The Jordan B. Peterson Podcast - Dr. Richard Haier: The Neuroscience of Intelligence
Episode Date: June 4, 2018There is almost nothing more important to understand about people than intelligence. It can be measured more accurately than anything else in the social sciences. It differs tremendously and important...ly between individuals. It is the single most important determinant of life success. It's very existence, however, remains subject to substantive debate, most of it highly politicized. Dr. Richard Haier has recently written a major book on the topic,
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Welcome to the Jordan B. Peterson Podcast.
You can support these podcasts by donating to Dr. Peterson's Patreon, the link to which
can be found in the description.
Dr. Peterson's self-develop neuro basis of human intelligence and cognition.
He works with neuroimaging technologies
to study individual differences in mental ability.
He received his PhD in psychology from the Johns Hopkins University in 1975,
and his since-held appointments in the Intermural Research Program
at the National Institute of Mental Health
and a medical school at Brown University and University of California at Irvin.
Irvine, he has served on the editorial board of three journals, NeuroImage, Intelligence
and Psychiatry Research.
He also served as guest editor for a special issue on brain imaging research for the
journal Intelligence.
He provides neuroscience consultation to university research groups, corporations, foundations,
and educational and legal professionals. He's a popular lecturer
and has appeared numerous media outlets. In 2012, his research was featured on NOVA Science
Now, and he received the Distinguished Contributive Award for the International Society for
Intelligence Research. In 2013, the teaching company invited Richard to create an 18-lector
course called the intelligent
brain.
So welcome to Dr. Richard Hire today and we're going to talk about the most controversy
of all topics I would say in social science, strangely enough, intelligence.
So maybe we could start with a little bit of historical information.
I would like to know how you got interested
in IQ research or an intelligence research, let's say. And so let's start with that and then
we can start diving into the nitty-gritty. Well really, it started in graduate school
at Hopkins when I really became most interested in personality research.
And I started out studying individual differences
in personality, but just by happenstance,
the year I started graduate school in 1971
was the year one of the professors there,
Julian Stanley, was starting to study
of mathematically-procoseous youth.
And I was one of the
proctors at the very first
talent search for mathematically
percosious kids.
And I wrote my first couple of
papers as book chapters in
books that still was
editing about this project.
And I saw these kids,
age 10, 11, 12, who were scoring higher on SAT math than
Hopkins freshman. And the question was, you know, how does this happen? Where does this
come from? So now it's kind of my earliest interest. And in graduate school, although I really completed my dissertation on personality, I took my first
job at the National Institute of Mental Health in the Inter-Rural Research Program in the laboratory
of psychology and psychopathology, which at the time, the lab director was David Rosenthal who had just finished the Denmark adoption studies of schizophrenia.
Right, right. And here is where I learned about genetics. My office was next
stored to a fellow named Monty Bucksbaum who was doing both the official research. research, and I'm very interested in that. And so my early interest in individual differences
slowly morphed into an interest in individual differences in intelligence. And at NIMH,
they were just going through a transition from kind of a psychoanalytic orientation to a neuroscience orientation.
And I was kind of caught up in that.
And so that's the origin of my interest in the brain and in technologies to make brain measurements
and relate that to individual differences.
Okay, right.
Okay, now you just wrote a book too in the neuroscience of intelligence.
I did.
I did.
I did.
University Press.
And so when did that come out?
That came out really just about six months ago.
So it came out I think in December of 2016, but they tell me for publishing reasons, they
call it a 2017 publication.
I see. Well, I part of the reason I was so excited to talk to you is that I've done a for publishing reasons, they call it a 2017 publication.
I see.
Part of the reason I was so excited to talk to you
is that I've done a very large amount of research,
especially not so much practical lab research
but investigation into the structure of intelligence
and into its measurement.
We designed back in 93 with a student of mine, Daniel Higgins.
We designed, I think what was probably
the first online battery, purporting to measure the cognitive abilities associated with
dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex, right?
So hypothetically the highest order cognitive functions in the brain, and we found much
to our chagrin, I would say, and this was a very painful discovery that a lot of what
we have been thinking about as potentially
separable neuropsychological functions were pretty easily collapsible into good old general
intelligence, you know, that killer central factor that seems to unite cognitive abilities.
And so it was quite a shock, especially because the neuropsychologists of the time, and they still do this, are as as a siguous investigators of the
psychometric intelligence literature as they should be,
and tend to underestimate the central power
of that initial factor.
So anyways, I'm really interested
in intelligence research partly
from a practical perspective too,
because the industrial organizational psychology
literature is crystal clear for complex jobs, the best predictor of long-term success as intelligence.
And it's a predictor that's probably, say imagine you give it an r of 0.4 to 0.5,
which is pretty decent. So let's say 25% of the variance. The next best predictor is conscientiousness.
And it's pushing its limit at more than up 10% of the variability in long-term performance
So IQ it's a killer man, and it we I make sure my students assess intelligence with everything they do and it always
Ends up being a major predictor of things that you wouldn't even expect like discussed sensitivity for example
So your discussed sensitivity is higher if you have a lower IQ
for example, so your disgust sensitivity is higher if you have a lower IQ. The G factor is powerful. You just said something though that I want to just
make a distinction about. You're talking about the G factor and then you kind of
called it IQ. This is very common in every language to talk about intelligence,
IQ and what we call the G-factor as one thing,
and it really isn't. So an IQ score is a good estimate of the G-factor, right? Also includes
other aspects of intelligence. And intelligence itself, although it's a broad term, is only
draw a certain is only part of the universe of mental abilities. So if you're very good at calculating on what day January 5th was in the year 1520, that's a mental ability
that some people have, doesn't mean you're smart.
Right.
Let's see that with autistic savants often, and that they're not often, but sufficiently
often they have these amazing calculations abilities, for example, that don't seem to
be manifest in a spectacularly high overall intelligence.
So what do you want to tell us about?
You want to start with the book and walk us through it?
Well, you know, that's interesting because the book is kind of a culmination
of things I've learned mostly from my neuroimaging work on intelligence. And it kind of came
as a surprise, it's the first book I've ever written, I would tire it actually, from
academia. Never wrote a book while I was in academia. I was writing
journal papers, but Cambridge University Press called and they have this series of fundamentals
of neuroscience. And they wanted to include intelligence. And I regarded that as a major
step because intelligence research has been relegated almost to the
peripheral of mainstream psychology. And the politically suspect to say the
least. Yeah, and you know, the switch happened overnight from being in the mainstream to being really peripheral in about 1969, before 1969, almost everyone
who was interested in education was concerned about the achievement gaps.
And they felt universally that once you equalized educational opportunities, those achievement gaps would
disappear.
Yeah, that was the head start, the head start point.
Yeah, even before head start, head start, it came to head start, but even before head start,
there were all these demonstration projects, there was one, the miracle and Milwaukee, and
there were all these things that showed that if you really intervened in early childhood education,
which at that time was called compensatory education.
The early childhood education term came much later.
But this idea of compensatory education really took off. And then in 1969, the Harvard Education Review asked one of the foremost educational psychologists,
Arthur Jensen, to write a review of the progress.
And this article in 1969 has become infamous.
The opening sentence was essentially we've tried compensatory education and it has failed.
Yeah.
And then you had a hundred pages of detailed statistical analysis of why there were no,
you couldn't demonstrate an increase in IQ score.
Yeah.
And any of these programs.
Now head start had had just begun.
So head start wasn't included.
Yeah, but I, you know, I reviewed the literature on head start too extensively and basically what happened was that the
So that was for those of the viewers who don't know head start was a
Nationwide attempt to to add additional education to the lives of
disadvantaged kids, especially you know at the preschool level and
Basically what happens what was that they actually did show
improvements in academic achievement initially.
So in grade one and grade two, they were performing
above their peers, but then the difference in improvement,
the difference in performance started to decrease.
And then by about the grade five or grade six,
the differences had disappeared completely.
So there was no evident whatsoever of that, either that either of a stable, one-time long-term gain
in cognitive ability, or what people were really hoping
was that if you intervened early enough,
you'd get something that would sort of
would turn into a positive feedback loop,
and the gains would actually advance across time.
And what ended up happening with the Head Start research
basically was the conclusion that it produced no cognitive improvements whatsoever. Although more kids who went through
Head Start graduated from high school, fewer than were delinquent, more of them became pregnant
in teenage years, and more of them went to colleges. But that seemed to be because they were
better socialized, not because they were in any way had been made smarter. So that was a really tremendous disappointment because it was a bipartisan attempt to come
to grips with the fundamental issues that sort of be devolved structural poverty in the
United States.
No one was happy about that outcome, I can tell you.
Well, not only that, but when Jensen published his article, he also said that since IQ increases seem not to be coming from
these intense environmental interventions, we should consider the possibility that these
differences have a genetic component.
And that really began the incendiary descent of intelligence research to the periphery.
To reaction against that was universal because it imploded inferiority if you didn't have genes
where I could.
It's also something that's universally hated on both sides of the political spectrum,
because on the liberal end, you know, the idea fundamentally is that everybody's the same and that if you
distribute education resources properly, then everyone can succeed. And so that
didn't work out so well for liberals. And then on the conservative side, the idea
is, well, if you could just get off your lazy asses and get a job, there is a job
for you out there. And the truth of the matter is, you know, you can tell me what you think about this,
but this was a statistic that just absolutely shocked and staggered me when I went through
the intelligence literature.
So, you know, it is illegal in the United States to induct anybody who has an IQ of less than
83.
And the reason for that is, you know, the American Armed Forces have been conducting
intelligence research for like more than 100 years.
And that was partly because they needed a way of sorting people rapidly during times of
military expansion during more time.
But it was also because IQ tests, and especially in the early part of the 20th century, were
used to identify, let's say, the deserving poor who could really benefit from additional
educational attainment and advancement
in the unit and the military was hoping to identify people from more class strata that
could be streamed into say officer training programs and so forth or even skills training
programs to move people from the underclass and to at least the working class and maybe
above.
So they had a bloody stake in this, man. They wanted to find people, they wanted to sort them properly, and they wanted
to do social good when they weren't just trying to win a war, let's say, which often also
is a social good. But what happened was that by, I don't remember when this legislation
was introduced, but it wasn't, it was in the later part of the 20th century. But their
basic finding was that by, say, the 1980s,
they had determined that if you had an IQ of less than 83,
there was not a damn thing that the Army could do.
The Armed Forces could do to transform you into someone
who could do something that was more productive
than non-productive.
And the territorial thing about that is that it's about 10%
of the population.
And so, you look at a statistic like that and you think, oh my God, you've got this
enterprise, this massive enterprise that's chronically hungry for people.
It's always, they're always looking for people.
They're really oriented towards taking people from the underclass and lower working class
and pushing them up the societal strata.
And during wartime, they're actually desperate to bring in recruits period, and their conclusion
is that 10% of the population can't be trained to do anything sufficiently useful to make
them militarily operable.
It's just, I just read that.
My judge has dropped.
It's like, well, you know, in the United States,
we have about 330 million people. And because of the distribution, the relative and
normal distribution of IQ scores, about 16 percent have IQs of 85 or less.
Right. Right.
It means they're not going to graduate school.
You know, it means that from what I've read practically,
it means the Wonderland company has actually done a really,
they have a nice IQ test from the commercial perspective.
You know, it's actually psychometrically valid.
And they've linked IQ levels to job,
specifically to job categories, you know?
Yes, I know.
And what I was gonna say is they're not only
not going to graduate school, they're
not going to find a stable job that pays a livable wage.
Yeah, especially even given that so many of the service jobs now require a fair, high degree
of computational savvy or a lead-a-built, no, it's not computational, but ability to interact
with complex computational technology
of the typical till at a checkout market or the till at a McDonald's because McDonald's
is actually very complicated is often far beyond the ability of people who are on the
low end of the intelligence distribution and they claimed I think it was Wonderlick although
it might have been it might have been Hunt, what's his name?
Is it Earl Hunt, I think, possibly?
Well, he claimed that if you have an IQ below 90, it's difficult for you to read well
enough to translate what you're reading into action.
So you can't actually read instructions and follow them.
You don't have that level of literacy.
That's correct.
Yeah.
So I was gonna say that in the United States,
this bottom 16% translates into 51 million people,
including 13 million children who are in school.
Right.
This is a very difficult problem.
Now I knew where I was going to have to weigh what I asked you.
You were pretty well.
He also would say that there is this cognitive segregation
in society.
This is a point that Charles Murray makes.
Yeah.
Well, and Earl would often ask,
once the last time you had someone
over for dinner who wasn't a college grad. Well, that was something that Murray and
Herndtstein wrote about in their book, The Bill Curve, which really struck me because
I read that book twice, unlike most of the people who criticized it. And, you know, one
of the things that they pointed out in there, it was, look, the typical educated person thinks
that someone isn't very bright if they have an IQ of 115.
So we're not going to go through a graduate level with PhD level research institutions,
right? Because 115, there's as many people at 115 above as there are at 85 and below.
Right.
And so it's a minority of the population.
And that's the top 15%.
And that's the dollar undergraduate.
Right.
So how do you just, people have, see,
I'm a clinical psychologist and I've dealt with people
who had arranges in the low 80s and tried to find them
jobs and tried to train them.
And I have some real knowledge about the stunning gap
between people at the low end of the IQ
distribution in the high end.
It's no bloody wonder people hate IQ research and intelligence research because it reveals
a set of seriously dismal facts about the incredible range of ability among human beings. Well, yes, this is true and moreover, I would add to this, that people in universities, professors
and a couple of graduate students, have a hard time understanding what everyday life
is like if you have an IQ of 80 or 85.
And you're making your way, you're living independently, you're
making your way in the world. But it is a challenge. It is a really, I mean, just just barely
begins to describe it. I had a, I had a client who he probably had an IQ of under 80, the nonverbal
portion of it anyways.
He was indistinguishable in physical appearance from, let's say, I hate to use the frame normal
person, but there was nothing marked him out about particularly intellectually impaired.
And I tried at one point, this was so telling to me, I got him a volunteer job, which by the way is very difficult.
It's harder to get a volunteer job than a real job because you have to do police screening
and all sorts of things.
And the selection process is just as extreme.
But I eventually ended up getting him a job at a bike store, a bike slash bookstore.
And at that place couldn't hold him once the subsidy program had expired. And then I got him a job at a charity, and his job was to fold letters into three
so that they could be put into envelopes.
Well, that sounds easy, except that he also had a bit of a motor tremor.
And, you know, it took me about 30 hours to train him to fold up a piece of paper
with sufficient precision so that it can be put
in an envelope rapidly so that the envelope wasn't so mangled
that it would get stuck in the automatic sorting machine.
And in other words, high performance demands on him too.
He had to whip through those letters pretty quickly.
And then sometimes the letters would have a photograph
appended to them that was stapled on.
And they weren't always stapled on in the same place. So then he had to calculate how to fold the paper over the photograph without bending the photograph in precise third so there would still fit in the envelope.
And then he had to separate the French letters from the English letters and associate them with the proper envelopes and like that level of complexity
just did him in. Let me say two things about this. One is I hope common sense and the other
is pretty provocative. The common sense thing is we have to be very careful when we have these
discussions, not to devalue the human dignity of people who aren't in the upper end of the distribution.
And if there's one criticism that I think is fair, sometimes in these conversations,
it sounds like we're devaluing people at the lower end of the distribution. And we
have to be very careful that we don't do that. Human life has dignity, and IQ is not the most important thing
that defines human beings.
Even if it's not associated with wisdom,
it's not necessarily associated with truth,
or with courage, or with many virtues
that are being extensible.
How many?
You've been delighted at all with being likable.
They're honest.
Yeah, that's right.
I'm ready.
But we know the psychometric relationship
between intelligence and conscientiousness is zero.
Right.
So after we have to make that point.
Yes, I agree.
I agree.
I'm trying to make the point about how difficult it is
for people who are on the low end of the cognitive spectrum
to survive in an increasingly complex,
cognitively
sophisticated environment.
Right.
Jobs are just disappearing.
Yes, absolutely.
And now, let's ask the question, is there anything that could be done about that?
Well, Western society has tried very hard with a number of environmental, environmentally-based
interventions, early childhood education.
By the way, you said the literature in organizational psychology is very clear.
The literature is equally clear in educational psychology.
Oh, yeah.
Well, the relationship between IQ and learning has been more powerful than the relationship
between IQ job performance.
That's right, which is kind of common sense,
or matches our common sense.
But if you put a bunch of variables
into a regression equation to predict academic achievement,
and you have all these school quality variables
and teacher quality variables and cognitive variables
of the students, and what you find
is the teacher variables and the quality of the school variables together barely account
for 10% of the variance.
Yeah, I know, I know, it's terrible.
Well, you know, and I talked to the guy who ran admissions at Harvard.
I taught at Harvard for a while.
His name was Dean Whitlaw.
And he was a really smart guy.
I really smart guy.
I really liked Dean.
And you know, he was, let's say, he was on the right side
of the human race.
And he was really trying to figure out how
to run the admissions policy at Harvard
so that it did the best for everyone concerned.
And he had run an interesting series of analysis
that I don't believe he ever published.
And one of them was, well, let's say
you segregate the Harvard
population into the relatively low IQ kids.
So maybe they only have an IQ of 130, you know,
and the relatively high IQ kids who are pushing up towards
to 160.
So you are two competing hypotheses there.
One would be that the lower IQ kids come to Harvard,
this remarkable environment, and they thrive because of the
high educational quality so well that they close the gap between them and the 160 kids.
Completely wrong, what happens is you put both those groups there, both very, very highly
selected, but in this sort of superman
rage intellectually, what happens is the gap just gets bigger and bigger as they progress
through university.
And it's a dreaded example of that Matthew principle that the economists talk about, which
is, you know, to those who have more will be given, and from those who have nothing, everything
will be taken. It's very, very, it's no wonder people dislike this research.
It's so anti-egalitarian in its essential structure.
Well, I wanted to make a second point that I said would be provocative.
If you want to do something about this, you know, we tried a bunch of interventions, earnest, well-funded, long-term interventions
don't seem to work, but neuroscience has been excluded from discussions about what to
do about this. And I believe that the progress in neuroscience research has the potential to really dramatically increase the G factor.
Well, that's an optimistic statement, so I'm sure looking forward to some support for that one.
It's optimistic and traversal, and you know, just as a thought experiment, and I can tell you why I believe this is possible. Well, first before I tell you the thought experiment,
the reason I'm optimistic is that it is because of the high heritability of the G factor. That means,
you know, with genes are involved, genes work through biology, even if environment interacts with that.
But basically you have a neurobiological system.
It's complex.
But as you begin to understand it, you can tweak it.
This is what all medicine is doing now.
They're trying to understand the neurobiology
slash genetic basis of our health and our diseases.
Why?
So they can fix it.
So when you go to the doctor, you're going because
your biology is broken and you let your biology fix. Well, let's think about the brain.
Now no one conceptualizes low IQ as a disease and it's a little dangerous, but to the extent
to which low IQ has a genetic input or a genetic influence, that's the extent to which you might be able to find out how that works I mean that's a plausible sequence of events.
What the problem is, it's a very complex sequence.
Sure.
But I also think it's a finite set of problems, not an infinite set of problems.
If physicists can figure out what happened during the first nanoseconds of the big bang.
We can certainly figure out what the neurobiology of intellect is and how to tweak it.
So I think that's possible.
So now let's do a thought experiment.
And let's imagine there's an IQ pill, and I mean that metaphorically, not literally a pill
you could take, like putting, you know, a Florida, but like having a Florida-dated water
just kind of raises the dental health of everybody.
It would be nice if we had that for IQ, but just imagine what it would be like if we shifted the distribution of IQ 15 points into the high end so that now
the average IQ, and I understand how IQ is computed and learning and everything, but the
point is that no one would have an IQ less than a hundred.
Right.
What would the world be like if everyone could reason sufficiently to get a reasonable job.
You know, that's a good question.
I mean, what would you call the perverse part of me?
As funny, I was just talking to one of my graduate students.
We'd be looking at the determinants of male attractiveness by the personality of female
viewers.
Okay.
Okay.
And so what we found is that there are some personality effects.
So extroverted, enthusiastic women tend to rate men, generally speaking, as more attractive
than introverted and less enthusiastic women.
And so there are some just straight personality effects,
but the biggest effect by far we found
was the proclivity of women in general to rate men
as less attractive as the women's IQ increased.
And so the other thing that we don't know
is what price we pay for accelerated IQ
from a broader perspective, you know,
because I know that there is some evidence,
and you can tell me what you think about this, because I know that there's reasonable evidence that
the average IQ of the Ashkenazi Jewish population is about 15 points higher than the standard population,
which kind of makes it a thought experiment, or an experiment, real life experiment,
it's equivalent to the one that you laid out. But Oskinawzi Jews also tend to suffer from a host of neurological diseases that seem to be
associated with increased neuroplasticity. And so to me, it's often, it's often hard to gain on
one front without losing on another, you know, I mean, that's the evolutionary conundrum, obviously,
but we call that the social justice theory. If you start one thing, you have to be bad on everything else to kind of balance it out.
Yeah, well, generally speaking, you do pay a price for your exceptionalism, you know.
I don't want that. That's true because the Julian Stanley studies of the math,
math, we per-cocious kids essentially found not only were they smart, but they were more mature
than their age peers. Better looking, they were taller smart but they were more mature than their age peers
and better looking, they were taller, they were physically more fit. I mean, it was kind of the anti-associated.
Yeah, no, no, that's true. Well, I mean, that also might be true because one of the things that
can interfere with IQ is poor health and poor nutritional quality and all of that. I mean,
it doesn't look like it's that easy to increase IQ, but it looks like it's pretty easy to decrease it.
Yeah, I think those things have to be pretty extreme to have an effect.
Those effects may not be permanent, actually.
There are some studies of deprivation of people who suffered deprivation during the
Second World War that suggest that those really severe deprivation didn't have lasting
effects on it.
Well, people are pretty tough, so I'm inclined to agree with that.
So have you seen any animal experiment, experimental work that you regard as compelling that shows something like the transformation
of animal cognition into something
that's higher order, that you regard as compelling?
Not sure what you mean,
but there are certainly animal work
that shows you can extract a G factor
from cognitive tests given to various animals.
Right, so what I was wondering is it has there been any evidence that you regard as credible
showing that that could be that that so-called animal G factor extracted in the same? We should
tell our readers to the way you extract the G factor. I'm going to say it very, very rapidly. Imagine you take a randomly selected set of 200 questions that require abstraction of one
form or another to solve.
And then you give those 200 questions to 100 people and you sum the scores and you rank
order them.
You get something that's roughly equivalent there to a G factor.
It's roughly that.
So just I would say it a little bit differently just for every time the same page. If you
think of all the different mental abilities and you devise a test for each one of them
and you gave this test to a lot of people across the range of ability, what you'll find is the scores on all those tests
are positively correlated with each other,
suggesting that all tests of mental ability
have something in common.
Right, sure, that's a great way to put it in.
And is this G factor, this general ability to reason.
And some individual tests have more G loading
than other individual tests have more G-Loading than other individual tests.
Right.
And tests of abstract reasoning tend to be highly G-Loaded.
Right.
If you said to you, repeat the following numbers back to me.
Three, seven, two, one, six, five.
That's not a very highly G-Loed mental ability to be able to do that.
But if I gave you a string like that and said,
repeat them to me backwards,
that becomes a g-loaded ability because you do a transformation.
So the other thing to say about that too
is that the positive relationship between those multiple
assessments that you described is actually quite high.
Right.
That's the thing is that that general factor not only exists across domains of cognitive
ability, but it tends to account for a substantial amount of the ability in each of those domains.
Right.
It's kind of like a G is kind of like a black hole for intelligence research and everything keeps falling
into it.
That's an interesting way to put it because now we have these genome-wide association studies
that are finding these bits of DNA that are related to a latent factor of intelligence,
which is the G factor, or to what they call educational attainment variables,
so attainment is so highly correlated with IQ, that's essentially the same thing.
So really moving, when I was in graduate school, the question was, is there a genetic component
to intelligence or not to this kind of DNA analysis, trying to find bits of DNA that are going
to be related to what we call intelligence or IQ testing or the G factor, and they seem
to exist, there seems to be hundreds of them, in which a tiny effect Which will make the ultimate story extremely complicated, but as I said before I think it's a finite set of problems
right right and at the end of that
Sequence of solving those problems. I think there's a good chance. We'll know how to increase IQ
And I think it's a good thing to be able to do that.
You know, I had said publicly that more intelligence is better than less.
Sometimes I get criticized because that implies that people with less intelligence aren't
as worthwhile. That's why I want to be very careful that I don't believe that.
Yes, that the harder life is more the accurate way of thinking about it.
And there are lives in a narrow range of possibilities and opportunities.
That's right.
And in my view, my political bias is, therefore, governments have a moral responsibility
to help those people.
And a lot of government programs aren't going to do it because job training requires a certain level of G.
My low IQ clients, they used to go to the government agencies
that were designed to help people find employment.
And the typical response was,
well, just go home and type up your CV and distribute.
It's like, you just, like, I can't use a computer. I can't
type. I don't know what a CV is. It's like it's a non-starter and all three counts. That's right.
That's right. So in the United States, there are 51 million people with IQs under 85, and there
are about 43 million people living in poverty. Do you think those Venn diagrams intersect?
Yeah. You know, we should also be clear about this because it is so politically
suspect is that it's not like it's it's it's it's it's it's self-evident that
people who have less cognitive capability are likely to end up poor because
they're serious complex problems in life that
beset them, that they have a difficult time dealing with,
and they can't learn as quickly.
And so, the relationship between poverty and intelligence is self-evident,
if you're willing to think it through for any length of time.
It doesn't mean that everybody who's rich, that it doesn't mean that everyone
who is rich is smart, and it doesn't mean that everyone who is poor is stupid to be blunt,
but what it does mean is that if you're intelligent, you're much more likely to become financially
successful. I think it was the hernstein and Murray, I think, that the calculations back
in the bell curve, that indicated that if you imagine
that you could, you were a fairy godmother and you have a, your newborn grandchild in front
of you and you can grant them three standard deviations above the mean in terms of wealth
at birth or you can grant them three standard deviations above the mean in terms of IQ at birth
and then you
wanted to determine which would work better for them by the time they were 40
and the answer to that was quite clear is that IQ trumps wealth.
If it's available to predict a positive future.
So yes.
Yeah. And that's why I'm so interested in the concept of increasing IQ or
increasing the G factor, not just the IQ
score, but really what under this reasoning ability.
So some people have tried to teach college students critical thinking.
I think that's a good thing.
It is.
It is.
You can think critically so much the better.
Exactly.
You know, and you know, it may sound to your
listeners. I just want to take a moment out here. It may sound to your listeners like,
here are these two guys pontificating about what it's like to be smart, what it's like
to be not so smart. I mean, the point of this, the point of neuroscience research on intelligence,
and what I hope to achieve by writing the book was to show that the genetic aspects are
not deterministic.
It's the opposite.
So probabilistic.
So the extent to which something like intelligence is genetic, in my view, is the extent to which
we'll learn how to change it for the better.
Well, that's definitely, yeah, because people do tend to think about
biological factors as deterministic, and that's a mistake because they can be shifted.
So with regards to the animal study, so you pointed out that you can come up with an IQ-like
estimate for, say, a rat.
And have you seen anything that indicates, I don't care how it's done through training or neurochemically or by promoting
brain function in different ways however you might do it that's actually indicated to you that
there is a way of biologically enhancing the general cognitive ability even of an animal.
Has anything come out credible? Just by breeding mice who run amaze faster than other mice. Together you get
mice that seem to be able to learn how to run amaze faster. But that's a work
from the 1980s. Right. I review this in the book. There are some interesting
technologies that have been developed where you can turn parts of a rat brain on and off
at will and see what happens.
This has not yet been applied as far as I know to learning.
But interestingly, there are human studies underway with things like trans cranial magnetic stimulation and the electrical
ways to low voltage ways to stimulate parts of the brain.
And there are some interesting experiments now being done with humans to see if you can
improve learning or reasoning ability.
Yeah.
As a matter of fact, you know, I edit this journal called
Intelligence, which is kind of a prime spot for intelligence
researchers to publish on all aspects of intelligence.
And we're just starting to put together a special issue
on human experiments that increase reasoning ability,
using these techniques of stimulating the brain.
Are any of you seen some positive things with regards to low level electrical stimulation?
Because that's also been used, you know, it's more it's more anecdotal, but there's
a bit of research being used to treat depression, for example.
That's right.
And I do cover in the book the studies that were published up until the time I wrote
the book and cautioned people
that they hadn't been replicated yet.
Right, right, right.
We're on the way.
I mean, this is an evolving area that's going to be very exciting.
I mean, if students are listening to this podcast and they're thinking about neuroscience
or psychology, this kind of experiment is really a new phase
of intelligence research where you can do experiments
on human beings that are completely ethical
and relatively non-intrusive.
And this is going to really going to change everything now.
It could be because it'll shift intelligence research
from basically psychometric correlations.
Yeah.
Past what neuroimaging has done, which really moved it away
from just psychometrics.
And then correlating psychometric scores
with measurable aspects of the brain,
like glucose metabolic function, or measurable aspects of the brain, like glucose metabolic function
or the amount of gray matter or white matter or the number of white matter fibers.
I mean, all these fabulous connections.
Great, great.
That was a new phase.
I got in early on that phase, but that phase is now moving into this new phase of actually stimulating the brain to improve learning and memory and
reasoning and all the while doing it with neuroimaging.
Great.
See what happens and adding DNA to it.
I mean, come on now.
This is a great time to be entering the research in this area.
Right, right.
Well, there's some optimism on the horizon.
I mean, I looked for a while because I'd be very interested in improving human performance,
measuring it and improving it.
And so I look and doing that also in conjunction with businesses because I like things to have
a practical end.
And, you know, I looked to find out what the research indicated with regards to improvement of intelligence.
And mostly what I found was not so much improvement as conservation is that if you exercise both aerobically and with weightlifting,
that that can help you maintain your fluid intelligence for longer across your lifespan because it tends to decline
rather precipitously as you age, which is one of the more dismal things that you also discover
with IQ research and starts to decline when you're in your early 20s.
And it's kind of linear downhill all the way along, but exercise really helps.
But it's preservation, which is pretty important at my age.
Yeah, it's, yeah, preservation is a big deal, man is pretty important at my age.
Yeah, preservation is a big deal, man, but enhancement would be good.
But you know, this thing about an IQ pill coming up with a way to manipulate the neurobiology
of your brain regarding intellect.
If there's a breakthrough in this, it will come either from Alzheimer's research
or from a formal aging research, trying to prevent the slow decline of your mental faculties
as you age, especially through intelligence, or trying to reverse the ravages of Alzheimer's
disease. These are neurochemical problems.
Right.
You know, and-
Well, an IQ seems to be quite tightly, tightly linked to,
well, this is another thing we could talk about,
because so, I know that there are certain biological markers
that IQ is loosely associated with,
so, you know, it's, it's fragment,
it predicts in a fragmentary matter.
So I know that even something as simple as simple reaction time, how fast you can push
a button when the light comes on, is correlated with IQ with fluid intelligence at about point
two. And having a bigger head is slightly correlated, especially when you correct for body size.
And so is brain mass mass and so is thickness of
the myelin sheaths on individual neurons and so there these there's these micro markers of
You might think about them as neurological integrity that seem to predict IQ
But you've been doing neuroimaging and I'm not as up on that
I haven't looked at that for a couple of years. So what have the neuroimagers
found about brain structure and function in relationship to
intelligence that you think is compelling and interesting?
Glad you asked this because as I was finishing the manuscript for the book, literally the
day after I turned it in, I had to ask for it back because it was this very interesting study published by a group at Yale that used a fairly sophisticated way to look at white matter connections, functional,
structural white matter connections and functional connections in the brain. And determining how one brain area is functionally or structurally related to all other brain
areas.
And you can put a map of a person's brain that shows from brain imaging, from MRI technology,
how their brain is interconnected.
In this paper said, these interconnections
are so reliable within a person
that they're like fingerprints.
And not only that, but the fingerprints can predict IQ.
And so- Is it density of connections, density of interconnections or something like that?
Or was there more something more specific going on? It can be the density of connections
structurally how much white matter connects this area to that area?
You know, and there are certain brain areas where you have a lot of white matter coming in and a lot of white matter going out to other parts of the brain.
They're called hubs and there are nodes that have lesser connections.
And so it makes kind of, it certainly makes sense that being able to make measurements of brain connectivity would be related to things like intelligence. And you remember what some of the major hubs were?
Like, are they identifiable as also as neuroinatomical areas with specific functions?
They're definitely neuroanatomical areas.
And they're what you might expect, but what was exciting to me is they mapped on to a model
of brain intelligence relationships that I had developed with my colleague Rex Young
and published in 2007, and it's called the Parietal Frontal Integration, or PFIT of intelligence. And the idea
is that the connections between the parietal load, which is here, and the frontal area,
are the key connections for intelligence.
Okay, so tell us, tell us why you drive that particular theory, because you know, people
have suggested, say, alternatively, that the seat of higher order intelligence is
basically let's say the Dorsal lateral preflight cortex or something like that
so why specifically the connection the connection patterns between frontal
and and perino areas? Well this article in 2007 was a review article where we
took every single brain imaging study we
could find that included a measure of intelligence and there were 37 such studies at the time,
including some I had done as early as 1988 and others had done with much larger samples.
And we just kind of qualitatively analyzed the results to see what brain areas came up in common across these studies using different measures, different imaging techniques.
And we found that there was not that not all brain areas were equally distributed. They tended to be concentrated in the front and the parietal lobe, but also we found areas in the occipital
lobe and the temporal lobe that were also related to intelligence.
And so we developed this model that we talked about how information would be processed and
how information would flow around this set of, I think there were 18 areas all over the pipe plots.
I said, the school sports high on intelligence tests would have some combination of these
areas. You didn't need all of them kind of working together. But some people would have
this combination, some people would have that combination. And if you could make measurements about the way information was flowing around
these areas with a technology like the Magneto and South of the ground which shows changes
in the brain millisecond by milliseconds, then you might be able to actually estimate
IQ from brain images.
In 2007 people were trying to do this with multiple regression
equations that never really replicated. Independent replications didn't go very far.
Because the sample sizes were relatively small. You had enormous individual differences.
But these newer techniques, these mathematical techniques of calculating brain connectivity really seemed to have
advanced this whole thing dramatic. Was there a map between the nodes that were
identified in this more recent research in the areas that you guys had
identified with your overarching analysis? Yes, to the way Rex Young and I
worked at the data, it seemed like there was considerable overlap and some of the authors
who we did not know personally when they wrote their papers noted that their findings
were consistent with our model. Any hemispheric differences?
Yes, there were more on the left than on the right, but there were also areas on the right as well.
And these areas tend to be areas that are also related
to language and memory and attention.
So the more fundamental cognitive processes
of language and memory and attention seem to be the architecture on
which intelligence is real.
Right.
Hey, do you, here's a question that I haven't been able to figure out because I've looked
at the attention literature a lot, and the more I look at the attention literature, the
more I find it difficult to distinguish it from the intelligence literature.
I mean, attention and intelligence seem to be different things,
and we certainly use the words in common parlance as different.
But I haven't really been able to, like imagine you wanted
to establish a battery of attention-related tests
that were independent of G-loaded,
so on cognitive abilities.
I haven't seen anybody manage that.
And so, do you, what do you think the difference is between the capacity to pay attention,
which also seems to be associated with conscientiousness, by the way, which isn't associated with IQ?
But I mean, what's the relationship between attention and intelligence as far as you're concerned?
Those studies have been done, where they take cognitive variables,
the elemental cognitive
tasks is what they call them.
The real basic things that cognitive psychologists like to study, they like to study reasoning.
And they all study learning and memory, but they don't want to address why some people
learn faster than other people or why some people can remember more than other people. That's a cognitive psychology study.
They study what's common to everybody.
But if you look at these elemental cognitive tasks, you can extract a G factor of cognition,
which is highly correlated with a psychometric G factor of abilities.
More than attention, memory, aspects of memory are more correlated to the G factor.
Processing speed is correlated. Right. Sure. And attention is also correlated. So, you
know, I kind of have this idea from being a parent, watching my kids grow up, that people differ in their baseline of attention
when they're not specifically paying attention.
This might be called consciousness.
So, you know, you have two kids walking through a museum
for an hour, and you come out and you say to kid one,
so what did you see?
And you got a whole long thing,
and you ask the other kid, well so what did you see? And you get a whole long thing. And yes, the other
kid, well, why did you see there? And you get a much less rich explanation. Yeah. Of what?
I've always thought about that as a difference in resolution of worlds. Well, you'll call it what
you will, but you know, there are these differences. And, you know, we have, we've actually studied consciousness
with brain imaging, with my friend Mike Alkair who's an anesthesiologist. We did the
first imaging studies where we brought normal volunteers in, and Mike gave them anesthetic
drugs to not come out completely while they underwent brain imaging.
And we had different levels of anesthesia.
We were trying to see what part of the brain is the last part of the brain to turn off
when you lose consciousness.
And they're listed.
And did you find anything that you could make sense out of from that?
Because that, of course, that's obviously an extraordinarily interesting question.
I mean, the collapse of these networks? Well, it's like, the real expert on this, he's published a whole series of papers.
And the mechanism of consciousness is still one of the great Nobel Prize winning.
Yeah, well, that's my interpretation of literature too. It's like, it's such a mystery
that it seems uncroicable in some sense.
Well, what was good about the imaging stuff here, you could study this experimentally.
You could put people into different levels of consciousness and bring them out at will and see how the brain reacted. So the FALMIS seems to be important. A lot of people are in FALMIS connections.
And each anesthetic, there are categories of anesthetics.
And they have different mechanisms of action in the brain,
yet they all produce the same consequence
when you arrange for justice.
Right, that's a very interesting thing too.
So I know with the ceramic, what is it?
Cortical philamic loops.
I mean, there's a guy named Vold.
What's his name?
Voldemar?
God, it's close to that.
I'm afraid I haven't got it right.
He suggests that one of the consequences of psychedelic drugs is to
decrease the gating of the
philamic cortical circuitry and that that's one of the
mechanisms whereby that expanded at least sense of consciousness emerges as a consequence
of experimentation with the psychedelic end of the pharmaceutical universe.
Yeah, I have a section in the book where I talk about consciousness and these studies and try to relate the concept
that if you can turn consciousness off, you should be able to turn it on. We know anesthesiologists
do this at will, even though they don't understand what they're doing. Right, right. You know, they can't tell you why it works.
But then can you use, if anesthetic drugs
kind of dissociate the brain.
And creativity seems to be related
to a dissociation of the frontal lobes.
Can low doses, very, very low doses of
anesthetic drugs cause just enough diss inhibition to increase your creativity.
And I don't think this experiment has ever been done.
Well, I know you see something similar in that. Sometimes reported with people who develop
front of temporal dementia.
Exactly right.
You know, and that's a very strange phenomenon
where as your brain deteriorates,
your creativity increases because so much of brain function
seems to be inhibitory.
So I don't know of a comparable disease
that produces increases in intelligence.
Right.
No, no, I've never heard of it.
Oh, yeah, the other thing that's been, so you tell me what you think about this.
I mean, I was curious for a while about these companies like Lumosity, because when we
developed our original prefrontal tasks, they were tests, they kind of had a game like
element, you know.
And we kind of thought, well, maybe if you had people practice doing them,
they would obviously get better at the specific task because that is what people do.
But then if you had people practice a whole bunch of them,
maybe they would get better at the whole, at the entirety of the tasks in a way that would generalize to other
to other measures like like the Ravenive Matrices, which is a good
measure of fluid intelligence.
But that never works.
And of course, the lumiosity claim that they were able to produce enhancement in general
intelligence.
But by all appearances, that's been a dismal failure as well.
It's very strange, in some sense, that general factor doesn't seem to be something
that you can actually improve by practice.
Right.
Well, you know, what the hell?
Why can't I just still get that?
It doesn't make sense.
But here's something you don't know about me, Albette.
But I am in the Guinness Book of Records, the Gamers edition because of my study, my brain imaging study of Tetris.
And I did a brain imaging study of Tetris.
I was published in 1992.
I really wanted to do a study of warning.
I wanted to see what happened in the brain before and after you learned something.
And back in 1991, when I was doing this, nobody had personal computers. Nobody had ever heard of Tetris.
Computer games were not what they heard today.
Right.
And I went to the Egghead software store when they had software stores that had just opened.
And I was talking to the guys there about, I needed something where I could study before and after they warned something relatively simple.
Yep.
They showed me this game Tetris.
It had just come in.
They just opened the box.
They put it up on the computer there.
And I thought, boy, this is really perfect.
It's simple to learn, but there's an enormous learning
curve.
And we brought in college students, a small number
of college students.
And we showed them how to play Tetris.
They practiced for a few minutes.
We injected the radioactive glucose for positron emission tomography, and we did PET scan studies
of them that very first day they played Tetris for 32 minutes.
They then practiced every day they had a come to my office because nobody had a PC at
home. They came to my office five days a week for about four weeks. They
practiced until they got so good at Tetris. You know, the game is moving
faster. Yeah, it's amazing how good people can get it. You couldn't even
believe a human being could do that. I know it's I've watched people do that sort
of thing. It's just absolutely unbelievable how good they can get out of it.
Right. And when they got really good, we scanned them a second time.
Yep. And we found that even though the game was faster and harder when they
had learned how to do it, they used less glucose metabolic rate.
Okay. So now it was not also okay. So I knew that research. I read that research.
Now, now, what do you think about do you think about the problem is I can't
remember where this research came from because I also read at approximately the same time studies
that appeared to claim. And I think this was reviewed by Elkonen Goldberg in his book
on Hemisphere Specialization for Routinization and novelty, respectively, that as you, when
you first start to learn something novel,
and I think this was demonstrated, for example, in people who were listening,
they were Danish native speakers who were listening to Danish in reverse, and
they used very large part portions of the brain when they were listening to
Danish in reverse, but if they were listening to Danish property spoken, they
used very small specified parts of the brain that were located
in the back part of the left hemisphere.
And there was another group of researchers who were demonstrating that as you learned, the
degree of activation decreased and it shifted from the right to the left and it shifted
from the front to the back and it got smaller and smaller.
And is that associated with that decreased glucose utilization?
Is that the glucose utilization?
Is that the same phenomenon?
I think it is.
I think we were the first to show it.
And we had done one imaging study before this tetra study
where we just correlated glucose metabolic rate
with scores on a test of abstract reasoning,
a high G-loaded test, they had taken the test
during the imaging.
So we got to see what brain areas were involved, and we did find some brain areas, but the
really interesting surprising thing was the correlation between the scores in glucose
metabolic rate was always negative.
I don't know if they did on the test, but lower their glucose metabolic rate.
And that was the first ink
When we had about this idea of brain efficiency, right?
That there's been a lot of research on that
And it turns out to be a complicated thing because nothing about the brain is simple right but
It does seem to make perfect sense that expert skill is associated with doing more with less
It makes sense now, but I tell you, nobody predicted it.
No, I can look, I understand.
That research stood out for me in a very striking manner.
But it also gets at this thing about that you raised about practicing on different kinds
of tests, the attractive things about Tetris is its visual
spatial, its planning ahead, or its attention.
There are a lot of elemental cognitive tasks necessary, you know, it's fine motor control,
really necessary to do well on Tetris and to learn it really well. And so the Tetris company found my research some years later
and asked if I'd be willing to try to replicate it
with more modern imaging, which of course I was willing to do.
So they funded this.
And we found, now instead of PETSKAN, we use Functional MRI
and Structural MRI both. And we did find, like we found with of PETSKAN, we used functional MRI and structural MRI both.
And we did find, like we found with the PETSKAN in more work areas where after teenage girls
with very limited gaming experience were Tetris.
Their brain activity decreased, but we also found from the structural MRI that there were increases in gray matter.
And the really interesting thing is the areas where there were increases in gray matter
did not overlap at all with the areas that functionally decreased.
It would have been a terrific story.
That's strange.
Yeah, well things are always more complicated
than you hope them to be.
Not only that, but I can tell you
that every time I did a brain imaging study,
we always found the exact opposite
of what we expected.
That sounds to me like, you might actually be
operating as a real scientist.
I mean things are so damn complicated that it's really difficult to guess right to begin
with. Well I went to one of my three laws that I
based the book on. Law number one is no story about the brain is simple. Law number two is
no one study is definitive and law number three is it takes a long time
to sort out all the various studies to see what's consistent and what establishes a reliable
way of evidence.
Okay, so let me put you on a different track momentarily, and maybe this won't work, but
I'm always curious about, let's say, the practical implications of scientific research
both at a personal level, familial level, social level, all of those things.
So I mean, one of the things that I'm planning to do in the near future is to launch a website
that will enable people to assess themselves with what we've developed a scale called the
Big Five Aspect Scale that breaks the Big five down into 10 aspects, each of which provides some additional high resolution and useful descriptions of personality.
So you'll be able to go there and find out what your personality is like.
You'll be able to compare yourself to other people who you know to find out where your similarities
and differences are.
But one of the things we've been thinking about doing as well is putting up on the same
side a real IQ test, nicely validated, probably focusing on fluid intelligence because it's
a little bit less linguistically complex to do so, but maybe measuring verbal intelligence.
And then showing people the strata of occupations in which they're likely to find maximal success.
of occupations in which they're likely to find maximal success. Because, you know, from, for me, given that I know that people vary in their cognitive
abilities tremendously and that that's actually an important determinant of their life outcome,
it seems to me.
So, let's say someone tests out in an IQ of around 115.
And so you could say, look, you know, you're pretty damn smart.
You're up above 85% of the general population.
You could probably do a pretty damn good job as an undergraduate in university if you're
also disciplined.
If you were conscientious, you hit the books hard, you're going to come out in the top
quartile of your class, assuming that you're not at a spectacularly selective university.
But you're going to have a much more difficult time
as a master student, and PhD level stuff
is going to be pushing your luck to really master that.
But you could be, here's a domain of what
do you call it, industrial organizational activity,
where you could really be in the top 10% tile.
So, if you have an IQ of 115, you might make one bang up plumber and you can have a spectacularly
successful career as a plumber and maybe as a manager of other plumber and all of that.
I mean, I actually happen to be a real efficient out of the trade.
So I certainly don't think of that as something that's a low quality or low status occupation in the least. But we would like to tell people, okay, here's
a, here's an intellectual domain that's probably too high for you to be successful without
working in sane hours to close the gap.
Cause you can do that with insane work up
to some limited degree.
But it seems to me that the logical thing to do
at least in part is to give people a sense
of what their advantages and limitations are
and then say to them, okay, well given that,
here's a place that you could go
where you could be optimally successful.
And so, you know, that's kind of my take from a policy perspective, let's say.
But, like, what have you thought about?
You know the massive diversity and intellectual ability.
What do you mean?
What are the implications from a policy perspective as far as you're concerned?
Well, I think Volcano Guidance is clearly one.
I actually did some consulting and some research for a nonprofit group called the Johnson O'Connor Foundation,
which is vocational testing.
They're not that big on G, but they have their own battery of tests.
They bring someone in and do a full day of cognitive tests.
And from that, they give some advice about what kinds of professions match their cognitive
strengths and weaknesses.
And I actually did some brain imaging on their tests and it was very interesting stuff.
Did you think they do?
Did you think?
Because it's hard to do the psychometrics properly with regards to vocational guidance because we don't really know, we don't
have a good handle on how to classify jobs into their various subtypes like John Holland has done some
good work doing that but there's so many jobs and it's hard to figure out what makes two jobs the
same or similar you know. I was at Hopkins when Holland was there and some of my friends were
graduate students worked for him.
And I wound all about that occasional testing.
It's very powerful.
And as you know, that's more of his scales that morphed into more personality dimensions.
Yeah, well that's it.
Well, that's exactly the next step that we want to play out.
It's like, okay, because there is a reason, I know people have been mapping Holland's job categories
onto the big five and we have fair bit of success,
and we're hoping that the differentiation down
to the 10 levels of personality will provide even more
precision, but with more general policy, okay, so fine.
So reasonable vocational counseling, That's a good idea.
When does it start?
Does it start in junior high?
Do you do what the Europeans do and start to track people into trades and hire education
at that kind of early age?
The Europeans seem to have had great success with that.
So it's certainly-
In Germany in particular.
Yeah, so it's reasonable to look at, but the problem in the United States,
there are so many problems with the way we conceptualize education.
And the whole idea of tracking,
I don't know how it is in Canada,
but in the United States,
this idea of tracking has a very negative balance to it.
You know, celebrating the smart kids into one set of classes
and the less smart kids into others,
and then there's medial education,
and there's been a tendency,
a strong tendency in the United States
to feel that kids are learning from each other.
So you don't want homogeneous groups
based on learning ability.
You want to...
Yeah, it's so funny because the people say that with regards to, let's say, academic achievement,
but they play exactly the opposite stunt when it comes to such things as childhood sports.
So if you look at football, for example, it's like, hey, let's say, you're gay like ability,
it's like you don't have the people who stumble around
on the field dropping the ball all the time playing with the top-end quarterbacks
and nobody thinks there's a problem with that they don't say well everyone
learns from everyone else in that situation so to me it
speaks more of a refusal to admit to the stark reality that there are
massive cognitive differences between people and to try to actually start to address that with some degree of seriousness.
And the seriousness should be something like, okay, well, let's look at the bottom 15%
of the population cognitively speaking.
It's like, what the hell can we do for those people that's going to be useful?
And, you know, like the guy that I was telling you about, I was trying to think of some way that he could find a respectable and productive and relatively stable position in society that would be useful.
And I thought there was a couple of things he could do.
One of the things he could have done, I think, he could have been encouraged, let's say,
to collect trash in the downtown areas. Like he could have been assigned a city block.
And it could have been said to him, look, your job
is to keep this damn city block clean.
Here's a bag.
Here's a stick.
You get up in the morning.
You go do this.
It's like, it makes everybody's life more pleasant.
It's a valuable contribution.
It's something you could do with a certain degree of pride.
And there's a socially valuable
end of it.
Now, what happens in Toronto is that people drive around these vacuum cleaner machines on sidewalks
and pick up the scrap paper and all of that.
That actually turns out to be a very cognitively demanding job because, well, you have to pay
careful attention.
You can't run over people.
You have to have decent social skills,
and you know, it's complicated, but it might be nice to see, but we're not mature enough to have a discussion like this as a society.
We might want to say, it's like, okay, well, there's a group of people who aren't going to be able to compete in the cognitive workplace.
They're not going to do it, and there's actually lots of them, and we're not going to say they're lazy and we're not going to say they're not looking for work. We're not going to say any of that. We're going to say look.
We need to find occupations that have public utility that aren't just make work projects that people of that level of abstract capacity could actually perform. But I don't think we have
the maturity, have that conversation. We don't have the economics. And so I would go even a little
to the left of you and I would say there's nothing wrong with make-work programs.
You know, to allow people, I mean, you have to work with dignity. So you have that option, but you also have this interesting
experiment being proposed of the minimum
the manual income.
Yeah.
Well, that one worries me because, see,
there's a couple of things about that one that concerns me,
because, you know, so we're having a conversation here
where we're taking differences in IQ seriously.
But the problem with the guaranteed annual income issue,
I think, one of the problems,
and I'm not denying its potential utility.
It's something that I think that would have to be
experimentally determined, and all of that.
And maybe it could replace a plethora
of less efficient social welfare programs.
But it isn't obvious to me. It's obvious to
me that there's a substantial proportion of the population, and I would say it's probably
5%. That would destroy themselves instantly if you gave them a guaranteed annual income.
And they would do it because they're very low in conscientiousness, for example, and
very impulsive. And like I've worked with many guys, often ex-kill-cane addicts who were often not all that high
on the end of the cognitive distribution, but very, very low in conscientiousness, high
in impulsivity.
And those guys were absolutely fine as long as they were flat broke.
But then, I tell you, as soon as they had money, they were done.
It was like three days in the bar, cocaine binge, face down in the ditch,
and then they were fine until they got money again.
And so, the problem with the guaranteed annual income solution is that,
you know, man does not live by bread alone, let's say.
If you have money and you have things to do, then you have a life. But if you
just have money, you don't have a life.
Well, you know, I look at, you know, I have a very narrow lens on this. What do I know
about the big social policies? And, you know, I don't know anything about that. More than
anybody else. I have opinions like everybody. But my narrow lens is through intelligence.
I think more is better than less.
That's more doesn't make you a better person, doesn't make you honest, doesn't make you
likeable, doesn't make you conscient, doesn't solve the problems of the world.
But if we can do something to increase intelligence generally,
I think that would go in some measure
toward alleviating a lot of these very complex problems.
So I imagine the homelessness problem.
Some proportion of chronicling homeless people
have schizophrenia.
Yeah.
Schizophrenia is a genetic disorder.
Nobody knows what the genes are or how they work,
but it's pretty clear there's a genetic component.
If you can find the genes, figure out what they do,
and come up with real good treatments
if not cures for schizophrenia and possibly
preventions for schizophrenia, that indirectly is going
to help alleviate the homeless problem.
Right.
So I don't know what to do about the homeless, but if you can figure out what to do about
schizophrenia, that's going to have some impact on that.
Yes, it's basically, yeah, well, you know, your claim to some degree is that these more
complex social problems should be decomposed
into isolated micro problems and that specific solution should be sought for them and that
that certainly strikes me as an appropriate approach. Because everybody is different, not everyone's
for the same reason. No, no, well that's for sure. That's for sure. Not everyone's poor for the same
for the same reason. By any stretch of the imagination. That's right. Poverty doesn't have one cause.
No.
And it's not merely caused by lack of money either because that would be a much easier problem to solve.
Unfortunately, it's not so simple.
So the one thing that we're probably getting toward the end, the one thing I would like to leave your listeners with,
is really optimism. These are not dismal problems and perpetuity because the genetic approach, I think, and
I'm kind of out there on this, this is not a mainstream view, but I think the more something
is genetic, the more likely it is we can change it for the good.
And that's why we should study the genetics of intelligence.
That's why I hope genes have more to do with it
that environment.
Not, no, not because I want to do something nefarious
with genes.
And I understand the history of this very, yes.
But because I think in neuroscience, there's optimism.
And to guard against the negative aspects,
the potential negative aspects of this,
the only solution is to have public conversation about this.
And to get people to understand IQ intelligence, G-factor,
these are not all the same things.
They have different relationships to the data,
to the genetic data, particularly.
So we have to understand these things.
And once we have some sense of this and not hate people who believe in that there's a genetic component
as our, as our pre-order racist or malevolent people, I think we can come to a common understanding about how June's an environment interact and figure out how all that works.
And begin to think how in the year 2050 or 2060,
we're going to have to resolve.
We'll have resolved some of these issues to practical benefit.
Well, that is a good place to leave it, I think.
We've had a very productive discussion,
so I think that's probably a nice place to bring everything to a close.
I've been speaking with Dr. Richard Hire,
who studies the neuro basis of human intelligence and cognition,
and who has recently released a new book called The Neuroscience of Intelligence,
published by Cambridge University Press.
It's an academic book, but for people who are interested in a serious discussion of the relationship
between biology and intelligence, then it's a good go-to tone, and it's an extraordinarily important
topic given the undeniable primacy of intelligence as the fundamental predictor of let's say success
in many many domains across the human life span.
So thank you very much for spending an hour or so with us today and with any luck maybe
we'll talk to you again in the future.
I'd like to talk to you perhaps about consciousness at some point that might be entertaining.
Well listen, I really enjoyed it. I appreciate the chance of having more than three sentences,
you know, three sentences, you know, related to the listeners.
I just want to say one thing about the book. It is written for the late public as well.
Oh, okay. Well, that's a good thing to know.
Cambridge wouldn't let me call it the neuroscience
of intelligence, calling what every parent and student
needs to know about neuroscience,
because I wanted to market it as a strictly academic book.
But it's getting quite a bit of attention
just among non-academic groups.
Okay, well, excellent.
Well, I'll make sure that I mentioned that in the description,, which I will I'll post a link to the book probably on Amazon
I that's usually the most straightforward thing and also point out that it is in fact written for people who aren't
Only specialists in the area. So that was good
Thanks. Yeah, thanks a lot. Hey, listen. Thank you very much. I appreciate it. No problem. It's a pleasure talking with you too. Good to meet you.