Today, Explained - Did we just invent telepathy?

Episode Date: April 4, 2022

A groundbreaking new study claims to have found a way for a fully paralyzed person to communicate entirely via thought. But the scientists behind it have a checkered past. This episode was produced by... Miles Bryan, edited by Matt Collette, engineered by Efim Shapiro, fact-checked by Tori Dominguez and Laura Bullard, and hosted by Sean Rameswaram. Transcript at vox.com/todayexplained Support Today, Explained by making a financial contribution to Vox! bit.ly/givepodcasts Learn more about your ad choices. Visit podcastchoices.com/adchoices

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 What you're hearing right now is a German guy asking his wife to go get a mixer to puree some soup for him. The reason it sounds kind of funky is because he's asking her using only his thoughts. You're hearing real-time activity, brain activity, that this person now has to sort of actively shape, actively modulate through his own mind. A totally paralyzed person asked his wife to puree some soup using only his thoughts. That request, thanks to a brain implant, manifested not in words but notes. And that's his sort of most basic form of communication, despite being in a completely locked-in state. On Today Explained, we're getting closer to telepathy
Starting point is 00:00:53 with fully locked-in, paralyzed people. Nine. Get groceries delivered across the GTA from Real Canadian Superstore with PC Express. Shop online for super prices and super savings. Try it today and get up to $75 in PC Optimum Points. Visit superstore.ca to get started. Today Explained, I'm Sean Ramos-Firm. I'm joined by Jonathan Mowens, freelance science journalist,
Starting point is 00:01:27 and we're going to talk about a guy whose name we actually don't know. No one really knows his name because, for privacy reasons, it's sort of kept secret. But he's the star of this story. He was diagnosed with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, or ALS. And this is a condition that entails sort of losing certain brain cells in your brain and spinal cords sort of degenerate over time. And these are related to motor controls. You sort of lose the ability to move, and you quite quickly end up quite immobilized, sort of totally paralyzed. And so this person, he was in his state, diagnosed 2015, and eventually
Starting point is 00:02:03 he starts to lose all of his motor control, but he's still able to communicate with his eyes. How do you communicate with your eyes? Right, so he communicates because he can move his eyes in horizontal and up and down directions, I believe. And essentially his family had devised a sort of pen and paper scheme which involves having these four columns, I believe. So there's red, yellow, green, and blue.
Starting point is 00:02:25 And each color has a list of letters. So say yellow has A, B, C, D, and then green has E, F, G, H. It's not actually those, but it's something along those lines. Basically, you have clusters of letters in each color. And so through eye movements, he says yes and no. So they point at yellow, he says yes. And then they list out each letter and he says yes and no. So they point at yellow, he says yes. And then they list out each letter and he says yes or no. The problem here is that he's aware that this ability to speak with his eyes is temporary. And so he asks and the family realizes that he wants some kind of strategy to go beyond this as things get increasingly worse. The family reaches out to these brain scientists, this guy called Niels Birbaumer and this other
Starting point is 00:03:05 scientist called Ujwal Chowdhury. And so they tell him, like, we might have a method for you. And they were like, maybe we just need to sort of actually get into the brain itself through the skull. They implant these two sort of chips and these chips have 64 micro electrodes in them. So it's like basically if you think about this microchip with like 64 little teeth almost. They put these in the outer layer of the brain, specifically in the part involved in motor control. So hand movements especially I think is what they were focusing at the time.
Starting point is 00:03:37 And those electrodes are then going to pick up the signal of the brain activity directly. It's not something that's on the outer layer of your head. It's something that's actually directly, physically inside your brain. How does this technology work? If you think of neurons, neurons being brain cells, they have what are known as action potentials. That's how they sort of drive activity.
Starting point is 00:04:01 To pass on information, it's sort of like a wire passing on electrical information. And so these microelectrodes are able to detect these sort of very small-scale electrical activity in specific parts of the brain. If we ask the man, whose name I do not know, if we ask him to imagine something, because he can't move, if you ask him to imagine moving his hand, then maybe we can get a response from that, that is directly picked up by these microelectrodes. So that's kind of the idea. Okay, how does it go?
Starting point is 00:04:34 They spent, I think, 86 days just trying this out, trying to see if they can elicit electrical activity for hand movement, arm movement, tongue movement. None of that works out. And so there's a sort of aha moment, I guess. Sort of the breakthrough moment is when Niels Burbaumer tells them, why don't we try this somewhat unusual technique called auditory neurofeedback? Auditory neurofeedback.
Starting point is 00:05:03 That's right. Yeah. What is it? Essentially, what you're doing is you're providing the person with real-time activity of what's going on in their own brain. And in this case, it's sound. So he's getting real-time activity of his own brain cells in the form of sound that he can then modulate actively by changing his thoughts. So by being provided this constant feedback of what's going on in his brain, he's then asked, well, can you push it up or can you push it down? Can you dial it up? Can you dial it down?
Starting point is 00:05:36 Can you dial up or down the sound? Yes, exactly. And he does this by thinking of something. And that was his able of just sort of boosting that signal up or boosting it down. And this is some pretty heady stuff. So help us understand how exactly this allows them to communicate with him. Right. Yeah. So there are two main stages to this and it's quite complicated, but hopefully we'll get through this. So the first stage is basically training. You're training this guy to be able to say yes or no. That's the goal. So how do you make him say yes or no? The first thing is to basically give him what's known as a target tone, either a high note or a low note. And that's the tone that he has to match with his mind through pushing up the second sound
Starting point is 00:06:23 that I'll describe now. Say he gets the first note, it's a high note, whatever it is. Then he's provided a second sound, and that also prompts him to start the trial, as you call it. Okay, now the second sound's here. Try to push this sound to match this first sound that you heard. And as he manipulates his own brain activity, you hear a series of tones that are hopefully getting closer to the target tone. And every time he gets it right,
Starting point is 00:06:57 he's actually getting a cash reward in the form of just like a sound that makes like cha-ching or something like that. So in a way, this is kind of like this operant conditioning Pavlov style where you're just priming this person to just keep learning how to say yes, keep learning how to say no, keep doing what you need to do to be able to get that activity up or down. It doesn't really matter what you do, to be honest, as long as you get it to that point. So how do these experiments progress? Is it just yeses and nos and hitting the right note for forever?
Starting point is 00:07:27 Right, so it's a lot of that, honestly. And when he got really good at it, essentially when he got 80% of these correct, he would move on to the next phase. Every few weeks they would come in and they would do this training with him, Pavlovian style, you know, conditioning him to be able to say yes or no. And then once that's done and he gets 80% of it accuracy correct, he then ends up doing the spelling section.
Starting point is 00:07:52 So this is the more sort of weird and spooky section. Here's when he applies yes and no's to a schema like he had learned before with his family. A very similar one, actually, where he's able to say yes or no to specific letters, and then thereby formulating actual words, sentences, and an entire expression. The current sentence is Mixer for soups with meat marionette closely show all the boys' legs it works
Starting point is 00:08:31 yellow no And following this tedious process, what does he finally say? Right, so he's saying all these things in German. And you know, if you translate these things in German to English, they are things like, I want goulash soup, or mom, head massage. It's a lot of sort of direct instructions. Or I want to listen to Tool, a rock band from Los Angeles. Or curry, potato and bolo, yeah, soup or something. An assembly of food and music and beer.
Starting point is 00:09:17 I wanted to watch movies with his son. I don't know how old his son was, but I think he was quite young. Okay, so our guy's hungry and and he loves beer, and he loves Tool. All that aside, what's happening here is hugely historic. Yeah, I mean, it is kind of like this breakthrough study in many ways. This is the first time a fully paralyzed person is able to communicate at this length. He's able to communicate entire sentences, able to express themselves. People who are family relatives have a lot of hope regarding this technology because
Starting point is 00:09:50 suddenly they see this as the ability to finally communicate to some extent, right? Even if it's very slow, this is kind of like this amazing technology. It's great advancement in science and technology. But there are very, very good reasons to be skeptical. A lot of scientists question Niels Bohr-Balmer and Ujwal Chowdhury specifically. And so I think there are good reasons to sort of take this with a big grain of salt is coming up next on Today Explained comes from Aura. Aura believes that sharing pictures is a great way to keep up with family,
Starting point is 00:11:03 and Aura says it's never been easier thanks to their digital picture frames. They were named the number one digital photo frame by Wirecutter. Aura frames make it easy to share unlimited photos and videos directly from your phone to the frame. When you give an Aura frame as a gift, you can personalize it, you can preload it with a thoughtful message, maybe your favorite photos. Our colleague Andrew tried an AuraFrame for himself. So setup was super simple. In my case, we were celebrating my grandmother's birthday and she's very fortunate. She's got 10 grandkids. And so we wanted to surprise her with the AuraFrame and because she's a little bit older, it was just easier for us to source all the images together and have them uploaded to the frame itself. And because we're all connected over text message, it was just so easy to send a link to everybody. You can save on the perfect gift by visiting oraframes.com to get
Starting point is 00:11:56 $35 off Aura's best-selling Carvermat frames with promo code EXPLAINED at checkout. That's A-U-R-A frames.com promo code EXPLAINED. This deal is exclusive to listeners and available just in time for the holidays. Terms and conditions do apply. Bet MGM, authorized gaming partner of the NBA, has your back all season long. From tip-off to the final buzzer, you're always taken care of with a sportsbook born in Vegas. That's a feeling you can only get with BetMGM. And no matter your team, your favorite player, or your style, there's something every NBA fan will love about BetMGM. Download the app today and discover why BetMGM is your basketball home for the season. Raise your game to the next level this year with BetMGM, a sportsbook worth a slam dunk,
Starting point is 00:12:46 and authorized gaming partner of the NBA. BetMGM.com for terms and conditions. Must be 19 years of age or older to wager. Ontario only. Please play responsibly. If you have any questions or concerns about your gambling or someone close to you, please contact Connex Ontario at 1-866-531-2600 to speak to an advisor free of charge. BetMGM operates pursuant to an operating agreement with iGaming Ontario.
Starting point is 00:13:18 Today Explained, you've heard about the breakthrough science of fully locked in, paralyzed person talking using thoughts, read by a brain brain implant translated into yeses and noes that then correspond to letters that make complete sentences like, could you get me a beer? Could you massage my head or curry with potato? But now we got to talk about the dudes behind the experiment and why a bunch of their peers don't really trust them. To help with that, we reached out to Meghana Kesavan. She's a biotech reporter for Stat News, and she recently wrote about Niels Bierbommer and Ujwal Chowdhury.
Starting point is 00:13:55 Their science does look quite amazing, you know, and it was published in one of the top journals in the world. But a few years ago, their work was completely discredited. They did something pretty similar with ALS patients. It wasn't a brain implant, but they used a brain-computer interface where they put electrodes on patients' scalps, and they found out a way to communicate with them through yes or no answers. But then someone at the University of Tübingen, they tested the data again and tried to replicate it and proclaimed that it was wrong. This allegation basically destroyed their careers. Does that mean they're trying to sort of redeem themselves with this new experiment? I think so. I think so.
Starting point is 00:14:37 Well, tell me a little bit more about these two scientists. Let's start with Niels Bierbommer. He was a pretty famous neuroscientist in Germany. He's kind of a bombastic figure. Let's start with Niels Bierbommer. wrote a book called Empty Brain, Happy Brain, which sort of says the quieter your mind is, the happier you are and the more present you are in like living. Now, you may ask, isn't the quality of life in such a terrible disease so bad that everybody would like to not to live? And it actually has implications, that whole thesis on this idea that people who are locked in with ALS can actually be happy in that state. The answer is many, many studies in the US and in Europe,
Starting point is 00:15:32 with these completely paralyzed patients, have shown that quality of life is extremely high and not low. So what we see is a surprising quality of life, despite a desperate physical situation. He speaks his mind, you know. He's been controversial. And his buddy, Ujwal Chowdhury? Oh, he was just a postdoc in his lab, and they just worked together. He came over to Germany to do postdoctoral work in computer interface stuff. Tell me more about this first experiment they did. Was it as groundbreaking as this new one, in theory?
Starting point is 00:16:16 So for 2017, yeah, it was super groundbreaking. Or so it seemed. This simple-looking, non-invasive cap is allowing researchers to communicate with patients who are trapped inside their own bodies because of degenerative nerve diseases like ALS. In 2017, they had a paper in PLOS Biology, which is another pretty high-impact journal, about how these electrodes on the scalp could read brainwaves and decide if someone was thinking the answer yes or thinking the answer no. A patient thinks yes, or a patient thinks no. The machine records the blood flow during that thought and calculates how the blood flow changes during yes and during no.
Starting point is 00:16:55 And after a while, the computer develops an idea, a pattern of the blood flow during a yes and during a no. They tested it in force objects and they claimed that it was pretty successful. Beerbomber and Chowdhury actually claimed to have been able to communicate with a fully paralyzed person before. Yes, there were a lot of headlines. They had some cute anecdotes. One of them was that a daughter asked her paralyzed father if he approved of her fiancé, and he said no nine times out of ten.
Starting point is 00:17:31 It was indication that he was still in there, and he definitely had opinions. Did he say yes the tenth time because he was just tired of being asked? That I don't know. I don't know about the order of it. It may have just been that the brain waves were read differently. What ends up happening to the study? Why does it get challenged? It was because the person who worked in the same university just decided to run the numbers and found that they weren't necessarily consistent and it wasn't replicable. One of the underlying tenets of science these days is that if you publish work, it has to be replicable by somebody else. You need to have consistent results that can be reproduced. And if they're not reproduced, then that calls into question
Starting point is 00:18:17 whether it's real. And so that basically happened. And then there was this big inquiry into his work from the university and then also from the DFG, which is basically the major research funding agency in Germany. And they looked into his work and they decided that there was a selective data selection, meaning that they only chose the good stuff maybe, and that there may have been a lack of disclosure of some data and some aspects of the challenging of these patients with these questions. There were portions of video that were missing. And so the DFG, the university, the scientific community, they doubted Baerbaumer and it led to the fall of his career in a sense.
Starting point is 00:19:05 Yeah. How bad does it get for him? Well, he got fired from the university. The DFG, I think, asked for him to pay back the research funds that he had used from them. He was put on probation for five years at the DFG. He basically decided that he was going to quit science and moved to Italy. It was so bad that he left his country? Yeah. He was like, I'm done with Germany. So he moved.
Starting point is 00:19:32 Did he ever concede that he used improper science, that he fudged his results? No, he didn't. He actually stands by his data 100%. He concedes that, yeah, he omitted certain portions of the video taping like these patient answers because they were videotaping most of the sessions. And he said it was because patients needed to be cared for, like spit needed to be suctioned out of their mouths or, you know, they had to be moved. And so these types of activities that were just patient care, like they said that that's why they turned off the video camera,
Starting point is 00:20:05 and that was their biggest fault. How was this looked at in the scientific community? Clearly, it didn't go well for him in Germany at this university, but what did other scientists think? I think it's mixed. There are definitely scientists that continue to doubt his work. I've heard from some of them. But there's also just a coalition of scientists that were signatories in a petition basically
Starting point is 00:20:29 saying like, Beerbomber is innocent and his work should be reinstated into the journals. I mean, there are dozens of names of different scientists around the world that are supporting them. So just last month, Beerbomber and Chowdhury published this new experiment. Groundbreaking stuff. Historic stuff. But of course, in the scientific community, people know that these guys kind of have the scarlet letter. What's the reaction? measured than what the broader public reaction has been. I think scientists are impressed that the results look good. Any work that's published in something like Nature Communications has been vetted really carefully. And I think Nature took two years to look through their data to try and validate it because they have this sticky history. To get vetted by Nature, it's going to take months anyway, but very rarely does it take two years. So I think it's an accomplishment to have this work published at all. Okay. So these two scientists have a checkered past, but this experiment was possibly extra
Starting point is 00:21:37 vetted as a result. What about the ethics? What are some of the ethical questions surrounding opening up paralyzed people's skulls to get them to communicate? There are a lot of ethical concerns with these brain-computer interfaces. Some researchers are stepping away from this because they're realizing that the broader ALS community just needs more support just in the ways that they're living their lives. You know, like the money could perhaps better be spent with treatments or with allowing better social supports. And so this is an invasive technology which involves these people like learning entirely new ways to communicate. And it takes time, probably millions of dollars.
Starting point is 00:22:20 And so it's not necessarily feasible for a lot of people, first off. The environment has to be exactly perfect for this to work in a person. And secondly, there are issues of consent. You can say that this person consented, but if a person's completely locked in, there are still going to be questions on how long they're going to want to continue to communicate in this way. Beyond that, there are questions on how many people with ALS would want to continue living like this, because these people are all on ventilators and they're completely paralyzed. And so, yes, they can talk, but how many people will want to? That's a big question. The implants can be, we don't know what their shelf lives are, like how long they will even work and how many surgeries it'll take to like reinsert them. So there's that. And then there are just the issues of what kind of a life
Starting point is 00:23:10 is worth living to society or to an individual. These are people who are making choices, but what does it mean to be locked in and communicate this way? It's been a hairy topic from the beginning. And we don't have all the answers to those questions yet, but maybe we will once this technology starts coming online for more and more people. And it sounds like that might be happening soon. Could, you know, if people continue this type of research. I think Elon Musk is into it. We'll see. You know, there are all kinds of sci-fi, fun thoughts on where this type of research. I think Elon Musk is into it. We'll see. There are all kinds of sci-fi, fun thoughts on where this kind of research could go. But right now, it's kind of nice that they're trying to help patients communicate and live a little better. And then it's only a matter of
Starting point is 00:23:57 time before you and I can do this interview without even talking to each other, I guess? Wait, what? Like Elon Musk's dream is to have people talk to each other, I guess. Wait, what? Like Elon Musk's dream is to have people talk to each other without talking, right? Okay. I guess that all of that stuff maybe could be possible, but I think people like to get ahead of themselves in what technology is capable of doing. We're not there yet. There are a lot of things that need to go right before one can do that. Meghana Kesavan reports for Stat News. You can find her work at statnews.com. Earlier in the show, you heard from Jonathan Mowens. He's a freelance science journalist who wrote about this man whose name we do not know for the New York Times. The headline was,
Starting point is 00:24:48 Brain Implant Allows Fully Paralyzed Patient to Communicate. Our program today was produced by Miles Bryan, edited by Matthew Collette, fact-checked by Tori Dominguez, and Laura Bullard, and engineered by Afim Shapiro. I'm Sean Ramos-Firm, and this is our very first episode of Today Explained that'll reach listeners as a podcast, but also a broadcast. Today we make our debut on public radio stations across the United States. We are thrilled about it. To all our new listeners, welcome to the show.
Starting point is 00:25:15 You can get in touch with us anytime. We're on Twitter at today underscore explained. Our email is todayexplained at vox.com. Let us know what you think of the show and thank you for listening. Thank you.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.