The Joy of Why - What Can Birdsong Teach Us About Human Language?
Episode Date: November 21, 2024It’s fair to say that enjoyment of a podcast would be severely limited without the human capacity to create and understand speech. That capacity has often been cited as a defining character...istic of our species, and one that sets us apart in the long history of life on Earth. Yet we know that other species communicate in complex ways. Studies of the neurological foundations of language suggest that birdsong, or communication among bats or elephants, originates with brain structures similar to our own. So why do some species vocalize while others don’t? In this episode, Erich Jarvis, who studies behavior and neurogenetics at the Rockefeller University, chats with Janna Levin about the surprising connections between human speech, birdsong and dance.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
There's tons of people online making these huge claims about how you should live your life.
On Science Versus, we take those claims on.
This season, we're finding out.
Are microplastics as scary as they sound?
Microplastics have now been discovered in human testicles.
Is social media rotting your brain?
Have we hit the climate tipping point?
Is it Waterworld? Is it Kevin Costner Waterworld?
New episodes of Science Versus, that's Science VS,
are out now.
All animals exhibit some form of communication,
from the primitive hiss of a lizard
to the complex gestures natural to chimps
or the songs shared by whales.
But human language does seem exceptional,
a vast and discreet cognitive leap.
Yet recent research is finding
surprising neurological connections
between our expressive speech
and the types of communication innate to other animals, giving us new ideas
about the biological and developmental origins of language.
I'm Jana Levin, and this is The Joy of Why, a podcast from Quantum Magazine where I take
turns at the mic with my co-host, Steve Strohgatz, exploring the biggest questions in math and
science today.
In this episode, we speak with neuroscientist Eric Jarvis about the evolution of language and the search for
answers in unexpected places like the songs of birds. Eric is a professor at
the Rockefeller University and a Howard Hughes Medical Institute investigator.
At Rockefeller he directs the field Research Center of Ethology and Ecology.
He also directs the Neurogenetics Lab of Language and co-directs the Vertebrate Genome Lab,
where he studies song-learning birds and other species to gain insight into the mechanisms
underlying language and vocal learning.
Eric, we're so glad to have you here with us today.
Well, thank you very much.
And thank you for that wonderful introduction.
Yes, we have the pleasure of actually being in person today, which is nice.
So I want to start with what we're actually doing here, which is using language and what
our audience is doing, which is listening to words.
I guess the first question is, is language built in biologically, even genetically?
And also, is it uniquely human?
So the first part, language is built in genetically
in us humans.
We're born with the capacity to learn how to produce
and how to understand language and pass it on culturally
from one generation to the next.
The actual details is learned, but the actual plan
in the brain is there.
Second part of your question, is it special or unique to humans?
It is specialized in humans, but certainly many components of what gives rise to language
is not unique to humans.
There's a spectrum of abilities out there in other species that we share some aspects
of with other species.
It really is quite amazing the proliferation of human languages, and yet we share some aspects of with other species. It really is quite amazing the proliferation
of human languages.
And yet we share this common biological genetic route.
Yes, yes.
And is it incredibly complicated genetically,
or is it kind of simple to identify
that there is this invariant genetic component?
I'm going to say that it is complicated in terms
of the genetics underlying language,
particularly spoken language, which is even more rare than other kinds of communication.
It's complicated because we haven't figured it out yet.
But I don't think it's impossible to figure out because we're getting there.
And I say we, I mean the collective we of scientists in the field studying this question.
Can you walk me through the mechanics of language? What's happening neurologically and physiologically when I'm both speaking to you but also inventing
the things I'm going to say and how I'm going to say it?
Well, language actually is like all kinds of different behaviors that we and other animals
display.
When we think of it only in the human experience, we tend to just think of language as a unitary behavior.
But actually when you look closely at the comparisons across species and in the brain,
you realize the language can be broken down into multiple components. One component is understanding
what someone is saying, perceiving those sounds, processing them. We call that auditory perception
and learning how to understand them. Another component would be like syntax or sequencing of sounds with some rules.
The other would be semantics, like meaning in the sounds.
And the component that's most rare is vocal production learning, the ability to produce
imitated sounds.
And so those are like five different components.
They're more. And when you look
in the brain, right, you can actually find that there are different brain circuits that
control these different components. Not all of them are separate, but the two that I like
to really highlight and make a distinction on is the auditory learning brain circuits
different from the vocal production learning brain circuits. The auditory learning brain circuits different from the vocal production learning brain circuits.
The auditory learning brain circuits many species have, like your dog can understand
the word sit, siente say in Spanish and so forth. But the dog can't say those words,
but we can, and that's the vocal production component that's more rare.
Right. He definitely understands you want to go out.
Yes, yes.
Very excited about those words. Yeah, in
some Breeze of Dogs you can get them to understand several hundred human words
but they can't say one of them. So my thinking and other people's as well is
that auditory learning and the ability to understand complex sound combinations
came earlier than the ability to produce those sounds. Now, what is the difference between, let's say,
language and speech and what you refer to as vocal learning?
Maybe we could pick that apart a little more.
Yes.
I'll have to say that the distinctions there are not,
let's say, uniformly or universally agreed upon
or defined in our field.
So many species that have vocal organs, larynx and mammals, serings
and burns, they produce sounds. Most of those species are producing innately
determined sounds like the color of your eyes are innately determined. You can put
lenses in to change the colors but that's some kind of physical modification
going on separate from the brain. And so this ability to produce learned sounds is an essential component
of spoken language, but not the only one. All right? But without the vocal learning
you won't have spoken language. Now, language broadly and speech. In the
traditional linguistic sense, many people will say speech and language as if
they're two separate things.
In our everyday experience as people, as citizens of this planet, when people say language,
they're thinking of speech.
They're thinking of perceiving it and producing it.
They're not thinking of sign language, writing, and so forth.
And so why is that the case?
Now people argue with me on this, but I'm going to say it.
When you look in the brain and look at what brain cells, what circuits are being activated
when we speak and then when we express language through speech, through gesture,
through facial communication and so forth, really what I think is going on is
that there isn't a separation really between speech and spoken language. All
the grammatical rules, the syntax,
all of that is using the same brain areas
that are responsible for producing the sounds.
All right, I don't see a distinction there.
For gesturing like sign language,
now some will disagree with me as well,
but I believe in the data that I see is that
those brain regions are directly adjacent to the brain regions that are controlling the voice.
And they're using similar grammatical rules and so forth in those brain circuits in a
parallel fashion that the spoken language circuit uses.
So what I think is going on there evolutionarily is that our brain, our ancestor's brain, and the brains of some animals already
have a lot of complex processing for syntactic-like structure of their behavior.
And what happened to the spoken language circuits, once vocal learning evolved, that new brain
circuit adapted what was already pre-existing.
I'm fascinated by that idea.
I've been wondering about other aspects of the brain that must have had to develop in parallel or in some kind of feedback loop
with these other neural pathways.
Yeah, that's right.
So there's this idea that the language circuit sort of evolved totally de novo as a separate language module in the brain
commanding everybody else what
to do and that it's sort of autonomous. I totally disagree with that.
So you raise some interesting issues as well. One is the motor aspect of speaking and
controlling your voice or speaking sign language and then the structure of
grammar which also is built in. I think we all know that new dialects crop up
and develop with languages,
but they have their own grammar
that has to be adhered to as well.
You can tell if you're not using slang properly
and you're laughed at, because you're making mistakes.
So can I see neurologically or biologically
a difference between the motor function aspect of this
versus the concept that there's a grammar built in as well?
Yeah, and there are two parts to the answer itself. That is, I think that the
spoken language brain circuits is basically in an advanced motor pathway,
controlling movement of the larynx, of the lips, of the jaw, and so forth. Not that
dramatically different from other motor pathways for learning how to dance,
learning how to dance,
learning how to walk, learning how to fly in birds.
So I think it's essentially an advanced motor learning pathway.
And I say that because we and others have found evidence that the vocal learning pathways,
we can also call them spoken language pathways in their analogous regions and other species
that learn how to imitate sounds, is embedded within a motor pathway
that controls body movement.
It's not embedded in the visual system.
It's not embedded in the auditory system or hearing.
It's embedded in movement systems of the brain.
Literally where it is in the brain.
Literally positioned physically in more frontal lobe areas.
The circuit for the production part of the sounds is more distributed to the front part of the brain in humans and mostly in the other
species we've looked at.
So a lot of people, when they hear movement control about the brain, they automatically
assume that the other stuff like grammar and syntax, all the rules that go with controlling
those sounds are all somewhere else in the brain in
some higher level cognitive areas. I disagree with that. I think they're
actually embedded too in the motor system, being fed, yeah, by some of these
other areas for perception, but I don't think the motor systems that control
body movement and sound are so primitive. Fascinating. Now there are a number of
species that don't have language in the way that we do, but that
are capable of what you're calling vocal learning.
Can you tell me a little bit more about some examples of those species?
Yes.
So among us mammals, the advanced vocal learners would be us humans, of course.
Bats who sing ultrasonic vocalizations in a range that we can't even hear for their
learned sounds.
And that's different from the echolocation.
That's different from the echolocation, that's right.
So it's a form of communication, it's not just a map of the space.
No, no.
Yeah, these are learned sounds that they use for various reasons other than trying to find
where the wall is at, you know.
So bats, the cetaceans, basically those are whales and dolphins, alright, and dolphins
are just basically small whales. And pinnipeds, like sea whales and dolphins. Alright, and dolphins are just basically small whales.
And pinnipeds like sea lions and elephants.
The elephants disputed, but there's some evidence, you know, I mean, there are some elephants
who are imitating human speech sounds, but they put their trunk in their mouth and they
move their mouth in order to make the sounds as opposed to do it voluntarily to control
human speech production.
So those are mammals.
And then there are three
bird groups. So it's songbirds, like canaries and starlings and so forth. Parrots, like
an African grey parrot. And hummingbirds of all species. They're the smallest birds around,
and they're also vocal earners.
That's an amazing range from birds to aquatic mammals.
And are all of these different species
having a common source to their ability
to demonstrate vocal learning?
No, that's the surprising thing,
is that all of these species,
all by mammals and the three birds that I named,
they appear to have evolved this ability
of vocal learning independently
of a common ancestor having it.
Really?
Yes.
That's kind of amazing.
Yeah.
So we call it parallel evolution or independent evolution.
That just strikes me as huge.
Yes, it does.
I think that's good for astrobiology, that if there are other species that crop up, that
language might be a natural part of the process.
That thought came to me, it's jumping a little ahead here
We and others found that the brain circuits
At least the species that have looked at also are convergent
You know their differences each lineage has evolved something different than the other
But a lot of the brain circuitry and the underlying genetics is convergent Wow and when we made those discoveries
I was thinking this has got to be
Some type of suggestion that life could evolve multiple times in a similar way on some other planet.
Amazing. And why do you think that, you know, bats and whales share this in common?
Yeah, there's so many theories about why the language evolved or even why the vocal
learning evolved and what's selected and why is it more common and so forth.
And no one really knows.
Steven Pinker thinks sexual selection
or some kind of advance ability
for communication and survival.
I think that there's something selecting for it
and there's something selecting against it.
And they may be more simple than you think.
I think the fundamental thing selecting
for the ability to imitate sounds is sexual selection, that is, mate attraction. When vocal learners tend to produce their
variety of sounds and try to use it to attract a mate, the more diverse your vocal repertoire,
the more likely you're going to attract mates. How do you get a diverse repertoire? You learn how to imitate sounds
and you also steal sounds from the environment,
like mockingbirds do or African gray parrots do.
So that's what's selecting for it.
What's selecting against it?
I think that a diversity of sounds
not only maintains the auditory perception
of the listening animal of your own species,
but also of the predators, right? So the auditory perception of the listening animal of your own species, but also of the predators.
So the auditory system of predators is going to have a harder time habituating to this sound diversity that you're trying to advertise from the top of a tree somewhere.
So you're more likely to be eaten, not survive.
So that's not a good thing. It works in the wrong direction. So then you ask why humans and dolphins and elephants and parrots and so forth.
I think most of the vocal learning species are either near the top of their food chain
or they're vocalizing in a high pitch range like bats where many other species can't hear
them.
And it turns out we've done some phylogenetic studies to show that songbirds and parrots were descended
from apex bird predators that are now extinct, but those were their ancestors and maybe they
were evolving this ability during that time and now have held onto it.
So they're not so afraid because they're at the top of the food chain.
And hummingbirds are pretty fast.
Those are my thoughts. So, about communicating, can they learn what a whale's trying to say?
Can a whale learn what a hummingbird is trying to communicate?
Yes and no. And the reason why I say yes is that you do have species, more likely closely related species, like you can take a zebra finch and raise
it with its cousin species, bengalese finch, and the zebra finch young animal will pick
up the bengalese finch song.
Not as good as a bengalese finch because there are physical differences in its larynx
and so forth, and even some of the brain circuitry may impose some limitations.
But you can get species imitating other species sounds being fostered, basically.
Not only cross-fostered in your own species, cross-fostered with another species.
All right, and vocal learners will pick up those sounds.
And they will communicate in whatever way they can, you know, not as good as your own species.
Now, the reason why I say no is that you know what it's even hard for us
humans to understand another language if we're not growing up with it. It's
particularly a language that's phylogenetic distant from the language
you grew up with. And so just plopping me in the middle of a population of people
speaking in different language is gonna be a lot of effort. Very challenging.
Yes. And we see that it hardens with age. That's right. Yes. So we have these
critical periods for vocal learning abilities.
That's why we can learn how to imitate sounds
at a younger age before puberty,
before we get this hormone surge.
Afterwards, the brain settles in and it makes it harder.
Not impossible, but harder
in all the vocal learning species.
We'll be right back after this message.
Now we've been talking about language, and I want to emphasize that a lot of
your work is on birds and in particular as you've said not all birds are vocal
learners. You've mentioned the songbirds, the parrots, and the hummingbirds. What
sets them apart? Do we know why even if their ancestry is in this apex predator
why only some of them,
these three categories, develop vocal learning?
Yeah, beyond what I was saying is selection for and against that I don't know, but I can
say in terms of the brain regions that control this behavior, what's remarkable is that all
three of those bird groups have exactly seven brain structures
connected in a similar network pattern in the brain that controls the syrinx
and birds and they have genetic differences inside the vocal brain
regions that differ in what we call gene regulation. So the up and down regulation
of protein products, okay, of certain genes
that control connectivity, that control how fast the neurons communicate with each other
and so forth.
We find differences in those brain regions in the same way in all three of the bird groups,
even though they're not closely related to each other.
Fascinating.
But there are other interesting correlations that all the vocal learners share, or at least
some of them do, right?
One is we found that the more advanced vocal learning ability they have, the more advanced
they are at problem solving, indicating there probably is some relationship between some
other cognitive abilities and vocal learning and language.
It's usually been an assumption, but we actually found this at least among songbirds recently. And another
one is that all vocal learners seem to have their juvenile periods of life
extended. They go through this altricial kind of growth where they have to be
cared for by parents. They're not like where they're born like a chicken and
can walk right away. So I think that is happening because it's necessary to be young with adults for a period
of time to pick up the culture, to pick up the learned repertoire of vocalizations.
And then an unexpected one, but one discovered about a decade ago, is that only vocal learning
species can learn how to dance.
And that is to synchronize body movements
to a rhythmic beat in music.
Wow.
Is that the connection with the motor function aspect?
What do they call that, synesthesia?
When you mix your senses?
So why would listening to something
make you wanna move your body?
We don't walk up to paintings and start dancing.
Yeah, it's very...
I wonder if there's somebody who does that.
That's a good one.
So I think what's going on there is that the larynx is the most rapidly firing muscle in
the entire body.
You need very good tight auditory integration from your ears to the brain to integrate it
with the brain pathways that control the larynx.
That tight integration of sound and movement for the larynx
I think then basically contaminate the rest of the brain in
vocal learners to now get tight integration between sound and not just
muscle of the larynx but muscles of the rest of the body. And now we can control our body movements to sound in more advanced ways than the non-vocal
learning species.
So does rhythm itself or pitch play an important part in language?
You seem to be saying that it does.
Yeah, at some societies there isn't really much of a distinction between singing and
dance.
The two might go hand in hand, and that would be consistent with a shared
evolutionary history there. Others think that actually dance itself can be a form of communication
and was a form of communication in early cultures. And by the way, I was once a dancer myself,
so this excites me and another reason why I do this. So maybe there's some kind of connection
there. I have wanted to ask this for quite a while.
Parrots, are they understanding human language
and to what extent, or are they simply emulating a sound?
Yeah, parrots, you can teach them to understand
meaning of human words.
Irene Pepperberg's work is the most famous for this.
A lot
of people just assume that these animals, whether they're vocal learners or not,
just rambling off with random sounds with no meaning and so forth, right? It's
really hard to think that nature was built that way, you know, just does these
things randomly for no reason at all. In fact, what I think is going on is that,
you know, from studies of vervid monkeys, Seifarth and
Cheney showed this a while ago, is that there are these certain alarm calls that would mean
an eagle in the sky or a snake on the ground to these animals in Africa.
And if you play these sounds through a speaker, you'll see them look up for an eagle, look
on the ground for a snake.
So these are species that are producing innate vocalizations. So already,
even without vocal learning, they have understanding of the meaning of sounds that they pass on
culturally from one generation to the next. So I think meaning and sounds came before
spoken language.
I wonder if pre-homo sapiens also had this. Is there any sense that Neanderthals were
also using vocal learning? Is that something
we suspect to be the case?
Yeah. I was just watching the Neanderthal documentary on Netflix yesterday. But what
they were saying and what I've been believing from our own work is the more and more you
study Neanderthals, the more and more you really question whether they're a separate
species or more or less like a hybrid kind of species
where you're in that gray zone where you're starting to speciate, you haven't quite become
distinct that you can't breathe anymore.
And I think that's where Neanderthals were.
Now then the question becomes, yeah, they have different facial features, but still
of all the species out there, they're the closest looking to us, right?
Well, what does that mean in terms of the brain and speech?
Well, fortunately, people have recovered DNA
from Neanderthal bones.
It's not as good quality as a living human,
but it's still decent enough quality
that you can get a good proportion
of the genetic code of Neanderthal sequence completed.
And all the genetic differences
that we thus far have seen in humans that are in genes correlating with the presence
of language, we see it in Neanderthal as well.
So I wouldn't be surprised if Neanderthals and our human ancestors that were living at
the time were speaking to each other.
It could have been much simpler than what we have now, both biologically and culturally, but I wouldn't
be surprised. I mean, it's hard for me to believe that's not the case.
I could go on about that for a while. Now, one of the projects that you've been pursuing
aims to neuroengineer song inter birds that are typically songless, like pigeons, for example.
I'd like to know how this is done and what the project's revealing.
So from the technology standpoint, we know that some of the circuitry of the brain that
controls learned vocalizations is different than other species.
And actually we're finding more of it may not exactly be a
difference that's categorically different binary, yes or no. Some of the
circuitry is, let's say, very weak in mice but very robust connection in
humans and sombers and parrots. And one of the ones that we've been focusing on
that's different in this way is the connection that goes from the cortical regions of the brain to the brain stem neurons that control the larynx.
In us humans and songbirds and parrots, there are hundreds of wires basically that go from
the cortex that control those motor neurons that control the muscles for speaking or singing
in these birds.
Whereas for many decades it was thought that mice and some other mammals had
zero connections. It didn't even have that cortical region. We found that mice
actually do have a rudimentary structure of what we see in our speech circuits.
It's just very primitive, so to speak, in its connectivity and its development.
And so what we're trying to do
is take some of those convergent genetic differences
that we see in all the vocal learners looked at to date
and change those genes to be a similar way
in the mouse brain to test the hypothesis
that these genes form that special connection
or enhance that special connection in others.
If we are able to genetically modify this one important connection, I don't think we're
going to get a full-blown mouse that's speaking like Mickey Mouse or anything like that.
Yeah, I'm worried the rats in Central Park are going to start yelling at the tourists
or something.
That's right, yes.
But I think we'll get a step closer.
That's my prediction.
So you're going to reveal something about the understanding of the biological basis regardless.
That's right. That's 50 years or more of people hypothesizing the importance of such a connection difference to vocal behavior.
So this will help us understand the principles of the molecules that are setting up these brain circuits in studies that we can't do with humans and we're trying to do it with the non-human species that you can
study in the laboratory like a mouse because there's so many advanced genetic
tools that have been developed for them. I want to ask about the tools. I'm clear
in understanding when you're talking about a molecular level, if you're
talking about epigenetics, turning certain things on or off, or if you're actually grafting genes
from another species into the DNA?
Actually, both, right?
Because I think both are different, where the actual genetic code of mostly regulatory
regions, so regions that tell a gene to make more of it or make less of it, right?
I think that there's a difference there in
those regulatory regions that influences an epigenetic difference on those same regions.
So you can manipulate that at a molecular level.
That's right, yeah. And so this would involve grafting parts of human genes into the mouse.
What would be good for us as scientists and society to have in a mammalian species that
you can work with in the laboratory to study brain circuits involved in speech disorders,
autism, spectrum disorders associated with communication, and so forth.
Since mice don't have these more advanced circuits, we can't use them.
We have to rely on the birds.
And the birds have been very insightful for us.
But their brain structures are different.
And we're never going to make a bird as a close model
to human as a mammal.
That's one reason that's also motivating for us,
is can we make a mouse model for communication disorders
and then figure out how to repair them?
Now, we've talked about this feedback loop and the connection between language and problem-solving. If you
succeed in wiring with greater multiplicity between, as you said, the
cortex and maybe some of the motor aspects of the brain, will the mouse have
to necessarily develop greater problem-solving skills? At face value I don't think it's going to be a simple correlation because we're trying
to specifically focus on the pathways involved in vocal communication and not change the
whole brain circuitry.
Yeah, if we put a gene in the whole genome, we might affect something, but we see hundreds
of genes that differ in humans relative to the other species.
So it wouldn't be that in a specific mouse it suddenly grows new parts of the brain that's
not genetically possible, but it would have to be in some sense in the germ line passed
down and then it could allow generations of these mice to evolve?
Well I guess while they start selecting upon each other, you know, we get them started.
Yeah, the chatty ones, right, reproduce more often.
That's right, yes.
Yeah.
But, you know, this correlation between vocal learning and problem solving, it gets us the
closest to think that, yeah, having this advanced form of communication made us humans a more
sentient, advanced species, more intelligent, and so forth.
I've been describing vocal learning kind of like a binary trait.
But if you really look carefully, you find that it's more continuous.
All right?
It's not all or none.
I was exactly going to ask you that, whether it was a discrete jump to language or if there
was a continuum.
And if there is a continuum, are we undervaluing other animals' capacities to understand
because they don't demonstrate every aspect of language?
Yes, my answer is yes to all those questions
in that it is not a discrete jump to get to a spoken language.
I think it's like a step ladder function
where you have a jump and then you stay there
and you have another jump and you have another jump,
but there are small jumps and those small jumps add up to a continuum among species. But the
continuum doesn't necessarily have to be phylogenetically linear. What I mean by that, just because the
species is related to you like a chimpanzee doesn't mean that the chimpanzee ancestor
was more primitive or more advanced, because of this parallel evolution
I described earlier, parrots can imitate us
in ways that chimpanzees can't, all right?
And so there's a continuum out there amongst species
that is not all genetically related
according to the family tree.
It's partly related to the family tree,
partly related to how the environment
influenced the evolution of that species. So who do we communicate with more effectively,
the chimpanzee or the parrot? That's a good question. You know what, that's one thing I
don't really have an answer for. That's an experiment. You know, I'm going to be biased and I'm going to say, put it this way, vocally will communicate
better with a parrot.
Gesturally with the hands will communicate better with the chimpanzee.
And why is that the case?
Well, obviously, because the parrot can learn hundreds of learned vocalizations, right?
Some species can go over a thousand, right?
Whereas the chimp can't.
But the chimpanzee can learn to do sign gestures
and can understand those words.
And so you can communicate with limited sign language,
if you wanna call it, with a chimpanzee.
You've mentioned sign language a couple of times,
which is fascinating to me.
I spoke sign language with my close cousin
when we grew up, but it was signed English.
It was the grammar of English.
And when she got older and started going to deaf schools,
she spoke a different grammar, American sign language,
which I find really hard to follow.
And so how much is sign language truly a language,
which I mean, I'm very biased.
I've seen every evidence in the world
that it's truly a language.
And in what sense is it a representation of speech?
Yeah, that's really a good question. And there are definitely people studying this. And so
I think sign language is a language. It's a form of learned communication that involves
movement but not the movement of the larynx. Although I wanted my colleagues to separate out the difference between moving
the oral musculature and signing to try to pull apart what's going on in the
brain with those two behaviors.
When I talk to people who study sign language, they say it's almost impossible.
It's hard to not move the mouth and sign at the same time.
And so I think because of that, there is a connection
behaviorally and evolutionarily in speech
and signing in humans.
What's interesting is when you teach gorillas
and chimpanzees to sign, I don't see good evidence
that they're moving their oral musculature.
And so in them, maybe it's not as connected evolutionarily, but in humans it is.
But that would mean that the oral movement part in humans came after the signing.
And why might that be the case?
It goes back to what I was saying earlier, the brain pathways for producing spoken language
are embedded in brain pathways controlling learned movement, including, I believe, signing.
Some people say they actually intertwine.
And I think this happened by a whole brain pathway duplication, where the whole motor
learning circuitry that controls the gesturing and other body movements replicated itself
and got connected to the vocal organs.
And this is also partly why they're connected.
Interesting because you've also mentioned
understanding disorders in human beings
and how to address certain disorders.
So it's my understanding that about 8% of children
in the US have some speech disorders
or issues related to swallowing, which is connected.
More than 3 million Americans stutter.
How is your work relevant to human disorders,
both the study and also the treatment?
Yeah.
So this is why, actually, songbirds
have been funded by the National Institute of Health
as a model for not only studying the basic science of speech,
but also disorders.
And what's interesting is because
of the convergent genetic changes we see in humans and songbirds
and parrots as well, the underlying genes and brain circuitry, when something happens
that's wrong with them, right, you get a similar disorder.
So convergent function is associated with convergent disorders.
An example is the most famous gene is FOXP2,
discovered by a colleague of mine, Simon Fisher and others. This gene is one of those regulatory
genes that modifies the expression or the amount of protein product of other genes in the brain
that are involved in connections. And when this gene, one copy of it is disrupted in humans,
this gene, one copy of it is disrupted in humans, those people have difficulty learning how to imitate speech.
So their speech is very limited to a few words at a time, and even the words are hard to
form, but they can understand speech well.
That must be frustrating.
That's right, yeah.
And so when you manipulate this gene in songbirds, you can get similar deficits in communication
and learning, vocalizations.
If you manipulate this gene in mice, what was interesting is that we got effects on
the vocal behavior just not as dramatic.
So that was consistent with the continuum hypothesis.
And so what I think is going on there is that old genes with us in vertebrates for millions
of years have now become genetically modified to enhance these and make these novel brain
circuits for vocal communication.
And some of these old genes that even if you touch them a little bit and mess around with
them a little bit, they have a dramatic effect on vocal communication in us vocal learners,
but not as dramatic effect on other behaviors.
Does this suggest that the path forward is gene therapy as opposed to say physical therapy?
Yeah, so I think both do work.
Gene therapy is actually becoming not only possible, it's happening in humans.
It's amazing.
I wouldn't have said it before, but I'll say it now.
I wouldn't be surprised at some point in the future, whether I'm around or not, we'll be
having gene therapy for some type of speech disorders and physical therapy on top of that.
So speech is a heavily controlled genetic behavior, but it's also a culturally
controlled behavior. And if you practice, you can overcome some of the genetic handicaps.
You mentioned that you were once a dancer. Did being a dancer spark your scientific interests?
I won't say being a dancer sparked my scientific interest, but being a dancer prepared me to
be a scientist.
I now realize that the discipline that I learned as a dancer in terms of practicing until you
make it perfect, failing a lot before you succeed, that's not quite a nine-to-five job,
being creative, all of these things is really what a scientist needs to be.
And so whenever I have some dancers or passionate artists
want to join my group, I know they're prepared.
They have what it takes.
I've always wanted to know if being a great dancer
or being a great singer is more in the mind
or more in the instrument, the body.
Yeah, I think it's a combination of three things, right? It's the mind and the body. Yeah. I think it's a combination of three things, right?
It's the mind and the body working together.
I don't think everybody's vocal chords is the same.
Everybody's brain is not the same.
Just like we're diverse people in terms of height, color,
and so forth, there's diversity in our body and our brains
as well.
However, that doesn't mean automatically
it's gonna make somebody a great singer
and somebody else not.
There's one thing also controlled by the mind
that you really need to be great at almost anything,
discipline, right?
And you need that discipline to really perfect
your instrument, whatever part of that body is gonna be,
to do well at it.
That discipline, who knows?
It could be genetically controlled as well.
And so I'm going to say that's the ultimate part,
is the discipline.
And a question that we like to ask at The Joy of Why
is what about your research brings you joy?
Oh, that makes sense, The Joy of Why, yes.
Yes. I'm one of those people that really just like learning.
And it's one reason why I went into science because I'm always learning something new.
I'm always involved in learning something new and involved in discovery.
I believe my science and scientists in general were doing good for the planet.
We're doing good for society.
I know there's a lot of science advancements
that led to toxins in the environment that cause cancer.
I don't like that, you know?
But I know we scientists also can do something about that.
My mother always told me when I was growing up,
do something that has a positive impact on society.
And I felt I can do that best as a scientist.
And so I get joy out of just knowing that I'm helping, you know,
that what I'm doing some day is going to be helpful to somebody.
I'll just add one thing, because you're an astrophysicist,
once I decided I was not going to be a professional dancer,
although I still dance, I was going to be a scientist.
Then I was considering the origins of the universe with my interests
or how the brain works. And a number of years later, I've been getting invitations to Astrophysicists
Conference, American Society, something like that for astrophysicists. And I'm fascinated
by the common interest here of advanced behavior and language and evolution and what astrophysicists
are interested in. So I'm hoping that our conversation together here
is a sign of more of a marriage between those two fields.
I love the idea of working on a biology project now and then.
It's become such a fascinating field
with the advances in genetics.
I can see why there's that comparison.
It reminds me of the Emily Dickinson poem,
which I'm probably going to butcher, but it's something like, the mind is wider than the sky because the
one contains the other with ease and more besides. Thank you so much, Eric. We've been
speaking with neuroscientist Eric Jarvis. It's been such a pleasure to have you. Thank you
for joining us.
You're welcome. And it's been great talking with you. Thank you for joining us. You're welcome. And it's been great talking with you. Thanks for listening. If you're enjoying The Joy of Why and you're not already
subscribed, hit the subscribe or follow button where you're listening.
You can also leave a review for the show.
It helps people find this podcast.
The Joy of Why is a podcast from Quantum Magazine, an editorially independent publication supported
by the Simons Foundation.
Funding decisions by the Simons Foundation have no influence on the selection of topics,
guests, or other editorial decisions in this podcast or in Quantum Magazine.
The Joy of Why is produced by PRX Productions.
The production team is Caitlin Folds,
Livia Brock, Genevieve Sponsler, and Merritt Jacob. The executive producer of
PRX Productions is Jocelyn Gonzalez. Morgan Church and Edwin Ochoa provided
additional assistance. From Quanta Magazine, John Rennie and Thomas Lin
provided editorial guidance with support from Matt Carlstrom, Samwell Velasco,
Arlene Santana, and Megan Wilcoxon.
Samir Patel is Quanta's editor-in-chief.
Our theme music is from APM Music.
Julian Lin came up with the podcast name.
The episode art is by Peter Greenwood,
and our logo is by Jackie King and Christina Armitage.
Special thanks to the Columbia Journalism School and Bert Odom-Reed at the Cornell Broadcast
Studios.
I'm your host, Jana Levin.
If you have any questions or comments for us, please email us at quanta at simonsfoundation.org.
Thanks for listening.
From PRX. simonsfoundation.org. Thanks for listening.