there are people in the middle
who are kind of equal in their empathy
and their systemizing.
We call them type B for balanced.
And then there are people at the extremes,
people who empathize nonstop,
but may find systemizing challenging,
and vice versa, people who systemize nonstop
and may find empathy challenging.
And in that big online study,
we had 36,000 autistic people take part.
And they tend to be in the type S profile.
So leaning more towards systems than empathy,
or even in the extreme type S,
where there's a bigger discrepancy
between their systemizing and their empathy.
But back to gender, you know, you can find females, well, maybe I should kind
of back up a little bit. What we find is that more women than men are type E. It's about
40% of women and 20% of men. And more men are type S. Again, it's about 40% of males and 20% of females.
But this is already kind of telling you that
this isn't about gender.
It's about, you know, because you can't prejudge
an individual man or a woman just from their gender.
It's about the type of brain that they have.
And so kind of bringing this into politics, if you like,
it would be wrong to prejudge a female applicant in a job.
Let's say the job is an engineering job based on her gender.
Or if the job was all about communication skills and empathy,
maybe you're being interviewed to be a good psychotherapist.
Again, it would be wrong to assume
that the woman would be the better fit
for the therapist job and the man would be the better fit
for the engineering job, because it's not about your gender,
it's about what kind of brain you have.
And, you know, even if we see these on average differences between the genders, and those
are interesting, we've been very interested to see whether one of the drivers of those
on average gender differences are prenatal hormones like testosterone, which, you know, and the male fetus produces twice as much testosterone
than the female fetus, and that hormone has been demonstrated in animal studies to change
brain development.
But supposing you're a female fetus exposed to a higher level of testosterone prenatally
in the womb, you might end up with a brain that's a bit
like Temple Grandin. You know, she engineers systems even though she's
chromosomally female.
There were studies done on a cohort of androgenized girls.
They were looking, the studies that I recall were done to look at sexual preference, because the androgenized girls had more male
pattern, had a more masculine pattern of play, but it turned out that it had very little
effect on their sexual attraction. They were more masculine in their play patterns as juveniles, but it
didn't tilt them towards female attract- I don't know if that- have the adrogenized
females been studied in relationship to systemization or?
So we did a study way back where we looked at a medical condition called congenital adrenal hyperplasia.
And so these are girls who, for genetic reasons, are producing more androgens prenatally.
And their play is more masculinized in terms of their choice. They tend to choose toys that
typical boys would choose, like the Lego and the toy vehicles and so forth.
And we gave them a test of autistic traits, actually.
So we asked the parents to assess
how many autistic traits the girls with CAH,
congenital adrenal hyperplasia have,
relative to their sisters raised in the same environment
who do not have CAH.
So, you know, one group of girls were hyperandrogenized,
and the other group of girls, biologically related, were not.
And we found that the girls with CAH
had a higher number of autistic traits.
And they have been studied for their kind of interest
in objects and systems by Melissa Hines,
the person whose work I'm most familiar with.
So I don't want our listeners to think that hormones are the only factor, because obviously
children are born into a particular world where maybe girls and boys are treated differently by
their parents, they're exposed to different messages from the media, from the toy industry,
even teachers may be unconsciously treating boys and girls differently.
So social factors may well be feeding into all this, but we were quite interested to
see whether prenatal hormones or some aspect of our prenatal biology may also be playing
a role.
I'm going to touch, unfortunately we have 15 minutes left in this section. I think
for everyone watching and listening, what we're going to do on the Daily Wire side is talk about
the political ramifications of this work, but also the personal consequences of the political
ramifications, because I'm interested to hear your story about the public reception of
your work and how that's impacted your ability to do
research and what that's taught you. So I think we're going to
talk about that on the Daily Wire side. One of the things we
haven't talked about yet that constitutes a shared interest
that's relevant to what we've been discussing as well is
you've written books that are focused
on the issue of malevolence, psychopath, like psychopathy.
You wrote Zero Degrees of Empathy,
you wrote Science of Evil.
And I mean, I'm very interested in malevolence
and in cruelty and in psychopathy.
Not so much the callousness as the cruelty and the delight in cruelty.
And so I'm curious, we only have 15 minutes, but I would like to know first how that interest
relates to your other interests, but also maybe you could give us a bit of an insight
into your broad conclusions.
Yeah.
So just to first of all, just to say that our listeners are not confused,
the book, the two books that you mentioned are the very same book, but they came out under different titles. In the UK
it's called Zero Degrees of Empathy and in the US it's called The Science of Evil.
Oh, okay, okay, okay.
I wouldn't want people to rush out to the bookstore and buy both books, because they'll just discover that as soon as they open it,
that it's the same book under two different titles.
But I start that book with a personal anecdote,
which is that when I was seven years old,
my father told me about the Holocaust.
You know, I come from a Jewish family.
And for many people, the cruelty of the Holocaust. You know, I come from a Jewish family and for many people the cruelty
of the Holocaust is kind of one of the big questions about how could human beings treat
one another with such extreme cruelty. You know, and he told me about the gas chambers,
he told me about how Jews were subject to human experimentation.
And he told me that his first girlfriend's mother had her hands amputated in the concentration camp
and sewn on backwards so that her thumb was where her little finger should be and vice versa.
So the Nazi doctors were conducting experiments on people
out of a fascination with what was possible,
trying to break through new barriers in surgery, for example,
but without any regard for the patients or the inmates' subjectivity or feelings.
You know, and I got, you know, so this has been a lifelong question for me about cruelty.
And often extremes of human cruelty
are just dismissed as evil.
I say dismissed because, you know,
what you often hear in a court of law,
a criminal court, is the defendant is evil.
That's why he or she committed these acts of cruelty.
Whereas for a scientist, I don't feel the word evil
really explains what's happened.
How was the person able to do that?
Whereas I think looking at the idea of empathy
and the idea that we may either be born
with different levels of empathy,
say for genetic and neurological reasons,
or we may lose our empathy,
perhaps because of social factors, ideological factors.
You know, think about the people that flew the planes
into the Twin Towers on 9-11,
and they believed that they were doing the right thing
without much thought for the victims
that they were about to create.
So I guess that's one answer to your question,
is that I thought that if we take the idea of empathy
and individual differences in empathy,
that might give us
more explanatory power into the question of why do some people behave with cruelty and
others behave with kindness?
Why do some people rush over to help other people when they see a victim in the street?
They see an old lady has fallen down.
The empathy is the impulse to rush over and help,
to alleviate somebody else's suffering.
And other people will walk by,
the so-called bystander effect in social psychology.
They can almost switch off their empathy.
So that's one reason why I got into that and wrote that book.
But also, and I was just starting to explain, when you look across the range of disabilities
and conditions in psychiatry, whether we're talking about autism or psychopaths, empathy
seems to be a kind of, it comes up a lot.
It seems to be something that crops up a lot across people with different diagnoses.
And I really wanted to kind of explore in a single book how different aspects of empathy can result in different outcomes.
So in the case of autistic people, they seem to have very good affective empathy. They care about others,
but they have challenges in cognitive empathy. That's to do with the recognition part, you
know, interpreting or inferring what someone might be thinking or feeling. Once it's pointed
out to them, they care about another person and they want to, you know, they want to stand up for justice,
they want to help people.
The psychopath almost has the mirror opposite profile.
They have good cognitive empathy.
We talked about this a bit earlier.
That's how they can deceive others
and make you believe that something's true when it's not.
But they don't seem to care about others.
So they've got reduced
affective empathy, and that's how they're able to torture their victims, for example,
without caring.
I read a study recently that showed that there was substantial variation in periaqueductal
gray activation in people when they were observing the suffering of others.
And so, periaqueductal gray activation seems to be pretty tightly associated with the experience
of pain.
And so, part of that empathy could be something as direct as when I see you in pain, what
I experience is a pain-like response.
And you can imagine wide individual variation in that.
And so, okay, and so let me turn that a little bit,
because there's a tough question lurking here too,
which is, it seems to me likely that what you're pointing to
is the relationship between the capacity for cruelty
or callousness and lower levels of empathy now, you know
It's one thing to walk by an old lady who's fallen down on the street and not help and it's another thing to kick her
Right. And so mere lack of empathy doesn't seem to me to suffice for positive cruelty
But then I'm also curious like you've contrasted systemizing and
empathizing, and I think maybe you already answered this with regards to
your distinction between affective and cognitive empathy, but is there, so there
is evidence that lower empathy might be associated with callousness. You'd see
also that with agreeableness, by the way, that's mirrored in the trait research.
But is there an indication that systemizing is associated with more callousness, or is
that merely a matter of that cognitive empathy blindness, so to speak?
Exactly.
I think it's more of the latter.
So when we were talking earlier, I was presenting this idea as if empathy and systemizing
are two independent dimensions.
Yes.
So in the way I've kind of tried to explain this in my work,
I literally plot empathy along the Y axis
and systemizing along the X axis.
And you can find all of us fall somewhere,
all of us fall somewhere in that space,
in the four quadrants.
But we do find a small but significant inverse correlation.
There's a kind of trade-off that the higher your empathy,
the more difficulty you have on systemizing and vice versa.
Obviously it's true that you can find very empathic systemizers and very systematic empathizers,
but nevertheless, you know, the population isn't randomly distributed.
We tend to kind of, you know, we said earlier that more women tend to
be up and towards the empathy end of that, those two distributions and more men tend to be,
you know, leaning more towards the or located more towards the systemizing end of those two distributions.
I think when we think about autistic people, as I said, they don't tend to be callous.
They tend to sort of, they tend to struggle
with understanding other people.
And so, some of them even avoid other people.
They're not out to hurt others.
They may just retreat into the world,
the predictable world of objects and systems.
You know, the child who's playing with their Lego is a kind of almost a stereotype of an autistic
child. They're not actively seeking to hurt others. But what you were describing, where somebody goes
over and actually kicks the old lady, that's not just low empathy. There's some kind of pleasure
the old lady. That's not just low empathy. There's some kind of pleasure in hurting another person.
So obviously we need to factor in more than just empathy. That if you're a child, for example, who's been bullied and it makes you feel bigger and stronger to bully others, we have this phenomenon where the victim turns into a victimizer,
which has always been a paradox for psychiatry.
Why is it that kids who know what it feels like,
who know how painful it is to be on the receiving end
of child abuse, for example, when they grow up,
might abuse their own kids.
You know, and it's not deterministic,
it's not that they all do that.
In fact, most of them don't, right?
Most people who are abused as children
don't grow up to abuse their children.
Although most people who abused their children
were abused as children.
So yeah, so there are these really interesting,
important developmental sort of effects that
we need to understand.
You know, and, you know, if you do engage in cruelty like child abuse or hurting others,
is it just a lack of empathy or is it that the activity itself is giving you something?
Yeah, positive delight.
I think you can't understand the phenomenology you were describing in the concentration camps
without taking real sadism.
The dark tetrad research seems to indicate that too.
The dark tetrad was originally the dark triad, Machiavellian, psychopathic, and narcissistic.
But further investigation indicated that sadism was a necessary addition.
And sadism is best defined as positive delight in the unnecessary suffering of others, right?
So there's a kick there that's not merely like inability to experience like direct physiological empathy.
Your pain is my pain.
Yeah, there's this word called schadenfreude,
I'm sure you know.
Taking pleasure in someone else's misfortune.
So it's not just a kind of lack of empathy or not caring.
It's actually sort of, you know,
that someone else's misfortune
actually makes you feel bigger and stronger. In the story of Cain and Abel, Cain becomes murderous, obviously,
and then he's the father of the genocidal mob, essentially. That's how the story develops.
But there's a very interesting interchange between him and God right at the beginning of the story,
because Cain is complaining
bitterly to God that his sacrifices aren't being accepted and that he's a failure.
And God says two things to him.
He says, if you did well, you'd be accepted.
So implying that Cain isn't exactly bringing his best to the table.
But then he also tells him that his theory of his misery is wrong.
God says, you think you're miserable because you've been rejected and you failed,
but actually what happened is that you were rejected and you failed, and you didn't learn from it.
And then something came along to tempt you, that sin that crouches at the door, and you invited it in, right, to have its
way with you. God uses the analogy of a sexually aroused predator and tells Cain that he invited
this spirit in and that that's what's possessing him. Right, it's a very sophisticated psychological
analysis because it's not merely, let's say, a deficit in empathy.
There's another factor which is,
it's something approximating a decision
or a series of decisions to revel in suffering.
And that's not something psychologists
have done a good job of explaining.
No, but I mean, you've reached to the Bible
as one set of stories to try and understand this,
but we could reach to a much closer example,
closer to home, which is sibling rivalry.
Because sibling rivalry was described
as something that's quite normative.
You know, if you have kids, as parents, it's one of the most heartbreaking things, is to
see two of your kids fighting each other or arguing with each other, you know, when we
just want our kids to love each other.
Right, right.
Competition for status and attention.
Well, that's part of the Cain and Abel story too, of course, because that's a sibling rivalry
story.
Absolutely. So, you know, whether they're competing for resources or competing for parental
attention, you know, there may be something more than empathy that's at play in terms
of what's driving our behavior.
Yeah.
That we feel better if we're maybe higher status or feel a bit more powerful.
Yeah, well, it does look like something like a turn to power as an adaptive strategy, right?
A turn to power and domination rather than reciprocity and cooperation.
But if we go back to those doctors in the concentration camps performing medical experiments. Mengele being maybe the prime example
where he was doing experiments on twins.
He was interested in the biology of twins
and trying to understand,
can we do experiments on twins,
children, Jewish twins that were brought in as prisoners?
Can we do experiments where we could remove the organ
from this child and see what happens,
given that they're genetically identical?
You know, that wasn't sadism in the sense
or he wasn't necessarily getting pleasure out of it.
He was just pursuing scientific curiosity,
but where the Nazis had said,
it's okay to treat people as subhuman. but where the Nazis had said,
it's okay to treat people as subhuman.
It's okay to treat this group of people
as if their subjectivity, it doesn't matter.
Just like in many labs, animal research is conducted.
It's almost as if we say, well,
it's okay to experiment on a rat or on a mouse.
Maybe we'll anesthetize them, but we're still going to sacrifice them for the sake of our
scientific question.
And under the Nazi regime, Jews and other minorities were treated as subhumans, almost
giving a license or permission to treat other people as if they were objects.
Right, right. Well, that seems, and I guess maybe we'll close with that, that seems at
least in part like a move to subordinate the empathic to the systemizing, right, is that
you can treat people. one of the contributing factors
to the kind of evil that you described is the possibility of treating people as if they're
no more than cogs in a machine or parts of a, inanimate parts of a system.
And I know that we're kind of running out of time, so I'll be brief on this point. But if we look at that period from 1933
when Hitler got into power, through to when
the concentration camps were in full swing
in the early 1940s, they were incremental changes.
Where step by step, Jews and other minorities
were being deprived of their civil rights, their human
rights, you know, being treated as if they were people without feelings and didn't, you
know, didn't warrant equal status in society.
And if that happens, that's a kind of shift in our political climate. And it's very easy for these social changes
and political changes to erode empathy.
We can start treating refugees as if they are less than,
or we can treat other minorities as if they are inferior,
if it's legitimized by, for example, the government.
So many ethical conundrums are rectified
by striking the right harmonious balance.
And I guess that's what we've concluded
with regards to systemizing and empathizing.
So for everybody watching and listening,
we're going to continue our discussion
on the daily wire side for another half an hour.
And I'm going to talk to Simon about his more the political ramifications and repercussions
of his research and what all that also means for the state of investigative science, all
things considered, especially on the clinical end.
So it would be good if those of you who are inclined would join us on the
daily wire side for that conversation. Thank you very much for agreeing to talk to me today. It's
been a pleasure. I could talk to you for much longer and delve into more of the details about
our differences and similarities in understanding and interpretation. I'd sure love to see that
big five research done on systemizing in particular and agreeableness
because it's a burning question.
So at least for a personality theorist
and I don't have a lab anymore, unfortunately.
So.
We'll, first of all, I want to thank you
because the conversation I've really enjoyed it.
I'm looking forward to the next segment after the break.
All right, good.
And thank you everybody for your time and attention.