The Sean McDowell Show - Is the Soul Real? A Neurosurgeon Makes the Case
Episode Date: May 23, 2025Many scientists and doctors believe that there is no such thing as the soul. Dr. Michael Egnor is a neurosurgeon presents the case that the brain alone does not explain the mind. Using modern neurosci...ence and his vast surgical experience, he believes there is a spiritual soul that transcends the brain, using fascinating case studies to prove his claim. Today, he's here to discuss his new book comes out June 3rd. READ: The Immortal Mind: A Neurosurgeon’s Case for the Existence of the Soul Hardcover by Michael Egnor (https://theimmortalmind.org/)*Get a MASTERS IN APOLOGETICS or SCIENCE AND RELIGION at BIOLA (https://bit.ly/3LdNqKf)*USE Discount Code [SMDCERTDISC] for 25% off the BIOLA APOLOGETICS CERTIFICATE program (https://bit.ly/3AzfPFM)*See our fully online UNDERGRAD DEGREE in Bible, Theology, and Apologetics: (https://bit.ly/448STKK)FOLLOW ME ON SOCIAL MEDIA: Twitter: https://x.com/Sean_McDowellTikTok: https://www.tiktok.com/@sean_mcdowell?lang=enInstagram: https://www.instagram.com/seanmcdowell/Website: https://seanmcdowell.org
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Is the scientific evidence for the soul overwhelming?
Our guest today is a former materialist who rejected belief in the soul,
but because of his work and research as a surgeon,
he now believes the scientific evidence is compelling.
Why did he change his mind and what is that evidence?
Well, you're about to find out.
We welcome for the first time Dr. Michael Egner,
author of the new book that I endorsed and found
fascinating. It's called The Immortal Mind. Dr. Eggner, thanks for joining us today.
Thank you, Sean. It's a pleasure to be here.
Well, the book starts with this fascinating, jarring story that when I read it, I went to my wife and I was like,
you've got to hear this story. It's somewhat of a conversion story
before you get into the scientific evidence for the soul.
Since you start the book there,
let's start this interview
with your powerful conversion story.
Sure.
I was born into a kind of a non-religious family as a child
and I fell in love with science at a very early age.
And I thought that all the mysteries of life, all the really meaningful things that one could
discover could be discovered with science. So I believed in the Darwinian way of understanding
man and, you know, physics and chemistry and all those things. and I majored in biochemistry in college and I wanted to be a doctor and I really wanted to be to be a neurosurgeon
Because I wanted to understand the brain and the mind because I thought that I would understand on a deep level
Who we are and what we are if I knew all the science there was to know about the brain
So I graduated from medical school, I trained in neurosurgery,
and graduated from my residency in 1991, and went out and practiced as a neurosurgeon.
And a number of things happened to me as a neurosurgeon and as a husband and a father
that changed the way I look at the world. And as a neurosurgeon, I began finding patients
who had large parts of their brains missing.
A good example is a little girl who was born
with only about half her brain.
And I counseled her family that this didn't look good.
I couldn't be sure how she would do,
but it didn't look encouraging. And couldn't be sure how she would do, but it didn't look encouraging.
And as she grew up, I followed her
and she was normal at every stage of development.
In fact, rather bright.
She's now in her mid twenties
and a vibrant, normal young woman has a great job.
Her mother says she's too smart for her own good.
And she has half a brain.
I have a number of other patients with major parts of their brains missing who were really okay, who were really fine. And that didn't fit
any of the neuroanatomy textbooks that I read in medical school. There were strange things going
on there. The whole thing kind of came to a head when I was operating many years ago on a woman who had a
brain tumor in her left frontal lobe. And we do awake brain surgery at times. And what awake brain
surgery means is that the patient is given a local anesthetic like Novocaine, so they don't feel pain.
The brain itself has no sensation of pain. So it's just like cutting your hair,
you don't feel anything.
And we can stimulate the surface of the brain
and get a map of the critical parts of the brain.
So for example, if I'm removing a tumor,
I can find out what parts I have to protect
and what parts I can safely remove to get the tumor out.
So it makes the operation safer.
So she was awake and I was taking
out the tumor and taking out much of the left frontal lobe of her brain and we were having
a conversation. And it was a perfectly normal conversation. It's the same kind of conversation
you and I are having here right now as her big part of her brain is coming out. And it
didn't seem to make any difference in her conversation. Fortunately, that was the whole point of the surgery was
to keep her healthy. And I, after that operation, I thought, my goodness gracious, we don't
understand what the brain does. There's something deeper here. Because none of my textbooks
said you could take out most of the left frontal lobe and have a person be perfectly normal. So at around the same time, I had a religious conversion, one might say.
And it was related intellectually to some of my doubts about Darwinism. I began reading
more deeply about the Darwinian explanation for how we became human. And it struck me as pretty weak science.
I thought it was pretty bad science.
I didn't look at it at that point
from a religious standpoint.
I just thought of all the scientific disciplines I knew,
this is the one that I think was the least rigorous.
But I have four kids.
And when my youngest son was born,
the first couple of months of life, he wasn't making eye contact and he wasn't smiling.
And he wasn't behaving as if he understood my wife and I
to be people.
We were like objects to him.
And I became frightened that he was autistic.
And autism has always terrified me.
The idea that you have a child who you love
and want to connect with,
and they don't even know you exist.
You're just like a piece of furniture to them.
So I was quite frightened of it.
We took him to some neurologists and they said,
it's too young, he's just a few months old,
you really can't be sure.
So by the time he was about six months old,
but he still wasn't making eye contact,
it wasn't really interacting with us.
And I was seeing a consult, a patient at an outside hospital, it was a Catholic
hospital. And it was, I was at a point where I was emotionally really, really diswrought
by this. You know, I was thinking, I'm never going to love my son, I'm never going to have
my son love me, he's never gonna know me.
And so after I saw the consult, it was late at night, there was a chapel in the hospital.
So I was stopped by the chapel.
And I went into the chapel and I got down in front
of the altar and prayed and I said, God,
I don't know if you exist, I don't make praying a habit,
but if you do exist, could you take this away
from me, please? This is something I can't deal with. I can't have my son be
autistic and not know me. And I heard a voice. Only time in my life I've ever
heard a voice. And it was very clear. And it wasn't me saying it. And the voice
said, but that's what you're doing to me. Whew. So I said, I kind of collapsed and I said,
okay, I won't do it to you anymore.
I won't be autistic to you.
But please make my son not autistic to me.
So I went home and a few days later
was his six month birthday and he was perfectly normal
It's pretty fine. He's currently a law student and anything but autistic
so
so I
That complemented in some sense my
discoveries as a surgeon and in neuroscience that there's a lot more to man than just meat,
that we do have souls, that there's a spiritual aspect to us. And so I converted to Christianity,
got baptized, many people in my family also got baptized. And since I had spent several decades of my life
as an atheist and a materialist,
I kind of feel like I have a debt to repay.
We wouldn't call it vengeance,
but I wanna convey to people that atheism
and materialism are wrong and that God exists
and that he's real and that we really do have souls.
So I began looking more deeply into the neuroscience of the brain and the soul to try to explain
why I had seen these strange things with people with parts of their brains missing were perfectly
normal.
Then I can take out part of a woman's frontal lobe as I'm talking to her and it doesn't change her.
And I found that there were other neurosurgeons
and other scientists who really had seen
the same kind of thing, that I wasn't the first person
to notice this.
The greatest scientist in the neurosurgical profession
was a guy named Wilder Penfield,
who worked back in the mid 20th century in Montreal.
And he pioneered the surgery of epilepsy.
And one of his most common operations
was awake brain surgery,
the sort of thing that I had done
with the woman with the tumor.
And he did about 1,100 patients with awake brain surgery.
So it was, no one's ever come anywhere near those kind of numbers.
Awake brain surgery is still done today,
but if you do a few hundred in your life,
it's a large volume.
So Penfield really was responsible
for a lot of the mapping of the brain
that we know about today,
because he operated on all these over a thousand people and mapped all
kinds of areas of their brain. He would map them by stimulating with a little
electrical probe the surface of the brain while they were awake and he would
ask them what they felt or what they saw or see if they moved. And he found
peculiar things and they really surprised him because he had started
out as a materialist just like I had.
He found that when he would stimulate the brain and he must have stimulated the brain
at least a million times in these 1,100 patients.
Well if you count out say two stimulations a minute and the operations last about eight
hours I actually did the math.
Yeah. It's over the math. Yeah.
It's over a million stimulations.
He could only stimulate four things from the brain.
He could make the person move.
He could stimulate the motor area
and they'd raise their arm.
He could make them have a perception.
Let's see a flash of light or smell something
or hear something. He could make them have an emotion. There
are parts of the brain, if you stimulate it, you get a powerful emotion. You get fear or
joy. There's a part of the brain that makes you laugh. If you stimulate that, you feel
like everything is funny. And he could stimulate memories. He could stimulate certain parts
of the temporal lobe,
and you'd have a memory of your mother's face or a memory
of a conversation that you'd had years before.
But those were the only things he could stimulate.
And what he left out is everything we're doing right now.
He couldn't stimulate abstract thought.
He couldn't stimulate the ability to contemplate philosophy or to think about the meaning of life. He couldn't stimulate abstract thought. He couldn't stimulate the ability to contemplate philosophy
or to think about the meaning of life.
He couldn't stimulate mathematics.
Nowhere in the brain could he stimulate someone to say,
ah, one plus one is two.
He couldn't stimulate logic or history or music.
All the different kind of abstract higher level things
we do. And he came to the conclusion
because he was a pretty honest man.
He said, well, maybe I can't stimulate abstract thought
because it doesn't come from the brain.
Maybe the brain is necessary ordinarily
for the exercise of abstract thought.
If you drink too much alcohol or if you get hit on the head,
you may not think as clearly for a while. But that doesn't mean that the thought comes
from the brain. So he said that he called abstract thought mind action. He said that
you can't stimulate mind action from the brain. You can stimulate individual things, movement,
perception, emotion, and memory, but that's it.
And then he looked back at the whole record of seizures, epilepsy. Looked back at thousands of
cases of epilepsy, and he found exactly the same thing. When people have epileptic seizures,
they do certain things. They can go unconscious, but not always. Sometimes they're awake when they
have the seizure and they have movements or they have perceptions
Sometimes you have memories. Sometimes you have emotions, but there are no calculus seizures
no one's ever no one's ever had a seizure where they couldn't stop doing mathematics and
Panfield said why not if if mathematics comes from the brain and there have been hundreds of millions of seizures
Once in a blue moon, you'd trigger off one plus one
equals two, and you couldn't stop.
He said, it never happens.
So the same thing that he found with seizures
is what he had found with the awake brain surgery,
that the brain is an organ that only does certain things.
And the main things it does is it makes us move,
it makes us perceive, it makes us perceive, makes us remember and gives us emotions.
But it doesn't serve as the source for abstract thought.
And so that was a remarkable finding
and it's been pretty much ignored
within the neuroscience community.
You read textbooks and people talk about it.
I've had debates with neuroscientists
where they say,
oh, that Penfield study, I mean, that was 100 years ago.
You know, we have much, much better studies now.
And the reality is we don't have much better studies now.
These were on 1,100 people, over a million stimulations
by a first rate scientist who studied the results.
And nothing we do nowadays comes anywhere near
that kind of detail and that kind of rigor.
So Penfield's work, I think is amazing.
He wrote a book called Mystery of the Mind,
if viewers are interested in that,
it's available on Amazon,
where he recounts all of his experiences.
He also found that free will doesn't come from the brain. And the way he
found that was he would ask the person during the operation, just raise your arm occasionally,
just by free will. And what I'll do is I'll stimulate occasionally the part of your brain
that controls your arm and I'll make it raise. and I want you to tell me when your arm goes up did you do it or did I do it and they always knew there were tens of thousands of
stimulations he could never simulate free will he said well it's because free will doesn't come
from the brain it's it's a it's a spiritual power it's not a physical power. So that got me to thinking and it
it made sense in light of what I was seeing in my own patients and so I
looked deeper into into the neuroscience. And I'm happy to talk about that.
Yeah let's go into some of these details. It's amazing that it's kind of this
coalescing together of this personal spiritual journey and the neuroscience.
It's not one or the other.
There's kind of this intersection of the two.
Let me take a step back.
You said this kind of earlier towards the beginning about the person with half a brain
who functioned normally and was even bright.
And you said something referring to kind of neuroanatomy of textbooks not
preparing you to expect this. What is the worldview that's often in neuroanatomy
textbooks? Why as materialists did you not expect this?
As a backstory to that there is a there was a science of phrenology of reading the bumps on a person's skull to
understand their personality and understand their brain that was developed back mainly
in the 19th century a little bit into the early 20th century and it was it was people
think of it as a pseudoscience it was was kind of crazy. You would feel the shape of the skull and say,
this person's an honest man or something.
But phrenology wasn't quite as crazy as it seemed.
It was wrong.
But they didn't have radiology.
They didn't have CAT scans or anything.
So they couldn't image the brain.
The skull does slightly, in some way,
conform to the shape of the brain.
So that was the only way they could have
to evaluate the brain.
And what phrenologists were facing was,
there was increasing evidence back then
that certain areas of the brain controlled movement
and perception and emotion and memory.
That was becoming clear even in the 19th century.
So they figured everything in the mind
was controlled in the brain somewhere.
So they started looking for honesty and integrity
and social deviance and they figured,
well, you know, I'll see if I can find that.
So it wasn't a completely irrational scientific project.
It failed.
It turned out that there is no part of the brain
where you have honesty or integrity
that doesn't come from the brain like that.
But, so it was abandoned, but many philosophers of the mind
have pointed out that modern neuroscience
is kind of like high tech phrenology.
That is that neuroscientists are saying,
well, you know, the frontal lobes are the processing areas
for abstract thought.
You know, so it's kind of another version of phrenology.
In fact, we use functional MRI imaging,
which is a method of MRI imaging
where you can see areas of the brain that light up
when a person is thinking about certain things.
And that's a lot like phrenology in a sense that you're looking for the spot that lights up
to tell us about your character or about what you're thinking and things like that.
And that's actually pretty weak science, and I think of it as high-tech phrenology.
So what I think the textbooks have gotten wrong is
that they don't understand the relationship between the soul and the brain, and they don't
understand how the brain is wired. There are certain parts of the brain that are hardwired,
that it's like your computer. If you damage your computer in a particular spot, you can
predict what's going to happen. And that's very hardwired for the motor areas, the sensory areas, the
speech area, the brain stem. If you damage one square millimeter of that part of the
brain, you can predict with great accuracy what's going to happen. But probably about
half the brain, maybe even two thirds, is what are called association areas
that are not like that at all.
You can remove much of them and be perfectly normal.
And the textbooks kind of lose that.
They don't realize that there are two kinds of,
there are two kinds of powers of the mind that people have.
There's the abstract thought.
Its relation to the brain is quite
nebulous. We don't have a good handle on that. And there's concrete things like moving and
perceiving and breathing and those, the concrete things are in the brain. We can find them.
We know where they are. The abstract stuff really isn't in the brain in the same way
and neuroscience hasn't caught up to that truth.
Mm.
Okay, so I want to make sure that I understand
and our viewers are tracking with this.
You made a distinction between kind of hard areas in the brain,
specific areas tied to functioning,
and those are damaged.
Humans can't perform those functions.
And then there's association areas
where if they're maybe damaged,
other areas the brain, you know, with the soul we would argue, can compensate for that.
Is the argument that from a materialist position there should be no association areas and everything
should be reducible in principle to certain areas of the brain because that's all we are is the
brain and matter.
So like a computer, you remove that part, it doesn't function.
Is that accurate?
Yes, that's a reasonable way of looking at it.
The term, one way of describing these areas is the areas of the brain that have very well
defined functions are often called eloquent regions.
And non-eloquent regions are the regions that don't have so much function.
People describe the non-eloquent regions as being association areas.
But that's because they don't know what they do.
So you just say it's association areas.
But you can remove them.
And you still associate just fine. So it's not even clear what they mean's association areas. Gotcha. But you can remove them and you still associate just fine.
So it's not even clear what they mean by association areas.
I mean, it's supposedly it's where coordination
and organization takes place.
But in any machine that has coordination
or organization going on,
if you damage the coordination and organization,
you substantially damage the machine.
But that's often not the case with the brain.
I mean, the anterior left frontal lobe
is considered an association area.
And I removed it in a woman as I was talking to her,
and she remained exactly the same.
So I don't even know what is meant by association area.
I think it's kind of a hand waving thing,
like it's a mystery to us,
so we'll call it association area. I think it's kind of a hand-waving thing like it's a mystery to us, so we'll call it association area.
That's really helpful. Okay, so let me ask you one more question before we dive into the evidence.
There's these split brain experiments you talk about, totally fascinating. You get into near-death
experiences, damaged brains, we're going to get there. But I was asked a question this past week.
I was speaking at my alma mater where I did my doctorate,
Southern Baptist Theological Seminary,
talking about the evidence for a soul.
And a student asked me this question.
He said, I'm engaging a friend of mine who's a skeptic,
doesn't believe in the soul,
and he points out how in certain areas of the brain are damaged,
the personality changes.
Doesn't that show that we are reducible to the brain
and there's no soul?
Now I had an answer that I gave him,
but how would you address that kind of objection
that I'm sure you've heard before?
Sure.
Well, I mean, it's obvious that certain areas of the brain
can be damaged and you do have a change in personality.
You find that just by drinking a glass of wine.
I mean, you know, I mean, you know,
the brain is a physical organ and emotion,
which is very much a part of our personality
is controlled by the brain.
So there are emotional centers.
So yeah, sure, you can change things.
The most famous person that people talk about
who had brain damage and a change is Phineas Gage,
who had a spike driven through his brain.
And he was a church going kind of conservative guy
before it happened and was a little bit more
of a libertine afterwards.
So he still wasn't such a bad guy.
I've actually read a bit about him.
He wasn't that bad, but he kind
of lived the high life a little bit after.
But that's not news.
That's not surprising.
You know, as I said, you have a few drinks of wine,
and you're going to behave differently.
So, there's no question that there's a component of us
that is lodged in our brain, that controls our emotional tone.
That's sort of the emotional background
of how we live our life.
That doesn't mean that we don't have souls.
And then it gets into the question
of how do you define a soul.
And it's, I'm happy to go into that now.
It's a little bit of Aristotle and Thomas Aquinas,
so it gets a little bit of Aristotle and Thomas Aquinas, so it gets a little deep, but
we tend to think of souls as like ghosty things that kind of inhabit our bodies, like they're the
same shape as us, but they step outside of us and they're translucent or something, and that's not
really the classical way of understanding the soul, and at least in many Christian perspectives, not the Christian way. St. Thomas Aquinas
believed that the soul was essentially everything about you that makes you different from a
dead body. So that everything a living body does that a dead body doesn't do is what the
soul is. So the fact that I'm talking now is part of my soul, because if I were dead
I wouldn't talk. That I'm breathing, it's also part of my soul, because if I were dead
I wouldn't breathe. So the soul basically was the thing by virtue of which we are alive.
It's what makes us a living thing rather than a dead thing. And that includes in plants,
and plants have souls, their souls make them grow and reproduce and take nourishment
and things like that. They're living things. Animals have souls. They make my dog like
food and walk around and bark and do dog things. And human beings have souls. Our souls differ
from the souls of any other living thing in that our souls are rational souls. They were rational, that's the abstract thought part that no other living thing has. And that
kind of soul is a spiritual soul. And therefore our soul is a spirit. Whereas other living
things just have souls that run their bodies. You know, souls that run the plant, souls
that run the dog. Our soul isn't that
kind of soul. Our soul is a spirit. And a spirit has many characteristics that are quite
different. Our spirits have the ability to reason, to act freely, to do free will. Our
spiritual souls are immortal. You can't kill a spirit. You can kill a body. You can even kill
a soul in the sense of if the body disintegrates, the soul that gave it life is no longer present
because it's disintegrated. But if you have a spiritual soul, spirits can't disintegrate
because they don't have parts. They're not made of matter. You can't break them up.
disintegrate because they don't have parts. They're not made of matter. You can't break them up
God could if he chose make them not exist, but they can't die in the way that everything else dies
Which I think is the most powerful argument for the immortality of the soul
The least most powerful scientific argument for the immortality of the soul is that our is a spiritual soul, we have capacity for abstract thought and free will.
Therefore, we can't disintegrate as a soul,
and we can't die, our body disintegrates,
and we will get new bodies.
So let's jump into some of the evidence on this for the soul.
I responded to this young man, I said,
you know, if we just are our brains,
we'd expect a change of brains to affect our personality.
But if we are body and soul,
and our soul operates through our bodies,
we would expect changes of the body
to affect the way the soul operates through the body.
So, Phineas Gage fits with both explanations,
so alone cannot be used as evidence for one versus the other.
That's how I look at it, but you walked through in this book,
the split brain experiments.
This blew me away. I was trying to explain it to my wife.
I thought this was so interesting.
Walk through what happened and why you think it points
towards the reality of the immaterial soul.
Yeah, the split brain research is just utterly fascinating.
There are certain kinds of seizures,
relatively rare seizures, that start out as a tiny seizure
on one side of the brain.
That doesn't bother the patient
very much, but that then can travel over the entire brain.
And if it travels over the entire brain,
you have a generalized seizure.
And they can be really nasty seizures.
There are people who have 30 or 40 of them a day,
and they often don't respond well to medication.
So they're constantly having these horrible seizures.
There was research back in the 1940s on animals with that kind of seizure to try to find a
way to stop those seizures.
And what the surgeons would do is they decided, well, if they cut the connection between the
hemispheres of the brain, then the seizure couldn't spread and maybe the person would
be better off.
And it seemed to help the animals.
So they tried it on people.
So by the 1950s and 60s, it became a fairly standard operation. I performed it. And what
you cut is the corpus callosum, which is a bundle of 200,000 nerve axons. I forgot the
number actually. And it's about the size of the palm
of your hand and it's connecting the two hemispheres of the brain. And you cut the whole thing
from front to back and that stops those kind of seizures. Patients may have small seizures
but they live a much, much better life. And What I found and what other neurosurgeons who've done that operation have found is that surprisingly these people are normal
After the surgery you'd think that cutting your brain in half
Would have a quite radical effect. Yeah who you are you might be two people, you know
So it'd be kind of like if you took a chainsaw to your computer and cut it right down the middle
And still work just fine
Think that there's something odd about your computer. You didn't you didn't expect that
so, um,
I if you were to meet a person who had had a corpus callus on me
Which is the name of the operation you couldn't tell you your these are perfectly normal people and you ask them
Well, do you feel
like you have two selves? I says, no, no, I'm still me. So there was a neuroscientist named
Roger Sperry back in the middle of the 20th century, who realized that even though these
people seem completely normal, it's a wonderful experiment in a way to separate the hemispheres.
And he did very detailed research to try to find out, well, is there anything abnormal
about them? And what he found was that some of their perceptual abilities were split.
That is, if you show a picture, the left hemisphere of the brain is usually the hemisphere where
we have language from. The right hemisphere can't speak. So if you show a
picture to the left hemisphere and you can do it by putting it in a certain place of the visual
field that only the left hemisphere can see, the person can say what's in the picture. But if you
show the same picture to the right hemisphere, the person can't say it, but they know what it is.
They can pick the object out of a basket if you show an apple to the right hemisphere.
The person will say I know what it is but I can't say it and if you give him a basket
of fruit he'll pick out the apple. So there are all kinds of weird things that Sperry
found like that that people aren't even aware of in everyday life. And he won the Nobel
Prize for it. It was his classic research thing, 1972, I think,
Nobel Prize in Medicine.
But Sperry himself noticed, noted that these people sure seem normal, you know,
I can find subtle perceptual stuff.
So two researchers since then have carried
out very profound studies on these patients. The first was Justine Surgeon,
who was a neuroscientist at McGill back 1980s.
Took split brain patients,
and she would put arrows in their visual fields.
So one hemisphere of the brain would see one arrow,
the other hemisphere of the brain would see the other arrow.
And she would ask them a question, and the question she would ask is, are the arrows pointing in the same
direction? And the patient's almost always got it right. But think about this. One hemisphere
saw one arrow, the other hemisphere saw the other arrow. No part of the brain saw both
arrows. So how can a person compare them? So that general approach has been by a guy
named Jere Pinto, neuroscientist in the Netherlands. And Pinto presents patients with visual stories.
It will have like one part of the story is in one hemisphere, the other part of the story
is in the other hemisphere, but the story doesn't make any sense
unless you can integrate these two.
An example, I don't know, I'm not sure
if this is when he's done, but this is a kind of,
this is the kind of thing he's done.
Where one hemisphere will get a picture
of a broken window with like a hole in it.
The other hemisphere will get a picture of a baseball.
And you ask what happened?
And people with split brain surgery will say,
oh, well the baseball broke the window.
But no part of the person's brain has seen the window
and the baseball.
So what is it that is doing the comparing?
And that's the spirit.
That's the spiritual soul.
That's the part that's not from the brain.
So I think that surgeon and Pinto's research
on split brain patients actually points out
the spiritual soul.
You can actually in some sense see it, see it intuitively.
That's the part of the mind that can compare things
that no part of the brain has access to both together.
So I find that fascinating.
It's totally fascinating.
So what if a skeptic pushed back and said,
okay, I think you're making a kind of God of the gaps, or in this case, we might say,
soul of the gaps argument.
Just because we don't know what physical mechanism helps explain this connection
between the hemispheres and the brain, maybe it's something else in the body. I mean, there
could be some other unknown natural phenomena that explains this and the Penfield experiments.
Why do we have to punt towards something spiritual to explain this? This is a soul of the gaps akin to the god of the gaps.
Right.
And that's exactly the argument they use.
I've debated this with materialists and that's exactly what they say.
Carl Sagan made one of the dumbest claims.
Sagan was an atheist and a materialist
when he said that absence of evidence
is not evidence of absence.
But that's not true, right?
The fact is that if you don't have evidence for something, it's perfectly
reasonable to draw the preliminary conclusion that it doesn't exist. In fact, there's been a
lot of that in science. I mean, there's no evidence for the luminiferous ether in the
outer space. That was a big theory back in the 19th century. There's no evidence for vitalism,
that there's a chemical that makes you alive,
that that chemical is different from anything else.
There's no evidence for a ton of theories.
And we accept that as evidence that theory isn't true.
So absence of evidence is evidence for absence.
It may not be proof, but it's evidence. And so what I say to neuroscientists
who say, oh, no, we'll be able to figure this out. You just give us time. Science always
progresses is have at it, have at it. Get back to me when you've got the evidence. Until
you have the evidence, you have nothing. So yeah, I mean you can always say hey my theory is correct
Just give me another hundred years and I'll prove it
Okay, you know have at it, you know, but until then
By far the best scientific explanation for the kind of research that that that that we're discussing is that the spiritual soul exists
And maybe neuroscientists will pull something out of their pocket.
Good luck.
Hmm.
But they haven't pulled it out yet.
They haven't yet? Okay.
So let's talk a little bit more about the section in your book on terminal lucidity.
Now I've done some programs on this, on deathbed experiences with Dr. Steve Miller.
We've covered this, but I thought it was fascinating fascinating that came up in your book as a piece of evidence
for the soul
So talk about what you mean about that why you think that points towards the soul
well terminal lucidity or I have a
Friend a Stephen Post who's a
Neuroethicist who's written a lot about this. So we talk
a lot about it. He's fascinated by it. And he actually likes calling it paradoxical lucidity,
because it's not always terminal. It's often terminal. A person has late stages of Alzheimer's,
a day or two before they pass away, they can have an interval of time, sometimes a couple
of hours, where they're all self again, and then they pass away. they can have an interval of time, sometimes a couple hours, where they're all self again,
and then they pass away.
But it doesn't have to be like that.
That is that there are people
who have these episodes of lucidity.
And the lucidity can be rather striking,
that you can have a person who's really not even verbal,
who after years of being like that,
suddenly begins having conversations.
There's usually an emotional
component to it, meaning usually it's with a loved one, a family member, and they'll
start reminiscing about their childhood and so on, and then it goes away. And from a materialist
standpoint, that's awful hard to put together. I mean, you have an Alzheimer's ravaged brain. There's no reason that you would expect
to just have that intermittently go away.
It's again, it's as if your computer is massively damaged
and it really hardly works at all.
And then all of a sudden it's perfect.
That's pretty odd.
This is not the way I would expect my computer to work.
So what terminal lucidity points to, it's not proof.
In science, you don't really get proof.
You get inference to best explanation.
What terminal lucidity points to
is that there's an aspect to us
that isn't locked into the damaged brain so much.
It can come out. And why it comes out, how it comes out, is that there's an aspect to us that isn't locked into the damaged brain so much.
It can come out and why it comes out, how it comes out, that's grist for the mill.
That's something else to understand.
But terminal acidity is certainly not something that a materialist understanding of the brain would predict.
Interesting.
Okay.
Talk a little bit about the section in your book on what are described as vegetative states,
sometimes permanent vegetative states, and how...
Yeah, go for it.
There's a landmark paper published, I think it was 2006,
in the New England Journal of Medicine called Detecting Awareness in the Vegetative State.
And it was a paper published by a guy named Adrian Owen,
who was a neuroscientist at Cambridge University in England.
Now he's, I think, working in Indiana.
He came to the US.
And he studied a woman who was diagnosed
as being in persistent vegetative state.
This is the same state, for example,
that Terry Shiavo was in.
You may recall the Terry Shiavo case.
But she had had an automobile accident, had massive brain damage.
And several years later, it's still not woken up out of coma.
And by examination by her doctors in Cambridge, she had no mind.
That is that she was just a shell. She was a body but had no
mental processes. Her brain was shrunken down to a half or a third of its normal size. Most
of it was destroyed. And there was just nothing there. She was reflexes. She had sleep-wake
cycles but showed no evidence of understanding anything about what was going on around her or of having any thoughts at all.
So Owen put her in an MRI machine and did functional MRI imaging.
And functional MRI imaging shows brain areas lighting up if you're thinking about things.
And he put her in the machine, he put headsets on her, and he asked her to think about things.
Think about playing tennis, think about walking across the room, et cetera.
And areas of what was left of her brain lit up, which was kind of odd.
He didn't expect that.
So he took normal volunteers, 15 normal volunteers, put headphones on them, stuck them in the
MRI machine and asked them the same questions.
And they had the same patterns of brain activation as this woman in persistent vegetative state
had, which on a superficial level would lead you to think, well, that means that she's
thinking like they're thinking. So what he then did, because he's a really good scientist,
he said, well, I'm not sure this is about thinking. Maybe just the noise, the sound
coming into the ear is what's making the brain light up. Maybe understanding isn't a part of it. So
he took the normal people, put them back in the MRI machine, and he asked them the same
question but he scrambled the words so they didn't mean anything anymore. And the brains
didn't light up. He puts her back in the machine, scrambles the words, and her brain didn't light up.
Wow.
Her brain only responded when what you asked her to do made sense.
Brilliant.
What he's been able to do since then, and this has been repeated by a number of other
researchers, is you can communicate with some people in the deepest level of coma.
They will tell you about their families. They can do arithmetic.
You can ask them, what is six plus eight?
And you count, and when you reach 14, the brain lights up.
So about 40%, the latest numbers are,
of people in the deepest level of coma are able
to communicate that way.
And so that of course doesn't prove the existence
of the soul and of all the arguments I make
that we recount in the book,
this is the one that gets the most pushback
from the materialist neuroscientists.
They say, oh no, I mean, it's still the brain lighting up.
You know, it's not the soul, the brain lights up. But what this suggests is that there's a disconnect
that your brain can be ravaged, can be almost destroyed. And you're still able to do some
things, you're still able to, to think on a pretty high level. Keep in mind that this
is a person who can't move, can't talk, is probably blind,
has no ability to interact with the world otherwise
except that they can think about what you say to them.
And this phenomenon is very well known, for example,
to nurses who work in ICUs,
people who are in deep coma.
You still have to be careful about what you say around them.
It's common knowledge among ICU nurses
that in the room with a comatose patient,
you have to pretend that they're awake.
Because if you say something upsetting,
like, this guy isn't gonna make it,
the heart rate goes up, the blood pressure goes up.
People respond to what you say.
So I always tell families that
when you're in the room with a loved one who's in a coma assume they're awake,
assume they can hear you. Don't say things in their in their presence that
that you know that would upset them. So there's a disconnect between the mind
and the brain and that disconnect I, is more evidence for the soul.
One of my favorite sections of your book, I thought the split brain was just absolutely
fascinating but the section on free will, because that's something I've had debates
with skeptics at times about morality, you can't tell somebody they should or shouldn't
do something if you don't have the capacity for choice
To do it or refrain from doing it
We also have this deep-seated belief that we make choices and that we're not just acted upon externally
So any adequate worldview to me has to account for free will or at least the belief in free will
now you argue that like physics and your study of
through neurosurgery and neuroscience
points towards the existence of free will and the soul.
Explain that for us if you will.
Well, free will is real and it's obviously real.
There is no intelligent debate about it.
There is a raging debate in our
society now, but it's not an intelligent one. It's just nonsense to deny free will. One
of the best ways to empirically show that free will is real is to go over to the desk
of a free will denier and pour your coffee on his laptop
Hmm, and he will of course blame you for doing it. He's a what are you doing my laptop and you say hey
I have no freewill
I had no more choice as to whether I poured my coffee on your on the laptop than the coffee cup had
So if you're gonna yell at something yell at the coffee cup
They I've debated the freewill question with Jerry Coyne, who's
a blogger who blogs a lot. And I pointed out he had a very funny post where somebody dented
his fender in a parking lot and then drove off. So he had this post where he was complaining
about this jerk that dented his fender in a parking lot and drove off. And I pointed out and said, but Jerry,
if free will isn't real,
the person who drove off is no more responsible for that
than the car he was driving is responsible for that.
If we're all just meat machines,
you can't blame anybody for anything.
I mean, it's no one's fault.
You're just a meat machine.
So even free will deniers believe in free will.
I mean, pour your coffee on their laptop
and you find out if they believe in free will.
Because if you don't have free will,
you say, hey, I had no choice.
You know, I had to pour the coffee.
It's my chemicals still, I can't blame me.
So the first bit of evidence that free will is real
is that everyone believes in free
will.
Even free will deniers live their lives as if they believe in free will.
There's no person outside of an insane asylum who doesn't believe in free will.
The second reason for believing in free will is that free will deniers invariably invoke
determinism.
They say that free will isn't real because everything
in our brain is already determined and we,
there's no room for free will in physics.
Determinism in physics has, for the most part,
been proven false.
It was proven false by a guy named John Stuart Bell,
a physicist in Ireland back in the 1960s,
who proposed a way of testing
whether a physical state of the universe at one moment
is completely determined
in the way it transitions into the next moment.
That is, is it all baked in the way it transitions into the next moment. That is, is it all baked
in the cake or is there room for randomness or choice in this? And the experiments have
been carried out by several physicists. One of them is named Elaine Aspect. And he did
the experiments. The experiments themselves are rather complex, but very clearly showed
that determinism is false.
That is that if you look at the state of things
in your room right now,
and you say the way the room will be,
say 10 seconds from now,
is it baked into the physical arrangement of things now?
And the answer is no.
There's always, there is room for free will.
There's room for randomness.
And that's been physically shown.
So the predicate for the denial of free will
that materialists use, that determinism is the way physics
is, is simply wrong.
There's room for free will.
The third argument against free will, which I find compelling,
is that if you don't have free will, which I find compelling, is that if you don't have free will, then you are a complete slave of your chemicals and what's happening inside your brain.
In other words, you're just a chemistry set, and we don't pay attention to the opinions of chemistry sets.
So if you deny free will, you say, I don't have the freedom to choose whether I believe in
free will or not.
It's all in my chemicals.
And I don't pay attention to meat robots.
I mean, if a meat robot wants to say something to me, I'll say, it's just a meat robot.
I don't pay attention to that.
It's as if you spill ink on the floor accidentally.
And it looks like it it looks like it
Spells out it's it's it's going to snow tomorrow
Do you really believe it's gonna snow tomorrow just because the spilled ink said it no, it's just spilled egg
It's just a coincidence. However, if somebody who you trust a weatherman says it's gonna snow tomorrow you believe him
the difference between the weatherman and the spilled ink
is a weatherman has free will
and can choose right and wrong and choose opinions.
So what people who deny free will are saying is,
I'm just a meat robot, and my answer is,
why would I listen to what you say?
So the fourth reason to believe in free will
is that the neuroscience really supports it.
Wilder Penfield found that he could not stimulate the will anywhere.
He couldn't find it anywhere in the brain, so he assumed it was free.
And there's a guy named Benjamin Libet who did a lot of very interesting work in the mid-20th century.
He found that there are brain signals, electrical signals that
happen before we make decisions that superficially look as though our decisions are driven by
those signals and we didn't really have any choice. But Leibniz showed that you can veto
those signals and the veto isn't from the brain. So he called that free won't.
And he pointed out that the idea of free won't
very nicely fits the traditional Christian understanding of temptation and sin.
That is that we are tempted by things
that we don't have control over.
Our brain is telling us to do stuff all the time that we are better
off not doing. But we still retain the libertarian ability to consent or not consent. And that's
the free will. So the argument about free will in my view is not even really an argument.
The evidence is overwhelming that free will is real.
And in fact, we all believe free will is real, even free will deniers, because they live
their lives as if free will is real.
So of course it's real.
The point you made towards me about Penfield, I find just compelling in the sense that when
he would prick the brains in a certain place, the patient's hand would move.
But then they could distinguish between, oh, you did that, and I'm holding with my hand
back the hand moving, I did that.
So there's this recognition of internal causation and external causation from the outside in.
So it seems to be-
Precisely.
And people always know the difference. And we naturally
recognize that. So you'd have to explain somehow we've been wired through this
blind evolutionary materialist process to be able to make this distinction for
some survival advantage. Like I don't even know why we would need the belief
that we have free will in the first place
Let alone the belief of making a distinction between internal and external causation
That's to me where it becomes just implausible
well the the the the spiritual qualities of the soul that we've talked about which are
Intellect and free will there's the compassion the capacity for reason and the capacity to choose freely, couldn't have evolved anyway, because they're not physical.
Evolution, to the extent that it's a real thing,
and I think overall, the Darwinian way of looking at
evolutionary changes is just junk science,
I think it's terrible science.
But even if that were true, you couldn't evolve free will
and you couldn't evolve the capacity for reason
because free will and reason are not physical things.
And if evolution is true, it can only affect physical things
because physical things are genes
and natural selection and all that stuff.
So this spiritual soul of man is created.
It's not evolved.
It can't be evolved.
Some parts of our body may have evolved.
That's open to debate.
But not the spiritual soul.
It can't evolve.
You're really getting to the root of what a materialistic process could,
in principle, explain.
So, if ink fell on the ground and said, you know, like,
take out the trash or it's gonna rain,
we know it's not gonna say that anyways.
It's mathematically improbable, but in principle,
if it did, we wouldn't ascribe any authority to it.
It's just an accident.
It doesn't mean anything.
But the origin of consciousness, the origin of free will,
the origin of information and DNA origin of free will, the origin of information
in DNA, these are not physical things. There's an immaterial essence here. And how are they
explained by a physical process? I had a debate with Michael Shermer for a couple hours, maybe
six months ago, and his explanation, not the only one, is just kind of an epiphenomenon.
It just appears at a certain level of complexity and to me I just think that's the ultimate
non-explanation it's a fancy word to not explain a phenomenon at all now let me
ask you this question you use this word in in the book and I'm an academic so I
was told in my interview at Biola early on, they're like, you're an apologist and sometimes apologists can, you know, overstate stuff.
In academics, we need to really nuance things and stay very sober.
And I'm like, okay, I understand.
Yet in your book, you talk about the overwhelming scientific evidence for the soul. Now I know you use that word
intentionally. Tell me why and just kind of sum up if you're going to say to somebody,
why do you think the science is overwhelming towards the reality of the soul?
Sure. Well, the reason it's overwhelming is because when you look at the really seminal experiments
done in cognitive neuroscience over the past century, many of them point very directly
to the existence of the soul.
And none of them provide any evidence to think that the soul doesn't exist.
I mean, you can certainly study the brain from the standpoint of ordinary
activity of the brain, action potentials, neurotransmitters, and that's perfectly good
science, but that doesn't disprove the soul by any means. But there's tons of evidence.
A very good example was Wilder Penfield's experiments stimulating the surface of the
brain. I actually recently had a debate about this with a neuroscientist.
And Penfield did, again, over a million individual stimulations of the surface of the brain in 1,100
patients and not a single time stimulated any kind of abstract thought at all.
I have to admit that abstract thought probably doesn't come from the brain.
Science doesn't get much better than that.
You have a million trials of an experiment and a glaringly clear answer.
The split brain surgery work, which is utterly fascinating, the idea that a person can compare images
that are presented separately to the brain hemispheres, which are disconnected.
And the capacity to compare the images cannot have come from the brain itself, because there's
no part of the brain that sees both images.
The idea that a person in the deepest level of coma, whose brain has been functionally destroyed, can do mathematics and can talk to you about their family and even talk about
philosophy using functional MRI imaging.
All of these things obviously point to the existence of a set of powers of the mind that
are unlinked to a substantial degree from the physical brain.
And that's the spiritual soul.
And frankly, if you want somebody who describes
the spiritual soul in a way that is neuroscientifically
the most accurate, I'd go back to St. Thomas Aquinas.
You know, the psychology of St. Thomas Aquinas
very, very closely fits what we're learning in neuroscience.
Dr. Michael Egener, your book is so fascinating.
I give it my highest recommendation for a book of its kind.
It's sophisticated but it's very readable.
I'm not a scientist and I was able to track with some of the nuances about the brain.
To me, when people ask me for evidence for the soul, I'm going to point towards the substance of consciousness.
Brandon Riccobar, J.P. Morland,
I think the most sophisticated philosophical case,
right alongside your new book, The Immortal Mind,
as more of what you might call a scientific case for the soul.
Of course, those certainly overlap.
Really appreciate your work.
In due time, we will definitely have you back to probe into some more of the depth of these.
But I want to encourage folks to pick up The Immortal Mind.
It comes out in June.
So if you're watching this before, you can pre-order it.
You're going to enjoy it.
It's fascinating.
Believer or not, to me, this is the kind of evidence worth wrestling with
because quite a lot is at stake.
Before you click away, make sure you hit subscribe.
This is a topic we're going to come back to,
the soul, the afterlife, near-death experiences.
You have a whole chapter in your book on near-death experiences.
We didn't even discuss that.
So there's much more in the book,
but make sure you hit subscribe
because these are topics we are coming back to.
In fact, I'm going to do a show soon on big lies about Satan.
I've wanted to do that show for a while.
That one is coming up.
So don't miss it.
Make sure you hit subscribe if you thought about studying apologetics.
At Biola, we have some of the leading philosophers of mind in the world
and experts in intelligent design.
We have a distance program.
We would love to have you be a part of it.
See information below. Dr. Eggner love to have you be a part of it.
See information below. Dr. Eganer, this has really been a joy. Thanks for writing a great
book and thanks for coming on.
Thank you, John. It's been a real privilege. Thank you.