Feel Better, Live More with Dr Rangan Chatterjee - Rewiring Your Brain for Better Mental Health: How Small Actions Can Transform Your Health with Dr Camilla Nord #543

Episode Date: April 1, 2025

Have you ever wondered why the same traumatic experience affects different people in completely different ways? Or why finding pleasure in life is so fundamental to our mental wellbeing? To answer th...ese questions and a whole host more, I'm joined this week by Dr Camilla Nord. Camilla leads the Mental Health Neuroscience Lab at the University of Cambridge and is author of the best-selling book, The Balanced Brain: The Science of Mental Health. In this fascinating conversation, we explore: How everything that impacts our mental health ultimately works through the brain, and why we often artificially separate 'mind' from 'brain' and 'mental' from 'physical' health Why pleasure is so fundamental to mental wellbeing that a loss of it is a core symptom of depression, and how activities like social laughter can boost mood by releasing natural opioids The fascinating overlap between chronic pain and depression circuits in the brain – revealing why experiencing one increases your risk of developing the other How motivation varies throughout the day based on our individual body clocks, and why morning people and night owls have different energy patterns Interoception – our internal body awareness – and how practices like meditation, yoga and body scanning can enhance this crucial sense Why the placebo effect is so powerful and how a doctor's communication style can significantly impact treatment outcomes Throughout the conversation, Camilla emphasises that there is no one size fits all approach and that it’s the small, consistent actions that ultimately end up transforming our lives.   Support the podcast and enjoy Ad-Free episodes. Try FREE for 7 days on Apple Podcasts https://apple.co/feelbetterlivemore. For other podcast platforms go to https://fblm.supercast.com.   Thanks to our sponsors: https://drinkag1.com/livemore https://vivobarefoot.com/livemore   Show notes https://drchatterjee.com/543   DISCLAIMER: The content in the podcast and on this webpage is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment. Always seek the advice of your doctor or qualified healthcare provider. Never disregard professional medical advice or delay in seeking it because of something you have heard on the podcast or on my website.

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 If what you do is you deprive yourself of everything you love in pursuit of some kind of optimized health for most people, that will not be the route to best mental health. Hey guys, how you doing? Hope you're having a good week so far. My name is Dr. Rangan Chatterjee and this is my podcast, Feel Better, live more. Have you ever wondered why the same traumatic experience affects different people in completely different ways? Or why finding pleasure in life is so important for our wellbeing? Well, to answer these questions and a whole host more, is this week's guest, the neuroscientist Dr Camilla Nord. Camilla leads the mental health neuroscience lab at the University of Cambridge and is
Starting point is 00:00:53 the author of the bestselling book, The Balanced Brain, The Science of Mental Health. During our conversation, Camilla reveals how everything that impacts our mental health – from our genes, to our diet, to our stress levels – ultimately works through one final common pathway – the brain. Yet as Camilla highlights, we often artificially separate mind from brain and mental from physical health, missing the crucial connections between them. We also discuss how pleasure is so fundamental to mental wellbeing that a loss of it is a core symptom of depression. Camilla explains the brain's hedonic hotspots and how activities like social laughter can boost mood by releasing natural opioids.
Starting point is 00:01:44 We also explore the fascinating overlap between chronic pain and depression circuits in the brain revealing why experiencing one increases your risk of developing the other. We also discuss the importance of something called interoception, how and why our motivation varies throughout the day and the incredible, underestimated power of placebo. Throughout the conversation, Camilla emphasises that there is no one-size-fits-all approach, that it's the small, consistent actions that ultimately end up transforming our lives. You're a neuroscientist at Cambridge University. What would you say are some of the things we commonly misunderstand about our mental health? So in my lab, we study the neuroscience of mental health.
Starting point is 00:02:44 And actually, when I tell people that, sometimes they're quite surprised, because one of the assumptions that really permeates our kind of culture of mental health is in fact that mental health is something ephemeral, psychological, maybe not something that we can kind of tangibly understand if we understood biology. But I suppose, you know, my perspective on this is, in fact, even the most ephemeral sounding things are actually quantifiable, measurable. And so in the case of mental health, I think that the real sort of missing piece of the puzzle is understanding the brain and the things that the brain does that make us feel
Starting point is 00:03:21 better or worse mentally healthier or mentally less healthy. Yeah, one of the themes throughout this entire book, which has really got me thinking, is that you go through the various causes or the potential causes of mental health problems, genes, cultural factors, economic security, stressful life experiences, the social world, diet, our physical condition, stress, all kinds of different things. But you make it very clear that the final pathway, the common final pathway for all of them to affect our mental health is via the brain. Which I think, I guess makes sense when you think about it logically, but I'm not sure everyone thinks about mental health issues like that. I get that. There are so many important factors, the things you've experienced, the world you live in,
Starting point is 00:04:13 the way that your genes predispose you, and these all affect your mental health. I'm not dismissing them. It's just that two people with the exact same traumatic experience don't have the same reaction to that traumatic experience. Two people with the exact same genetic predisposition, at least when it comes to mental health traits, it's no kind of black and white binary you either will or won't develop a disorder. And the reason for that is essentially the complexity of the brain. So sometimes I use smoking and lung cancer as a counter example. So everybody knows that an environmental influence like smoking is quite important when it comes to thinking about your likelihood of developing lung cancer. You wouldn't want
Starting point is 00:04:55 to dismiss it. But then if you really wanted to understand lung cancer, you would have to study the lungs because that is the final common pathway. Whether it's been some other environmental factor, a genetic factor, smoking, whatever has kind of led you there, the final place is the lungs. And to me, the brain and actually the wider nervous system is that place for mental health. Why do you think that we've not thought about mental health or mental wellbeing through that lens before? Why do you think, I mean, as I say that, I want to explore even the term mental health shortly, but that sort of brain perspective on our mental wellbeing, do you think that's
Starting point is 00:05:38 been missing? And if so, why? I think we live in a kind of naturally quite dualistic society, meaning we all feel there is a distinction between the mind and the brain. And even though I go around saying, and I do genuinely believe that there isn't really a distinction, it doesn't stop you feeling, oh, I am this way
Starting point is 00:05:59 and my brain is this kind of somewhat separate thing about me, like, for example, another bit of your body. But actually that feeling of I, that is a consequence of your brain's processes. So in fact, that sort of that gut feeling, that intuition that these two things are separate is wrong. They are one in the same or at least one kind of comes from the other. I think we could even extend that one step further. The body and the brain,
Starting point is 00:06:30 well, I'm giving my perspective and I welcome yours. I think one of the problems when we think about our health in general and let me broaden it out beyond mental health is that we have separated body and brain. We even have these different terms. I understand why we have those different terms, but it's very clear to me, having seen many patients over the course of my career, that sometimes with some patients, let's say with a mental health issue, helping them do things that you would say are helping their body,
Starting point is 00:07:07 it's incredible how many times it helps their mental wellbeing. So, I don't know, exercise or sleep, for example, can have transformative effects on some people with mental health problems. Not everyone necessarily, but some people. I share that perspective entirely, but I didn't always actually. When I first started doing this kind of neuroscience of mental health research, I, like any other neuroscientist, was really focused, you know, neck up. And actually it was only maybe a couple years after my PhD, and I thought to myself, actually, a lot of what the brain is doing is not processing the world around it, but processing the world inside of you, meaning signals from your heart, your gut, your metabolic
Starting point is 00:07:51 system, all over your body. So that means necessarily that your physical body is consequential to the brain, so consequential, it's constantly receiving information from it. And so I began to kind of, I suppose, delve into body brain research. And that's actually the major focus of my lab now. And I do think that one of the other ways we kind of get mental health wrong
Starting point is 00:08:15 is by creating this artificial division between physical and mental health. Yeah, so much to talk about there. I definitely want to get to that internal sensing interoception shortly because I think it's something that can have a profound impact for all of us if we start to tune into our body's own signals, if we're able to, and if we're able to, let's say, practice. So we can come to that. I really want us to talk about these various ingredients for mental health that you write about and you dedicate separate chapters to, pleasure, pain, motivation, learning.
Starting point is 00:08:53 Because I think we hear a lot about these things. Dopamine, for example, is the neurochemical of the moment online. Everyone likes talking about dopamine. I know you've got some thoughts on dopamine and where we perhaps get misled on our understanding of dopamine. But if we could just start off talking about pain and pleasure, you spend a lot of time
Starting point is 00:09:19 at the start of this book talking about pain and pleasure. And I love what you've written because it's, its core, let's take depression for example, there are many symptoms that we look at when trying to diagnose depression, but you know, a loss of pleasure or an inability to gain pleasure is one of those symptoms. So can you talk about pleasure and pain and why you think it's important for us to understand them in the context of our mental well-being? Not everyone knows this, but you don't actually need low mood to be diagnosed with depression. Instead, you could have their second core symptom is just a loss of pleasure or motivation. So that is so fundamental, it is actually at the centre of the depression
Starting point is 00:10:06 diagnosis. That's probably why I first became interested in it. I thought there is this central system, symptom I should say, that is somehow neglected, both culturally but also to some degree clinically. It's not as well treated by typical treatments like antidepressants. Often that's the kind of symptom that might remain even if you see improvements in some of your other symptoms. And I thought to myself, but that is so disabling and could actually kind of have this real negative cycle where if you stop getting pleasure from things, stop being motivated to seek out what you used to enjoy, that's just going to self-perpetuate. It's just going to make your symptoms worse because you'll stop experiencing the very
Starting point is 00:10:48 things that could have helped you get out of a depressive episode. So that was the reason why I began my book on mental health with this kind of deep dive into pleasure and pain because I think a lack of pleasure is so central to poor mental health and in fact experience of pleasure in my opinion so central to poor mental health and in fact, experience of pleasure, in my opinion, is central to mental health. Now pain may be a little more surprising and I think that's why I like it so much. People don't think of pain or chronic pain
Starting point is 00:11:17 as a mental health syndrome, but I think they probably should because there is so much in common with your experience of chronic pain and your experience of depression. In fact, experience of say chronic pain in the past makes you more likely to develop depression in the future. Maybe not surprising, it's really hard to have chronic pain,
Starting point is 00:11:38 but the other way round is true too. So if you are someone who's experienced depression, you're more likely to go on and experience chronic pain in the future. And in my opinion, that is because there is this biological overlap between the circuits supporting chronic pain and those that we see are dysfunctional in depression. And they have to do with your perception of your internal world, your perception of negative beliefs about the world, and these other factors that can make your experience a bit more negative.
Starting point is 00:12:09 So that's why I wanted to start with pain. I think it provides a kind of surprising insight into how the brain and the body could come together to change your experience of the world, to make it worse. And then I also think it could maybe provide some clues for how we could get out of it through routes like pleasure. Yeah. It's so interesting.
Starting point is 00:12:32 I'm thinking back to a conversation I had with Dr. Howard Schubiner a couple of years ago about treating chronic pain. And one of the things he was sharing with me when he came on the podcast was this idea that physical pain and emotional pain is often expressed in the same area in the brain. You know, I don't want to misspeak for how and that's my recollection of one of the things he was saying to me, which is really, really interesting because you're saying that there's an overlap in the brain between the regions disrupted in depression and in chronic pain.
Starting point is 00:13:11 And Dr. Shubhna was talking about how there's an overlap in the brain between, you know, in those regions that express physical pain versus emotional pain. It's just hard not to come away from these things with this idea that I feel we've lost a little bit in medicine, that the whole body and brain is interconnected. And whilst it's useful to try and separate things, to study them and see what impact they have, when you're dealing with this kind of three-dimensional human being in front of you, as I've done for much of my career, you realize that a
Starting point is 00:13:50 lot of these symptoms have multiple inputs, not just one. Does that make sense? Exactly. Even something that feels localizable, obvious, like pain. I feel pain in X region of my body. Actually, that sensation is already filtered and modified by so many processes in your body and brain. It's not like a straightforward signal that gets sent to your brain and perceived perfectly. Actually, it depends what else is happening in your environment. For example, if you're escaping a lion, you need to suppress any kind of pain signal as much as possible because your body, your brain, has a priority in its environment that kind of overrides any need to listen to a painful signal. And then in the real world or in most of our real worlds, there are other priorities that can either
Starting point is 00:14:41 dampen down or sometimes upregulate those sorts of symptoms. So it's not as obvious as you might think. Yeah. Is that why, or is that one of the mechanisms, let's say someone is an endurance athlete or they like to do half marathons. So half marathon, you know, roughly 13 miles. Let's say someone's toe starts hurting at mile six for whatever reason, maybe the way they're running or their shoe or something. Endurance athletes will tell you that they can push through. They're in the race or whether they're racing or doing it for fun. I've been in this situation where you feel something, but through your mind, you just keep going, you get through. And then a few hours later, when you've got home, man, it's in agony or you see something's red and swollen, but you didn't perceive it.
Starting point is 00:15:37 It was still there. I'm sure that inflammation was going on, maybe not to the same degree, but it's that perception of pain, isn't it, that's so important? Absolutely. And I'm not a marathon runner, but I suspect what happens in cases like that is two things. One, they have a priority, which is to finish the marathon, and so you can override other kinds of physical sensations. But the other is they have practice. So often when you're running long distances, you have various kinds of aches and pains. And actually you sort of practice suppressing them. You keep going, perhaps you learn that actually
Starting point is 00:16:12 it wasn't that significant, it goes away. And so that sounds quite adaptive, but I think it can also be bad. Although I'm not a very long distance runner, I practice a quite intense form of yoga. And it's very physically intense, I should say. And what happens sometimes, I think, in my practice is that I can get injured because I'm so immersed in kind of exactly what I'm doing.
Starting point is 00:16:35 Perhaps I'm sort of, you know, you get in sort of almost a meditative state, but also you have these kind of physical goals. And I think that's quite dangerous because it can mean you're dampening down that sort of listening to information from your body, which could be significant and stop you from getting injured. Yeah, absolutely. And just to be really clear, I'm not necessarily saying that what that theoretical half marathon runner is doing is necessarily good for them because of course, you can, you know, cause more of an injury and maybe four months out of the sport and
Starting point is 00:17:06 moving because you pushed through. But I guess the point is that pain isn't always what it seems and we can change our perception of pain in a variety of ways that we're perhaps conscious of and also unconscious of. So in that chapter on pain, you start off by talking about stress and stress-induced analgesia, which I found really, really interesting. And you spoke about hot and cold bath experiments in the 1980s, skydiving, cold water swimming. So I think we should go into this area because I think it really helps to demonstrate this concept of pain being amenable and it can change depending on what's going on.
Starting point is 00:17:49 Yeah, I mean, when I was writing that chapter, I had just had a visit by a friend of mine, Jen, who was an expert cold water swimmer. She loves it, she's great at it, and she convinced me to jump into the Cam, which is a very cold river near where I live, around maybe late March, early April. Oh my God, it was so cold. It felt like daggers all over my body. I probably lasted about a minute. And then I got out and I felt,
Starting point is 00:18:17 just as she'd predicted, euphoria. Like you feel incredible. And that's not just because of the relief of pain, though some of it probably is, it's also because ages ago, we've known for a really long time in neuroscience that kind of just brief, but temporary pain, discomfort, very cold water,
Starting point is 00:18:39 can induce opioid release in the brain. So just like opioid drugs, but endogenous opioids that we have in the body, which you sometimes call endorphins. So they are a reaction and it helps you do things like push through pain sometimes. Maybe the more mundane experiences, if you stub your toe, you feel very intense pain immediately.
Starting point is 00:18:59 And then you get almost like a slight head rush and the pain dampens down. And that is an endogenous opioid release. Okay. Okay. So you go in the cold water and many people enjoy, enjoy, enjoy the word. I think they do. I think some people really enjoy. I think they genuinely do. Other people don't want to go anywhere near the cold. But how does that help us understand mental ill health? It's interesting, pain and pleasure comes, I think pleasure we get, right? I think most of us would
Starting point is 00:19:34 recognize not being able to experience pleasure in life is not going to feel particularly good. But what's the relationship between pain and that cold water immersion and our mental health? I think it would be good to try and draw that link for people. Yeah, in many ways, I think it's kind of surprising because the general assumption is that pain is this really negative thing for mental health. And of course, it definitely is
Starting point is 00:20:02 if you're experiencing pain all the time. But actually, this kind of very brief discomfort in this case can help acutely like immediately with mental health because of this very specific biological mechanism. So I think it proves or suggests that there are these kind of little short circuit ways that your brain can experience pleasure, even from quite surprising sources. Yeah. What is pleasure?
Starting point is 00:20:33 Through the lens of the brain as a neuroscientist, what is happening in our brain when we experience pleasure? Or does it depend what we're doing to get that pleasure? In some ways it doesn't totally depend what we're doing. Those do change where in the brain is kind of interested in it, but there are very pleasure specific regions that become involved no matter what the source of pleasure is. These are sometimes called hedonic hotspots, a phrase from Morten Kringelbach and others who are kind of pleasure neuroscientists. And this means that like volcanoes dotted across the surface of the earth, we have these
Starting point is 00:21:11 little hotspots of pleasure dotted in our brain. It's actually not, they're not big and they're not that many of them, but they are really essential and kind of quite characteristic of our experience of pleasure. And do we know what neurochemicals are being released when we experience pleasure? So they actually have multiple kinds of neurotransmitters involved in them, but one of the best examples is the opioid system. And there is actually a nice debate about opioids, whether you exactly feel pleasure from them in and of themselves, or whether it kind of depends a lot on the context, which is a fun example of where neuroscientists like to take things quite literally.
Starting point is 00:21:51 Yeah, you mentioned that section on pleasure that migraine sufferers or a certain percentage of migraine sufferers finds that having sex actually reduces their pain, whereas you said that with cluster headaches, so that's different type of headache, having sex can exacerbate their pain. What can we learn from those kind of bits of research? Well, what we can learn is that when I initially found out the migraine fact, I thought this is great. I should tell my friends who get migraines. But in fact, I actually think it can be quite difficult to distinguish between a migraine and a cluster headache
Starting point is 00:22:27 for many people. So you might want to be quite careful before you take that kind of advice. But the fact that we can do something, let's say pleasurable, right? And it can then change our perception of pain, that's really interesting, isn't it? Yeah, I think that is quite significant.
Starting point is 00:22:45 And actually sometimes, when you're in a painful state, as we all have been at some point in our lives, it can feel inescapable. And that's part of what makes it so bad, I think, that it can feel eternal. Do you know if you have even something like a stomach ache, if it goes on for long enough, you just start to think too much about it.
Starting point is 00:23:06 You start to think, oh my gosh, this is just the way I am. My stomach is just going to feel like this. This is it. And actually, if you can have even temporary relief from those symptoms, it might not necessarily reduce the kind of pain signals coming from somewhere in your body, but it will definitely change your interpretation of those signals. The moment you start to think of them as less permanent, less all-encompassing, less kind of, this is who I am,
Starting point is 00:23:32 then that's where you get the key to kind of overcoming them and making it through despite those symptoms. Let's talk a bit about laughter and laughing with friends, because you've been sharing in your book that laughing with friends actually reduces pain. Do we know how it does that? This is a good example of what pleasure can do. So what laughter does is it releases endogenous opioids.
Starting point is 00:23:57 It's a really fun experiment that was done when people were watching comedy videos with friends. So this is a social laughter experience, kind of one of the most pleasurable things, I suppose, that humans can do. And so people engaging in this social laughter, not only did they show opioid release in brain scans, proving that there's this kind of biological,
Starting point is 00:24:20 like almost painkiller mechanism, but you can even show that in literal painkilling effects because people were able to do these sort of uncomfortable wall sits and uncomfortable exercise for longer afterwards. So that is an immediate effect of having experienced this pleasurable thing on a kind of, you know, a reduction or an overcoming, let's say, of painful inputs afterwards. Yeah, it reminds me again of this idea that I end up talking about quite a lot on the podcast these days. This idea that we can't really separate psychology from physiology. And what you just said there that if you are doing wall sits, that sort of like half squat
Starting point is 00:25:07 against the wall and seeing how long you can hold it for, you're saying that if you're laughing with friends watching something, you can actually go for longer. So instead of it being, let's say 30 seconds that you can manage, that might go, it's a 45 seconds or a minute. So your sense of wellbeing, what you're doing, your laughter, the kind of hormone and neurochemical release from that allows you to do something physically challenging for longer. And I guess people who do race, whatever sport that might be, and it's well known this when they've studied athletes, is that you can go faster in a race than you can go
Starting point is 00:25:46 by yourself because in, I don't know, in the last 200 meters of a race, you can push yourself mentally in a different way if you're trying to beat someone than you can do when you're by yourself. Or for example, in the London Marathon or in marathons around the world, if they look at the spread of times, it's really interesting. I think it's quite even. But to go just under four hours or just under five hours, there's a disproportionate amount of people who can do that. Presumably, they're seeing the clock and they're like, actually, if I just go into fifth gear, although I'm tired and I want to stop, I can actually beat this kind of milestone time in my head. It pushes them on, right? So there's this interesting idea about our psychology
Starting point is 00:26:37 actually then changes or I don't know if you would say it changes our physiology, but it certainly has an impact on our physiology. Yeah, I think I probably would say it changes our physiology, at least indirectly by changing our behavior, which then can affect our physiology. Yeah. And there's also this interesting thing about laughing. Laughter and pleasure is of huge interest, right? I came across some research a few years ago showing us that laughing, well, it wasn't directly on laughter, it was saying regularly doing things that you love makes you more
Starting point is 00:27:11 resilient to stress. And at the same time, chronic work stress makes it harder for you to experience pleasure in those day-to-day things. So again, this kind of bi-directional relationship, in terms of pleasure then, if you're someone who is not experiencing pleasure from things that you used to, like is there a practical take home that people can say,
Starting point is 00:27:36 oh, well, I used to find this enjoyable. I've been diagnosed with depression, I don't anymore. What can I actually do about it? Just taking a quick break to give a shout out to AG1, one of the sponsors of today's show. Nutrition can often seem really complicated. We get confused about what exact diets we should be following and which supplements we might benefit from taking. And that's one of the many reasons that I love AG1 and have been taking it for over six years. AG1 makes it simple to be the best version of you. Over 70 ingredients,
Starting point is 00:28:21 one scoop once a day for less than a cup of coffee. It's a science-driven, daily health drink which supports your energy, focus and immune system. It also helps support your gut's health. For example, it contains calcium, which contributes to the normal function of digestive enzymes, and biotin to maintain your own intestinal mucous membrane. The scientific team behind AG1 includes experts from a broad range of fields, including longevity, preventive medicine, genetics and biochemistry, and I talk to them regularly and am really impressed with their commitment to making a top quality product. In fact, AG1 has
Starting point is 00:29:06 gone through 53 versions as they continue to iterate in line with the latest research. And the best thing of course is that all this goodness comes in one convenient daily serving that makes it really easy to fit into your life. So if you want to support your health seven mornings a week, start with AG1. And for the month of April only, AG1 are offering a free morning person hat, bottle of vitamin D and five free AG1 travel packs with your first subscription, just go to www.drinkag1.com forward slash live more to unlock this exclusive offer and get started on your journey to better health today. Today's episode is sponsored by Vivo Barefoot. So if you've been listening to my podcast
Starting point is 00:29:59 for a while, you will know that I am a huge fan of barefoot shoes and believe that everyone at some point in their life should try wearing them. I myself have been wearing Vivo barefoot shoes exclusively now for over 11 years, well before they started supporting my podcast. So why do I think you should give them a go? Well, not only are they really comfortable, many people also find that when they start wearing them, all kinds of niggles like back pain, knee pain, hip pain, even neck pain can sometimes get better. We also know from research that wearing barefoot shoes regularly helps to strengthen your foot muscles.
Starting point is 00:30:44 Now, this is really important. Wearing barefoot shoes regularly helps to strengthen your foot muscles. This is really important. People think about strengthening their arms in the gym or other body parts, but your feet muscles are some of the most important muscles in your body. They help you walk, interact with the ground and balance. Now that spring is finally here and many of you will be trying to get outside more and move your bodies, why not give Vivo barefoot shoes a try? One of my all-time favourites is the Primus Trail, which come in a variety of different
Starting point is 00:31:15 colours for men and women. And although I've been wearing them for many years now, I do think that the Primus Trail are a great shoe to start off with if you're new to barefoot shoes. So why not make 20, 25 a year, you give them a go and don't forget it is completely risk free to do so because Vivo offer a 100 day trial for new customers. So if you're not happy, you can just send them back for a full refund. If you go to vivobarefoot.com forward slash live more, they are giving 20% off as a one-time
Starting point is 00:31:52 code to all of my podcast listeners. Terms and conditions apply to get your 20% off codes. All you have to do is go to vivobearfoot.com forward slash live more. So I suppose the most practical thing for someone diagnosed with depression is to kind of take that signal as being very important and significant, something that they maybe should seek treatment for, psychological treatment, pharmacological treatment, you know, whatever works for them. But I think a second kind of corollary of that that applies to everyone, irrespective of their diagnosis, is to maybe prioritize pleasure a little bit more than we do. Especially, I think people have this kind of, this inclination that everything fun is bad for you.
Starting point is 00:32:46 And some fun things are bad for you, but actually when it comes to your mental health, there is a kind of inherent benefit of doing things that you really love, even if some of those things come with small consequences for your health. So I think, you know, it is actually about a balance. If what you do is you deprive yourself of everything you love in pursuit of some kind of optimized health for most people, that will not be the route to best mental health. Okay. I wonder if I can share with you an experience I had with a patient quite a number of years ago now. And I have told it,
Starting point is 00:33:25 I don't think I've told it on this show for a while now. Your work and your description of pleasure really makes me think about what actually happened with that patient. So are you open to this? So it was a chap who was, I think he was 53 from recollection. He was the CEO of a local company. This is years ago now. And he came in to see me with basically a number of symptoms, including low moods and an inability to get pleasure. He was, he was quite indifferent about his life. And I remember seeing him and spending a bit of time and trying to understand his life
Starting point is 00:34:04 and what was going on. I asked him about his relationship with his wife. He said, yeah, it's fine. We'd be married for a while. So, you know, he wasn't that excited to talk about it. He said, yes, it's fine. I said, how about your relationship with your kids? He said, yeah, but I rarely see them. You know, they're busy and what about your job? Do you like it? No, but it pays the bills. He had this very, nothing really excited him. And he definitely had symptoms that you could quite reasonably diagnose them with depression. Okay. And I asked him, do you do anything fun, you know, in your week? Do you have any hobbies? And he said, no doc, I don't have time for hobbies.
Starting point is 00:34:45 And he was basically just saying, I'm too busy. I said, what about the weekend? Oh, the weekend I'm taking my kids to their clubs and their classes. I don't have time. I've got a busy job. I've got a busy weekend. I don't have time.
Starting point is 00:34:55 And yes, I did some blood tests and we did some screening tests and all those sorts of things. But one of the things I said to him was, I sort of probed him about hobbies more. I said, well, what did you used to do? Were there things you used to enjoy? He said, yeah, when I was a kid, when I was a teenager, I used to enjoy building train sets.
Starting point is 00:35:18 I said, okay, do you have train sets at home? And he said, well, I do, but they're in my loft. I haven't used them for years. I said, okay, what I want you to do this week when you have some time or make some time, I want you to get down from your loft, some of those train sets and start playing around with them. Okay. There were other things as well, but I'm trying to just simplify the story to try and get to the point about pleasure. I didn't see him again for a few months, which is not uncommon. You know, I saw him in NHS general practice. You see so many patients, you can't follow up everyone.
Starting point is 00:35:51 But about three months later, I bumped into his wife and I knew this family pretty well over the years. And she said, Doc, he's like a different person. Ever since he saw you that day, he got the train set down. He started playing with it. He now has subscribed to a monthly magazine. He's going on eBay to buy collector's items. And I actually then saw him about three months later for a well man's check. He came in to get some blood tests done.
Starting point is 00:36:16 I said, hey, look, you know, how are you getting on? He goes, doc, everything's different now. I feel really, really good. His low mood had gone, his indifference had gone. When I asked him about his marriage, he said, yeah, my relationship's really good. His low mood had gone, his indifference had gone. When I asked him about his marriage, he said, yeah, my relationship's really good. I'm enjoying my job. I'm closer to my kids.
Starting point is 00:36:32 That was so powerful for me, Camilla, because it would be easy. Look, I'm always very holistic minded. I'm always trying to get to the root cause of a patient's problems. And I just felt with him, what would happen if we, in some ways, correct the pleasure deficiency in his life or try and address it. And it would seem to me that by engaging in pleasure, and again, I appreciate not everyone can do pleasure things and feel that pleasure, but he was able to. Yeah, he had something. He had something. He got into it.
Starting point is 00:37:07 Everything else in his life came back online. His marriage, his relationship with his kids, his enjoyment of his job. So you're saying that pleasure, neurochemically, as a neuroscientist, looking at it through the lens of the brain is very, very important for our mental well-being. Can you hypothesize for me what might have been going on? Why did him engaging with his toy train sets, you know, not the sort of thing I learned at medical school, which is, you know, for depression, prescribe a toy train set. I didn't learn that. So it's an unusual prescription, but with him, it worked. Why do you think that might have been the case?
Starting point is 00:37:45 Yeah, it's remarkable actually. And I think it's remarkable because of if you draw a line between train sets and mental health, well, that's going to be a very complicated line. I think for many people, it wouldn't have an effect on mental health. For that patient, it did. And I think the reason for that is actually something that is in common with other, maybe more expected treatments for mental health. So what some pharmacological drug treatments for mental health do is they help you engage in the world a little bit
Starting point is 00:38:16 better, perhaps with a little bit less anxiety, and help you re-access the sorts of, say, social pleasures that you used to get. Or what some psychological treatments for depression do is they help you, for example, take a little bit of a social risk, see some friends you wouldn't normally see, and essentially also give you a route to that pleasure, or help you engage with other kinds of activities that you might have previously not. So I think, although trains is unique and special, what it is, is it's
Starting point is 00:38:47 kind of facilitating that for me it's a positive prediction error. So in the brain we have this kind of predictive model of the world. Otherwise we wouldn't be able to kind of deal with all of the information that we have. So instead we learn from what we've experienced. We learn to expect what's happening next. And when something deviates from our expectations, say when something is better, we weren't experiencing any pleasure at all, and then all of a sudden,
Starting point is 00:39:14 we have a little dose of pleasure. What that is, is it becomes a positive prediction, or a surprise that we start hopefully to integrate into our model of the world and eventually our belief about the very nature of the world can be affected. It can start to become more positive because we've learned that perhaps not 100% of the world is quite so bleak. Yeah, it's fascinating. Do you think some of that would have been mediated through dopamine?
Starting point is 00:39:48 Yes, so that particular learning process, it's not that the train releases dopamine, that's a kind of, often a misconception, that sort of, if you were to see something you like, that causes some kind of huge rush of dopamine. It's not exactly how it works, it's more that the dopamine is this signal, a signal perhaps of significance that means this is something you should learn about. So it helps your brain figure out what an unexpectedly positive event was. And when that event is sort of reasonable within your world, then it can get integrated into your wider model of the world and it begins to affect your future expectations, such that actually eventually then it wouldn't have that same effect because it would be expected.
Starting point is 00:40:34 In your RSI talk, which has been seen by many people now on YouTube, you talk I think about is it the early monkey studies on dopamine dopamine about is that a light and juice? Yeah, this is where this process that I'm talking about was first discovered. It sounds so far away from pleasure and train sets, but it really is the kind of, yeah, the cellular basis of what's happening. Could you just walk us through it? Because I think, I think dopamine is very much misunderstood and overly simplified. And I think this helps us understand it. And I think then if I could hear you go through it,
Starting point is 00:41:08 I'm then going to try and put it through the lens of that patient to try and hypothesize what might have been going on. Yeah, I mean, dopamine gets, it's beloved in some ways or hated in other ways, if you look at it kind of in popular culture. And between those two ends, I'm on the love side, I think dopamine is a fascinating molecule, but perhaps I love it for slightly different reasons because it actually has a number of
Starting point is 00:41:32 somewhat neglected functions outside of neuroscience, so things like our ability to initiate movement, that's dopamine, that's what goes wrong in Parkinson's disease. But another of its really central properties is its role in learning. And these are the experiments in the kind of late 1990s that were done recording from dopamine neurons deep in the brain. And what these neurons do is if you give monkeys
Starting point is 00:41:59 little drops of juice, which they love, initially the dopamine fires when they get the drop of juice. They're like, oh, something unexpectedly good. But what the scientists running this experiment did was they paired the drop of juice with a flash of light, like Pavlov and his dogs. So you'd get like flash of light and then drop of juice. And as the monkeys learned to expect the juice, the dopamine response moved earlier and it responded to the flash of light in anticipation of the later juice.
Starting point is 00:42:31 And then, if the scientists sort of meanly left off the juice one time, the monkeys got water instead, which is fine, but not as good as juice, then there was a dip. So the dopamine cells reduced their responding because that was worse than what they were expecting after a flash of than what they were expecting. After a flash of light, they were expecting juice, they got water. So this is called a prediction error and it's something that our brain does in all sorts of contexts when you experience something that is different than what you're expecting. And in this context of kind of rewards and losses,
Starting point is 00:43:00 dopamine is a really central player in sending this signal in the brain. And is this related to one of the reasons we feel that a lot of these modern apps, let's say social media apps, have the potential for people to engage in addictive behavior with or to have a compulsive relationship with those apps is because the reward is unpredictable. So you may associate, you may initially really enjoy being on the social media app. So your dopamine is releasing and then it's in advance, you know, you're going to sit on the sofa in five minutes and have time to chill. So the dopamine release starts to come earlier. But then sometimes you go on and things are great.
Starting point is 00:43:40 Sometimes you go on and then not. So that unpredictability, that's all mediated through dopamine, right? I think the unpredictability does contribute to why people sort of keep scrolling even if they haven't had fun so far, because it might, I suppose there's always this chance that they might see something really enjoyable. But I actually think people use social media for lots of reasons and probably the kind of reward, addictiveness reason has maybe gotten all the publicity. But I think a lot of people use it because like they find boredom so aversive. There's a famous psychology experiment from maybe about 10 years ago, I think, where
Starting point is 00:44:17 people were put in a room with a shock box, which is like a box where you can deliver yourself a painful stimulus. And they were told how it worked and then they were just left. And basically loads of them just set it up on themselves and gave themselves a painful shock. And that's because, one, humans are like incredibly curious, and two, because they didn't really just want to be there bored on their own with something that could give them something different, I suppose. So one of the interpretations of that is boredom is extremely aversive. And what people use
Starting point is 00:44:50 social media for often is in those in-between times. Yeah. Let's go back to this patient for a second, because I'm truly fascinated by our inability to feel pleasure, you that happens and then how, in this patient at least, engaging is something that he hasn't engaged with for a while that used to give him a lot of pleasure as a young boy or as a teenager, seemingly started to change his life around.
Starting point is 00:45:21 Might it be, for example, that he goes back to his house that evening, he gets the train toy set down, and then he surprisingly finds, actually, this is really good fun. I forgot how much I enjoyed this, right? Because you talked about our expectation, that is maybe surpassing his expectation. He didn't think it would be that good, but it was actually really good. He's like, oh wow.
Starting point is 00:45:48 So it's better than he expected, which is a good thing for us. We feel happier, we feel more positive. And then let's say he played on that train set twice a week. Could it be that, you know, he's getting to the point because he enjoys it, that he's finishing work in the evening, and it's, you know, it's 5, 5 to 8 pm, he's writing emails, and he goes, hey man, just let's crack through this, because in an hour I'll be home, and I can start playing. So,
Starting point is 00:46:13 his dopamine starts to be released even at work in advance of him doing that. Does that sound reasonable that this could be one of the be one of the mechanisms by which he started to turn his life around through pleasure. Yeah, and it's just kind of like one little tweak, and then I think it will have had a lot of knock-on consequences. For example, it will have helped him feel perhaps some kind of fulfillment or connection, because if he started subscribing to magazines and feeling like this was kind of, you know, maybe he was part of a community that he hadn't been part of before, it also
Starting point is 00:46:48 perhaps would have helped him access, you know, one of the other ingredients that I think is really essential for mental health, which is motivation. So it maybe will have given him a kind of invigoration for something that he didn't otherwise have. You know, an apathetic state is not a very pleasant one to be in. You feel, you know, maybe neutral, but actually it stops you from engaging in things in life.
Starting point is 00:47:12 And that, no matter what it is, if it's something that really motivates you, gives you that kind of get up and go, then that is often a really important access to things that could give you pleasure and happiness and so on. What is motivation? I think of motivation as, so I sometimes use the word drive for it throughout my book,
Starting point is 00:47:32 because it can mean this ability to go from a state of passive reception to a state of kind of active engagement. But there's a cost to that. So motivation inherently comes with a cost. As soon as you exert effort, cognitive effort even in your mind, but especially physical effort, you have an energetic cost to the body. And our brain doesn't necessarily want to spend that cost. So everything that might take energy is something that we weigh up. Is it worth doing this thing for the possible either reward or alleviation of punishment?
Starting point is 00:48:10 And these can be very abstract, distanced things. And people have actually slightly different kind of models of this. So people are, some people are a little bit more sensitive to the cost of what effort might come, like the potential cost of effort. Some people are really quite sensitive to like little small fluctuations and how rewarding something would be. And they would, you know,
Starting point is 00:48:30 maybe give much more effort to something more rewarding and much less effort to something less rewarding. These are all kind of interesting differences in the population. Yeah, I love that idea that motivation has a cost. I've never really thought about it through that lens before. And it makes me think of something Daniel Lieberman, do you know Professor Daniel Lieberman
Starting point is 00:48:49 at Harvard? He writes in his book, Exercised, about this idea that basically, you know, we're not born to exercise. Ultimately, if you go back to a hunter gatherer tribe or today, let's say you go to the Hadza tribe in Tanzania and ask them to run for an hour on a treadmill. I mean, they'd be totally confused. I mean, just run and not go anywhere and be on the same spot. It's the most bizarre thing in the world, but obviously it's our way of trying to cope with the modern world where we're not moving enough or we're certainly not moving as much as our ancestors used to move.
Starting point is 00:49:34 So he makes the case in that book that actually don't feel bad that you don't want to exercise. You're not meant to. It's not that there's a failing there from you because obviously the body's trying to conserve energy. If there's no reason for you to go and move your body, why would you? And I feel that it's a similar analogy, a similar theme through the way you're talking about motivation. Don't feel bad necessarily if you're not motivated to go and do all the things that you see the other people around you doing and you go, why, why have I not got that motivation? I think it's quite reassuring for people, at least initially, to go, yeah, because maybe you're naked and maybe your brain's going, actually, I'm not going to give you motivation at the moment because you need to rest. So I'm going to make you tired, so all you want to do is sit on the sofa and recoup your energy.
Starting point is 00:50:18 Does that make sense through your model? Sometimes that's exactly what your body wants you to do. And I think one thing that's quite interesting is that there are actually a number of variables that go into this effort cost equation. And one of them is actually the time of day and your circadian rhythm. So we all feel a little bit lazier at night
Starting point is 00:50:40 and a little bit more motivated during the day. But in my lab, my PhD student Sarah Merhoff did a series of experiments showing that actually this really depends on your chronotype. If you are a morning person, then that kind of person shows this very different equation, an equation really on the side of effort and motivation in the morning. And then that dips as the day goes on,
Starting point is 00:51:03 exactly as you would imagine. But it makes them look almost apathetic if you test them in the afternoon, it makes them look actually exactly like people who have high levels of apathy, high levels of anhedonia, which is that lack of pleasure. And then the flip side is true for people who have late circadian rhythms,
Starting point is 00:51:21 I think for, we initially thought they would just look like people who were a little bit apathetic, had perhaps higher levels of depression, who also show this shift away from energy, away from effort, but they only look like that in the morning. If you test people who are, you know, essentially night owls, people who really get kind of a lot of their energy later in the day, then they show this ability to engage in effort, engage in kind of physical cost as the day goes on, more as the day goes on than they do in the morning. Yeah, that was truly fascinating. And I think as a daughter, when you're assessing someone kind of depends on what time of day you're assessing that patient. That's exactly what I thought from it. Yeah.
Starting point is 00:52:06 Of course, you know, the assessment on mental wellbeing isn't just about what happens in that moment. It's, you know, over the course of a few weeks and a few months, but I'm definitely that kind of morning person. I mean, no question at all. If I don't get my stuff done in the morning, my, you know, all the things that I like to do, my journaling, my meditation, my movements. Once it hits 3pm, I ain't going to do a lot of that stuff. I'm not going to start working out. I'm not going to go for a run. Whereas other people can. That isn't
Starting point is 00:52:36 the time for me. And I think that has wider implications beyond mental wellbeing in terms of just our productivity, our work, you know, understanding yourself and when you are more energized and when that motivation, I guess, comes a bit more naturally. It's probably good to know that about yourself, isn't it? I think so. And I think kind of harness that because many people sort of expect to be able to work at a consistent level throughout their day, throughout their week. But actually, that's quite outdated because most of us don't do jobs where you have to just do the same thing again and again. And it's really about the number of hours that you do that same thing.
Starting point is 00:53:13 Actually if you have the sort of job where what you produce is what matters, kind of the quality of what you produce, then you probably need to modify your working based on your best hours. And that's how you're going to get the best output. Yeah, and that's something I was talking to Cal Newport about. He wrote a book about this last year about we need these new ways of working. The neuroscientist and eye surgeon Mitsu Storoni I spoke to a few months ago about this idea that we're no longer in the factory where we need to clock in and just do a mundane task for eight hours.
Starting point is 00:53:44 And when we're done, we clock out and go home. Like a lot of this work now, this modern work is using brain power. It requires creativity, problem solving. You cannot stay at the same pace and the same ability to do that for eight hours in a row. You're going to be better at certain parts of the day, which is why I think a lot of companies who are able to give their staff a bit more autonomy, I think are probably going to get better results from them and probably better rates of wellbeing, I would imagine. There's a statistic that was published maybe about a year, a year and a half ago in the UK saying that 88% of UK workers have experienced a degree of burnout in the past
Starting point is 00:54:30 two years, which I think is a pretty worrying state for modern society, at least in the UK. What is the relationship between chronic stress and chronic work stress do you think and our mental wellbeing? Ever have one of those days when everything seems to go wrong and before you know it, you've slipped back into those habits you're trying so hard to avoid like sugar, doom scrolling and wine. When it comes to making changes in our lives that actually last, most people think the solution is to try harder. But this is simply
Starting point is 00:55:14 not the case. In my new book, Make Change That Lasts, I reveal how exactly you can break free from the habits that hold you back. What many of us don't realise is that our behaviours follow our beliefs. So when you change your beliefs, and my book will show you how to do that, you'll spend less time having to think about your habits because they'll happen automatically. Make Change At Last, the number one Sunday Times bestseller is available to buy now all over the world as a paperback, ebook and as an audio book, which I am narrating. And if you've not got your copy yet, what are you waiting for? Yeah, that is a really sad statistic because actually if you experience chronic stress,
Starting point is 00:56:10 it has effects on mental well-being, but those effects come essentially physical by physiological processes. So it changes levels of cortisol, other kinds of important hormones. It also changes things like how exhausted you feel, which is part of your sort of brain's computation of your own mental health. It also probably changes the way and the time that you have to engage in, I don't know, playing with train sets or other fun things.
Starting point is 00:56:35 So I think actually that kind of chronic stress and burnout can have direct and loads of indirect consequences on your mental health. Yeah. And anhedonia, the inability to experience pleasure is also a sign or a symptom you will often see in burnout. Although, yes, it's a symptom that's used in the diagnosis of depression, it's not exclusive to depression. You see it in other things as well. And I've certainly seen it in patients chronically stressed, on the road to burnout,
Starting point is 00:57:10 or even at burnouts. That's just this apathy and this, you know, just this reluctance to want to do anything, which is pretty sad really. Yeah, and how do you get around that? I mean, I think it's a real, I think it's a conundrum. Yeah. How do you get around it? Okay. Let's take this beyond burnout. How do you get around this when people are feeling as if they're in a really dark hole and they just don't know how to get out
Starting point is 00:57:36 of it? We can talk about drugs in just a moment. I once had a suicidal patient for a variety of different reasons. And the way I helped them to get out of it was through five minute interventions. And I really would love to hear your view as a neuroscientist on what might be going on here. I have found, whether with this patient or patients with depression over the years, there's not a feeling that they're making progress in life. Okay? They're not moving forward. So years ago, I realized, okay, how can I help this patient feel as if they're making
Starting point is 00:58:21 progress? Because a lot of things they get asked to do or they read online, the truth is when you feel really, really low, you know, you're probably going to struggle to motivate yourself to get to the gym and move your body in a way that's going to increase your endorphins, for example, right? Absolutely.
Starting point is 00:58:38 So I came up with this sort of five minute concept that I use myself. So I have a five minute strength workout every morning in my kitchen, in my pajamas. But with many of my patients with depression, I said, well, what is one thing that we can get you, what is one thing you might want to do? And I'd always personalize it to them,
Starting point is 00:58:57 but I remember one suicidal patient, it was working out, right? They wouldn't go to a gym, they said, they haven't got motivation. And I broke it right down. I said, what would it look like if you did five minutes every morning? And for that patient,
Starting point is 00:59:11 they used to work out when they were teenagers. So they were quite familiar with that as a concept. So this person bought a kettlebell, kept the kettlebell in their kitchen. And again, this is not about the kettlebell. It's not about it being exercised. This is what that patient wanted. And you make it very clear in your book, there is no one size fits all, which I completely agree with in terms of a solution. But we started off with saying, okay, what would it look like if every morning, the first thing
Starting point is 00:59:40 you do when you come to your kitchen, you literally pick up that kettlebell and you just swing it on and off for five minutes. Right. And I think we start with two minutes and built up to five minutes. I'm not kidding you. That was the start of all the changes because by doing that every morning, because I made it so simple that he could never really say, he could do, but he didn't say, I don't have time or the motivation. I said, even put it in your sink if you have to, so you have to at least move it to get it. You know, I was really trying to help him go, no, you have to pick that thing up. And my, my feeling was, was that doing that every day changed the way that he viewed himself. Instead of being someone who didn't have motivation and didn't have
Starting point is 01:00:26 this ability to do anything, it's like, no, he started doing that. And within seven days, within 14 days, within 21 days, he's now someone who actually, yeah, man, I actually, I can do stuff. And then yes, it's working on his muscles, his hormones are changing, his dopamine will be changing, maybe his serotonin is changing. But that was me trying to help him as a clinician, right? I didn't need to know what was the neuroscience going on. I'm just like, how do I help this person? What do you think might be going on from a neuroscientific perspective? Yeah, I love that five minute idea because actually what you've just done in that effort cost equation
Starting point is 01:01:05 that we are all kind of subject to is you've made the cost smaller and smaller and smaller. And in some ways you've actually kept the reward constant, the reward of getting to do something which he wants to do like exercise, but you've made it even more accessible and even less effortful to do. So until the point where it's a kind of inflection point where it was worth it for him. And that will have been the initial cost for him. But actually the cost will even have reduced over time
Starting point is 01:01:38 because as something becomes a habit, it becomes almost easier to do than not do. So the hardest change is that first change which you kind of enabled with that intervention. That's interesting. When it becomes a habit, it's harder to not do than do. That's a really interesting concept, isn't it? I guess that's how I would even look at my morning strength workout now. It's been over five years since I've been doing this and it's just five minutes while my coffee brews every morning. You may not have heard
Starting point is 01:02:12 me talk about this before, but essentially while the coffee brews for five minutes, I'm in my pajamas and I'm doing a strength workout in the kitchen. I'm not going on email or Instagram in those five minutes. And I actually think my morning would feel really weird now if I didn't do it. Do you ever skip it? Very, very rarely. Even when I travel, and I've been traveling a lot recently, I even take my cafeteria with me. That's your timer, so you've got to.
Starting point is 01:02:43 Yeah, but what it does is when I'm on the roads and I've been doing a lot of interviews for my book recently, so I've been on the road a lot, in a hotel room in the morning, because I drink black coffee, it's easy, just boil a kettle and you make it. And I put the timer on and I do it in a hotel room. So for me, it's actually this little habit that I take around with me to sort of ground me and centre me every morning. And yeah, I'm just wondering, because, you know, we started off this conversation talking about this idea that permeates your entire book, which is whatever's going on, whatever we're doing that's improving our mental wellbeing, it's always happening through the brain and
Starting point is 01:03:20 the nervous system. So I'm truly fascinated as to what these five-minute actions are doing. Can you hypothesize what kind of brain chemicals might be changing? What will be changing are specific circuits in the brain that are involved in this kind of instantiation and then maintenance of habits, which are sort of two different circuits. So the first time you do something effortful, it involves regions in the brain that work on that computation. How costly is it? How rewarding is it? Is it worth it to me? And those are the regions we know from animal experiments
Starting point is 01:03:53 and also human experiments that say if you have an injury in them, it can really change this cost-benefit calculation. So that's how we know that they're related. And it does involve neurotransmitters like dopamine, but not only. So it's really about the kind of communication between these different regions that help you make that decision.
Starting point is 01:04:13 But as something becomes a habit, actually the circuits change from kind of decision-making circuits, which often involve weighing up possible options. It's a little bit more effortful for the brain that is, to these quite automatic circuits that make something a habit. You hear actually most about these circuits, places like the nucleus accumbens, dopamine-rich
Starting point is 01:04:37 regions of the brain, because of their involvement in drug addiction. But it's not just drug addiction. These are the circuits that help us do things again and again without using all of our cognitive resources to focus on them. So we move into this other kind of mode of processing in the brain, something that can be ongoing almost in the background
Starting point is 01:04:59 while our brain is doing other things or can be focused on other things. So this is the same process that when you're learning to drive a manual gear car or for the transatlantic audience, a stick, in those first few driving lessons, you're utilizing, it's hard work, you know, you're trying to figure out how to time the clutch with the gear changing without stalling the car. Yeah, like the speed of coming up. So that's presumably quite a lot of effort for your brain. Boy, can you not think about anything else when you're learning to drive a manual car.
Starting point is 01:05:32 Exactly, but then at some point it becomes automatic. And then when you have to teach someone how do you do that, you don't actually know how you do it. Yeah, in fact, you might become worse if you start thinking about it. Like when you're walking downstairs and you kind of forget. Yeah. It's like, you know, golfers, when they choke in the final few holes of a major, you know, one of the theories, which I think makes the most sense to me is that instead of doing something that's just habituated and automatic, they're just looking at the hole, they're
Starting point is 01:05:59 swinging the club, they're now trying to control every aspect. This matters. I've been training for this since I was four years old. And suddenly they can't actually swing the golf club anymore because they're now overly thinking about it. This is the same principle, right? Exactly. Yeah. It's a shift to a...
Starting point is 01:06:16 They're often actually very proximal next to each other, anatomical brain regions, but they are involved in these two kind of complimentary action processes, processes that support our action. One of them, very effortful, involves lots of cognitive control, things like that. One of them, automatic, habitual, and so on. When we think about these four brain processes, which of course are linked, pleasure, pain, motivation, and learning, I'm just thinking that a lot of people are trying to change their habits, okay, at various stages in their life.
Starting point is 01:06:52 They're trying to implement certain things. And so initially when we try and implement those things, it can feel hard. So let's use that five- minute workout for that patient, right, as a model. The patient feels apathy. They feel indifferent. They don't have motivation. They start doing something where together we've lowered the cost right down. Hey, hey, this is super easy. Even just do one minute. Okay. Right. So they do that. So we've lowered the cost. So they start doing it. And presumably at some point, those endorphins from, you know, swinging kettlebell will start to be released, you know, so it's going to help you feel good. So it's going to perhaps give
Starting point is 01:07:40 you some pleasure. Yeah. And it may become much more kind of self-reinforcing than it would have been before, because before you had to kind of imagine that it would be good. You probably didn't even have that solid a concept of how good it would be, whether it would be at all. You might've been actually quite uncertain.
Starting point is 01:07:58 And as you experience it, if it is good, your estimate of that kind of future feeling becomes more and more and more precise, statistically. And so, you are able to predict just how good it will be and the likelihood that it will be good much better. That makes it more rewarding. And presumably that then speaks of this idea that if you want to increase the movement in your life, whether it's for your mental well-being or your physical health, not that we can separate them as cleanly as that.
Starting point is 01:08:29 Ultimately, you want to find something that you do find enjoyable. Yeah. I think it's a much, much harder road to try to engage in something that you kind of conceptually know is good for you, but that you get literally no enjoyment out of. It's possible, you know, you really have to work your kind of motivation system hard. There are some highly, highly like motivated people who can ignore the cost of that, who engage in it. But boy, is that a rough road to go down.
Starting point is 01:08:58 Why that movement snack concept is something that I'm so passionate about. Movement snack. Yeah, I love that term. Yeah, because it simplifies movement right down. It's helped me no ends. It's helped, I'm going to say thousands of my patients over the years because I think it does something beyond what we might hypothesize that movement is doing. So we often look at these things. Oh, you know, when you move, it's going to release myokines in your body. It's got an anti-inflammatory component.
Starting point is 01:09:26 It's going to help your muscles, which is then going to help your insulin sensitivity. I don't disagree with any of these things. Sure, but you think it does this kind of self-efficacy thing where it like makes you realize you can do it. Yeah, I think for me at least, my observations are that that might be the gold behind some of these things. Don't get me wrong, those other biochemical changes for your physical are fantastic,
Starting point is 01:09:52 but I think that's what it does. I think it maybe targets that motivation piece and makes people feel that they can do stuff. Yeah, I think you're right about that. I think it is motivation that becomes the crux. You know, the kind of maybe maybe it's a little bit, a little bit of pleasure, a little bit of a prediction error. But actually I think that getting people able to do something that they want to do, kind of finding access into their motivation system
Starting point is 01:10:19 could make, it could be really black and white. It could make you all of a sudden realize you can do things, You can feel motivated. You can change things and so on. In your book, you talk about this term mental health and you also sort of split it up in various places to, you know, mental health is this broad term. There's mental illness within that, but I guess there's also mental wellbeing. I don't know how you feel about this. I've always struggled with the term mental health. I know
Starting point is 01:10:51 it's commonly used and I've used it as well, but I would say it never really connects with me. Like I never really intuitively know what does mental health mean. And of course that will be because of my perception of the world and what associations I have with that term. How do you see that term and how do you see the differences between mental illness, mental well-being and you know how do you sort of pass those all together in your brain? I thought very deliberately about whether or not to use the phrase mental health. I think it gets thrown about in all kinds of different ways and sometimes used interchangeably with mental illness,
Starting point is 01:11:29 which is really unhelpful. But I also have some problems with the idea of mental wellbeing or wellbeing in general, which is probably why I went with mental health, because actually I think there is value, for example, if you are in a really low state, into feeling just healthy. Not necessarily great, not necessarily splendid, not any of those other kind of, you know, well-being type terms, but actually the ability to function is the biggest difference between mental health and a lack of mental health. And I actually think that boundary is the kind of critical boundary. And that's where I wanted to write my book.
Starting point is 01:12:11 So it's also about within mental health, how you can feel better, but really a lot of what matters to most people, all of us will have either, you know, known someone who experiences a mental health condition or experienced one ourselves, is how to get over that boundary into mental health. Many people would say that our mental health is currently declining.
Starting point is 01:12:36 You've written this wonderful book on mental health. Do you think it's fair to say that as a society things are getting worse or do you think there's other things that explain that? I'm very torn on that. There is data to suggest that more and more people are diagnosed with a mental health condition all the time, especially younger people. But there are a lot of problems with that data. So some of it is a good thing. It means people are accessing services, accessing diagnoses when they might not have in the past. Some of it means, you know,
Starting point is 01:13:13 perhaps you might've felt shame for feeling this way before. And now people are able to kind of come forward, seek help, get treatment for it. And that looks like a rise in rates of mental health diagnosis. So, get treatment for it. And that looks like a rise in rates of mental health diagnosis. So that explains some of it. But I suppose if you really dig down,
Starting point is 01:13:31 I don't think it explains all of it. There's still data suggesting that if you ask people the same exact questions, especially young people, especially young females, over time, their general wellbeing, general sense of mental health does seem to be worse than it used to be. So it's perhaps not as dramatic as some of the graphs would show, but it is happening. And I think we don't really know why. Do you have any hypotheses as to what the big things are in society that might be contributing
Starting point is 01:14:04 to this? I think it may be a lot of little things in fact. So many of the young people tested at the moment are people who experience the pandemic at very critical ages in brain development, which means they were deprived of socialization at really critical periods. It means they experience something quite traumatic at a very critical period and so that will definitely have had a knock on effect on long term mental health. So I think that's kind of one contributor, maybe in that case not a very small contributor. I also think there's a role, you know, potential role for the conceptualization of mental health problems in and of themselves. for the conceptualization of mental health problems in and of themselves.
Starting point is 01:14:46 So it sounds very complex, but this is an idea that has gained traction in my field that actual awareness of mental health can sometimes make mental health problems worse. So it can make you aware of your symptoms, but it can also make you kind of focus on your symptoms, consolidate your symptoms, focus on your symptoms, consolidate your symptoms, perhaps enhance your symptoms, especially in social settings in adolescence.
Starting point is 01:15:10 So this is a kind of testable hypothesis that I would say has not been proven by any means, but is a kind of one reasonable explanation for why there might be, especially kind of concentrations of mental health problems emerging in some people. Yeah, it's really, really interesting. I think it's pretty reasonable to think that for some people that may well be the case. I think it's reasonable and I think one of the reasons why it's plausible is because this can happen even with physical symptoms. So we know that in certain social settings, if for example, you know, the kind of classic example is often teenage girls,
Starting point is 01:15:47 but it really doesn't have to be. It can be kind of any concentrated, intense social setting. Often in adolescents, you can see a transmission of physical symptoms in certain communities, pejoratively sometimes called like mass hysteria, but actually it's a real process that is not conscious, that can just happen in experiencing someone else with very, very like kind of profoundly different physical symptoms, you can have a kind of contagious effect. Yeah, and we see this in all kinds of different things, you know, we are social animals, and that we respond to what the people around us are doing and what they're feeling, You know, there's studies showing that when you walk or run in groups, your cadence and rhythm starts to become more and more similar.
Starting point is 01:16:33 Yeah, synchronized. Synchronized, yeah. Which is fascinating and you know, that idea that we're social animals and the fact that in the lockdowns, there may well have been and still be a serious impact on certain people from that isolation. I know you mentioned it in certain young people, but we've seen this in a lot of the elderly as well. Older people who literally changed in those few months of lockdown and they've never returned. I remember so clearly, maybe between the first and the second lockdown, I went to my local leisure centre to go for a swim and I hadn't seen that manager for months.
Starting point is 01:17:14 We had a chat and he goes, man, people have changed. He says, I used to see six months ago, these like 15, 16 year olds that come in, they'd be chatty, they'd be talking. He says, they're either not coming in and when they do, they're withdrawn. They're not looking up in the eye. We sometimes don't think enough about how important that social interaction is for our physical and mental wellbeing. It reminds me of what you were saying before about laughter. Laughing with friends can make you go longer on a wall sit.
Starting point is 01:17:46 Laughing with friends, you write about in your book, reduces pain. And you put in a hypothesis there, I think, for one of your colleagues, that opioid activity may be a safety signal, calming and relaxing people to facilitate social cohesion. I thought that was fascinating. Do you remember who that researcher was? Yeah, I love that hypothesis. So I think that was written by Sophie Scott, Carolyn McGettigan and Nadine Levan, who are all these kind of laughter neuroscientists, speech neuroscientists who also work on laughter.
Starting point is 01:18:16 Can I just say, laughter neuroscientists. What a gig. I know. They've got such a good gig. But I particularly loved that idea that laughter is this kind of mechanism to enhance social cohesion. And there was a study that they wrote about in that paper that I still kind of use in my day-to-day life now, which is that when you're having a fight with your partner, it turns out that people who laugh during their arguments with their partners, they kind of made couples fight in this experiment, very, very cruel.
Starting point is 01:18:45 The ones who laugh report higher levels of marital satisfaction, kind of less importance of the fight, and then crucially show fewer physiological signals of stress during the fight, things like cortisol, hormones like cortisol. So basically, what I got from that was that couples who laugh together regularly, when they then have a bit of conflict, which for whatever reason, you know, the stress is lower, they recover from it quicker, I think all these sort of things. Is that your recollection? I think that's true too, but I actually think it was people who managed to joke a little
Starting point is 01:19:20 bit during the argument because that it has this, I think it has this immediate effect on your physiology. That's why you see those physiological signs. So it would be, yeah, people who can kind of find a way to take a step back. But I think what you said is also true in this experiment, that people who kind of laugh together do have a kind of better response to argument. So one of the take homes from this conversation,
Starting point is 01:19:44 whether it be for mental health or mental wellbeing or just generally improving your satisfaction and your sense of wellbeing in the world is laugh more. Right, is that reasonable? I think it's reasonable. Do we think from your laughter neuroscientist friends, man, I'm gonna have to get a laughter neuroscientist on the show, that sounds wonderful as a job.
Starting point is 01:20:06 Do you know from your interactions with them what laughing by yourself does So I am interested in this because often they use you know in this field in general They use social laughter as the kind of example But then they use single laughter as the you of example, but then they use single laughter as the stimulus. Like in an experiment, you put people in the scanner and they might be listening to, for example, real or fake laughter. You can see brain differences there. Or they might even be laughing themselves, although that's actually quite difficult because
Starting point is 01:20:39 the brain can't move in the scanner. But you can sort of measure what ends up happening with vocalizations when someone is laughing. So I'm always quite interested that the way experiments have to be done as a scientist are sometimes solo, but then the implications that that has kind of for our communities, for our societies are not solo at all. They're conjoined. They're all together. Yeah.
Starting point is 01:21:04 I mean, it'd be interesting to know the difference. I mean, I know on a personal level, if I'm ever feeling a bit overworked or, you know, I just put on YouTube and watch a comedian for 10 minutes, and you just laugh and you get taken to another world, you feel completely different. You know, you feel like you've gone to a different place. My perception, I haven't measured this,
Starting point is 01:21:26 but my perception is that my stress levels are lower. I feel better about the world. Oh, that's definitely true. But what's interesting is that that's hard. It's harder to laugh on your own. People laugh much more easily at much less funny jokes when they're with other people. So social laughter is more accessible.
Starting point is 01:21:43 I don't know if it's different, but it's easier. Wow, truly fascinating. Okay, we can go down a laughter rabbit hole. I'm gonna stop us from doing that. Right, let's think about some things that we can do treatment-wise, okay? And I really, really love, because it so aligns with my view on the world
Starting point is 01:22:03 that there's no one size fits all for improving our mental health. You make that point at multiple times throughout your book. And you also say we need to get away from this idea that something works or it doesn't work for mental health. One person's miracle is another person's snake oil. I thought that was really interesting. I wasn't expecting to read that from a neuroscientist. What do you mean by that? I genuinely think this is what we now know in neuroscience in kind of the science of mental health more generally,
Starting point is 01:22:36 which is that treatments that in the past, we maybe had a binary view of antidepressants in general work, other things might not work in general. Today we have a much more nuanced view of what it means to work and also what it means to not work. So there's a general acceptance that kind of nothing works for everyone. But I think also there's a real kind of growing understanding of there's a reason why for that. That in fact, there might be the thing we call depression might actually be caused by different mechanisms.
Starting point is 01:23:12 And so that's why one thing might work to treat one person's depression, but a totally different thing might work to treat someone else's depression, because actually the processes in your brain and body that are kind of causing that state are different and so are differentially affected by treatments. I would argue that that goes beyond mental health problems and actually that kind of rationale is what we should be applying more to all chronic disease. I believe that from what I've seen and from the research I've read, that chronic illness, and I put mental health within that, so non-acute things like a heart attack or a chest infection, for example,
Starting point is 01:23:56 these sort of more long-term symptoms and afflictions, there's very rarely, in my experience experience only one cause. There's multiple things. So I could have 10 patients with a headache and they could have 10 different contributing factors. The end symptom is the head hurts, but depending on what the cause is, the treatment's going to be different. And so, you know, we said earlier on about all these different things that, you know, our genetics, our social world, our diet, our physical conditions, like depending on what's driving the end products of low mood and anhedonia in the brain, well, the treatment's then going to be different depending
Starting point is 01:24:41 on what the cause is, right? Exactly. Which is to say that, you know, that the train intervention that worked so well for that person who needed one source of kind of motivation, one source, perhaps, of pleasure might not have worked for someone else for whom their kind of contributing factors for depression were just different. Yeah, exactly. Okay, so in terms of some of these treatments then, people are familiar with the term antidepressants. One of the most common ones are SSRIs, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. In your view, do they work?
Starting point is 01:25:21 And if so, how? In my view, SSRIs work in the old definition of the term which is to say they work mostly, they work more than placebo for a large group of people. But that doesn't mean that they work for everyone. And I think to understand why we need to understand how they work. So there was a kind of old fashioned idea that came about through the discovery of SSRIs that what SSRIs were doing was fixing a deficit of serotonin in the brain.
Starting point is 01:25:54 Actually in the beginning it was serotonin, dopamine and noradrenaline and then they sort of modernized the drugs and modernized the explanation. And that's a compelling theory because it's so sensible and it's how other disorders work, things like Parkinson's disease. It's a deficit of dopamine you treat with the precursor to dopamine and then that remedies the symptoms. Unfortunately, it turns out depression is not so simple. So there is very mixed evidence to support a deficit of serotonin in the brains of people with depression. Some studies, but nothing kind of really definitive or convincing.
Starting point is 01:26:33 The only thing I would say is convincing is that if people are on SSRIs and it's working, well then if you deprive them of serotonin, you can give them a kind of really disgusting milkshake that depletes the amino acid precursor to serotonin, they get depressed again. So it is the route by which SSRIs are working when they're working, but it's not necessarily kind of how they work. They don't seem to be remediating a deficit in serotonin. So we've known this for at least a decade, probably more. But what they actually do, I think is really interesting and we
Starting point is 01:27:06 might have to kind of think about the brain at a slightly different level than just these kind of you know general global levels of brain chemicals. So these were experiments done by Catherine Harmer and Phil Cowan in Oxford and they discovered that while your mood doesn't improve from a single dose of SRIs, one of the kind of conundrums is like, oh it raises your serotonin right away, but then why doesn't it affect your mood right away? And it doesn't do nothing after that dose, it does something. What it does is it changes your perception of the world around you. It almost shifts your perception in a more positively
Starting point is 01:27:43 valenced direction. So ambiguous things that happen to you, ambiguous information that you see in the world around you, you know, a colleague ignoring you in the corridor, an email with a title that could be like quite foreboding but could also mean nothing, your interpretations become more neutral or even more positive instead of having this kind of negative bias that's very common in depression and other kind of related conditions. So by doing that what antidepressants do is they enable you to experience the world in a slightly less negative way and for people for whom this is the kind of core driver of their depression, they work. For people for whom changing this
Starting point is 01:28:27 isn't a core driver or even perhaps they don't quite change this in those people, then I don't think they work. And so let's say that is one of the main mechanisms by which these SSRIs work when they do work. And there's a lot of patients who they don't work for. That's for sure. I mean, there's different rates if you look at the published studies and I don't know if you know the latest stats on that or not in terms of how many people they work for. I often say 40%, but they don't work for. So 60% they do work for. But even that 60%
Starting point is 01:29:01 often involves trying kind of more than one version. And, you know, so that it could even be, it could turn out to be about 50-50 when you turn up to your GP. Yeah. So given the potential side effects of these SSRIs, of which there are many, including suicidal ideation, it's published as a rare side effect, but nonetheless, it is a well recognized side effect. Yeah. And a really important one, obviously. Yeah, so if the mechanism by which they work is they help to change our perception of the
Starting point is 01:29:33 world, could one argue, well, you can change your perception of the world in a variety of different ways. You could perhaps see a therapist or do CBT or do some other exercises that can help you reframe and look at the world differently without all the potential negative side effects. So I think yes and no. There are some things that SSRIs do that I think are quite difficult to do intentionally. So what psychological therapy can definitely does help you do is it can help you engage with the world more. It can help you reevaluate where your beliefs about the world come from.
Starting point is 01:30:13 But actually, if your everyday life experiences and the kind of mass of interpretations that you have of everything that's happened in your day, in your past, in tomorrow, if all of that, the bulk of that information disagrees with what your therapist is telling you, with what you're working on therapeutically, then there may be a point at which to access the benefits
Starting point is 01:30:36 of that therapy or other interventions, changing that underlying perception could be critical. So for some people, therapy works after they've taken a drug or while they're taking a drug. That doesn't mean that antidepressants do something completely unique that nothing else can do. It might just mean that for some people, they are a kind of slightly unique pathway
Starting point is 01:30:59 towards accessing those other interventions. Yeah, on that theme of taking something that then perhaps changes or gets or helps jolt us out of our current viewpoint and helps us see the world through different eyes, that's something that people who take psychedelics will often repose. And you do cover this in the book. What do you think about psychedelic therapy? And I know you've got some interesting thoughts on placebo and the placebo response and psychedelics. So on the kind of pro side, I think psychedelics can work in this kind of rapid way that antidepressants don't, but they might work on more similar systems to something like therapy.
Starting point is 01:31:47 Things like your beliefs about the world around you, your beliefs about yourself, these seem to be able to be modulated by even like a single dose of some psychedelic drugs. And the research supporting that goes back many, many, many decades. But I do have one kind of really big pause when it comes to psychedelics. I very much support research in that area. I think it's been kind of suppressed for far too long. They certainly show potential. But my biggest worry is that so many of the big trials, trials showing like huge effects of what a psychedelic like psilocybin, active ingredient magic mushrooms could do for depression are impossible, impossible to dissociate from
Starting point is 01:32:32 the placebo effect because when you take a psychedelic you know that you're taking a psychedelic you just do and the problem is the placebo effect is so powerful and people probably underestimate just how powerful it is. It's even dependent on what you think the drug you're taking. So when you think you're taking a painkiller and it's say a sugar pill, the brain networks changed by that are different
Starting point is 01:32:57 than when you think you're taking an antidepressant, but it's a sugar pill. So it's not like you just get sugar pill brain networks, you get what you expect. So in psychedelic drug trials, because almost all of them have this problem where people can tell they're taking a psychedelic and they've also been reading everywhere in the news
Starting point is 01:33:17 about how great psychedelics are for mental health, it's really difficult to pull apart how much is placebo and how much is a true effect. There are ways to combat this. So there is another kind of class of drugs, so ketamine, which is not a classic psychedelic. In those trials, for a long time, they've been using as their placebo condition, a drug that also causes like somewhat similar effects. So if you're kind of drug naive, you haven't taken ketamine before,
Starting point is 01:33:45 you could easily be fooled by midazolam, which is their control condition. And what did that show? And those studies are generally quite positive, but they reduced- For ketamine. For ketamine, but they reduced in the kind of size of the effect dramatically when they started using this good placebo.
Starting point is 01:34:02 So I think that's what we'll see in psychedelics. Not necessarily that the effect just goes away entirely, but that it's actually more subtle, probably more variable between people than what it looks like right now. And for people who don't know these terms, psychedelics, ketamine, psilocybin, can you just break it down simply? Because I think there's people who read all this stuff and are very familiar what those things are, but there's other people who just want to improve their mental wellbeing. They're hearing a little bit about psychedelics, go, well, you know, are they all the same? What's the difference between ketamine and psilocybin? So if you
Starting point is 01:34:35 could just walk us through how to think about them. Yeah, completely. That would be helpful, I think. I mean, it's been a really interesting trajectory, actually. You know. When I started my PhD, drugs like psilocybin, active ingredient of magic mushrooms, classic psychedelic, another one would be LSD, these were drugs that people had used in scientific trials in the 1970s and earlier, but were really kind of out of fashion, you might say, but more than fashion, also kind of for legal reasons, for political reasons, very, very difficult to access
Starting point is 01:35:10 for even totally scientific robust trials and experiments. But there were some groups who'd started working on them at that time. And those groups have truly been proven right, I think in their kind of radicalism at the time, because these drugs that had been neglected scientifically, clinically, by the establishment, if you will,
Starting point is 01:35:32 actually seemed to have some kind of clinical potential, possibly for a wide range of conditions. Doesn't mean they're without side effects. So you mentioned antidepressant side effects. Psychedelics have side effects, too. Some antidepressant side effects, psychedelics have side effects too. Some people have really bad reactions to them, but it does mean they might hold promise. And you've seen a lot of excitement about that.
Starting point is 01:35:52 Now kind of in between, fools drugs like ketamine. It's a dissociative drug, it's like an anesthetic, but it's not a classic psychedelic. It doesn't cause kind of hallucinations or it also doesn't have the kind of drug properties that we associate with psychedelics. And that earlier had been shown to cause a rapid remission of depression symptoms.
Starting point is 01:36:14 And today there are ketamine clinics, including on the NHS, at least one I know of in the UK. So this is a kind of special way of treating depression for people for whom nothing else has worked. This anesthetic drug can sometimes deliver really rapid relief of symptoms. Via this mechanism of resetting of view of the world and perceptions? Well, that was my kind of hypothesized mechanism for psychedelics. I actually am not sure that ketamine works the same way. It could work a little bit. I know there's some data suggesting it might work a little bit on the kind of motivational side of things that we were discussing.
Starting point is 01:36:57 But there's also, you know, pharmacologically, it's so different than SSRIs. It's very different from psychedelics. It may have something to do with kind of neuroplasticity mechanisms, it seems to work on things related to that. But, you know, who knows really, we don't have a solid answer. Yeah, it's fascinating this whole idea of the placebo effect. And again, you really delve in detail in your book on the placebo effects and just how powerful it is. And, you know, I, for many years have thought we've kind of, we've looked down on the placebo effects. You know, we've always been a bit derogatory about it, but I think we should be harnessing it again.
Starting point is 01:37:31 No, no, the placebo effects is really, really powerful. Some people may be thinking, well, yeah, I get it for drug trials or when you're taking the medication, but come on, Camilla, for a psychedelic, surely you're going to know because you're not going to get all of those wild experiences when you're taking the placebo. But you're writing actually that some people do get fooled.
Starting point is 01:37:54 Yeah, exactly. So this just shows just how powerful expectations can be. So I said that in psychedelic trials, the placebo condition does feel very, very different than the psychedelic condition. So people can tell, but actually in other studies, if you only give people a placebo, but you really big it up, you say like, this is a new psychedelic that we've discovered,
Starting point is 01:38:16 it's gonna make you feel this and that, it's gonna feel a little bit like magic mushrooms. A good proportion of people, not everyone, but over 50% experience some kind of psychedelic like effect. Oh, I loved reading that study. There was even one participant in it who loved it so much. They didn't believe it when they were told it was a placebo and they said, you've got
Starting point is 01:38:38 to help me get my hands on this placebo again then. My God. So that's how powerful the placebo is. And why do you think the placebo is that powerful? I think it has to do with the strength of the role of expectations in the brain. So we talked a little bit how pain can be dampened up or amplified by brain mechanisms. And that's, you know, one thing that the placebo effect is an example of. It's an example of how your brain can kind of turn that dial up and down. But it's not just on pain, it's on everything.
Starting point is 01:39:15 So what we expect to see constrains our visual perception of the world. That's what causes visual illusions, is essentially a kind of visual placebo effect, a visual effect of our expectations. But of course, our senses are not just seeing. So what we feel in the body, emotionally and physically, is also subject to our expectations. So that is the route by which the placebo effect can change things all over the body. You even see differences in the spinal cord when people take a placebo.
Starting point is 01:39:47 So it's not just in the brain, which then transmits signals to the body in the spinal cord. You can even see literally differences in the spinal cord. Yeah, it's absolutely incredible. But then when you think about placebo, it goes so much further than this because, let's say with talk therapies for example or therapy we know that actually your relationship with the therapist is very very important in terms of whether that therapy is going to work and it's going to be effective for you. You know that patient with the toy train set with the toy train sets, I have to acknowledge that my interaction with that patient, my belief and what I said to that patient about hobbies and how important pleasure was going
Starting point is 01:40:37 to be for their mental wellbeing and give this a go, I honestly think it's going to help you. That is altering his expectation because then when he's going up the ladder to his attic that evening, if I had a good rapport with him, which I did, and I'm, I as a clinician have always tried to be very empowering to make sure people walk out the door feeling, oh, there's something I can do about this. Well, that's also going to play a role, isn't it? It's not just the train set. It's the idea I put in his brain about what that train set may or may not do. Exactly. This is a problem actually for a kind of psychological therapy trials and really sort of developing and honing new therapies.
Starting point is 01:41:16 It will always be influenced by the therapist themselves. But there's an optimistic side of that as well, which is that if you're like, oh, I've tried CBT before, it was crap. It didn't work for me. Might've actually been your particular relationship with that therapist and you might, you could try again and it could be slightly different. The flip side is, let's take back pain. Back pain is a leading cause of disability around the globe. We've covered this a little bit on the podcast with Howard Schubiner and this amazing physiotherapist from Australia, Peter O'Sullivan. And just sort of piggybacking off what we're talking about expectation and the placebo response, if you are, let's say, 35 and you've got backache and then your doctor scans your
Starting point is 01:42:02 spine, let's say they send you for an MRI. The way we report those MRIs is actually potentially very, very problematic. So it's very common to say degenerative disc disease, which frankly is a normal finding. But if you think about that language, degenerative disc disease, who wants to be told that they've got degeneration in their spine, right? So you're then this kind of fit 35 year old patient who has got back pain. You don't think it's serious, but it's really bothering you. After a few months of trying things aren't working, you persuade the doctor to send you for an MRI scan. The doctor goes, oh, there's nothing serious. You know, and you see the result and you read a degenerative spine.
Starting point is 01:42:49 What does that then do to your perception of your movement, your pain? I think it's massive. Yeah, I think so too. I think it's absolutely huge. And I don't think as doctors, we, I don't think we've been educated enough into how we communicate with our patients. If the placebo effect is as powerful as you're saying, and I believe it to be, actually the way we communicate anything to our patients is of the utmost importance.
Starting point is 01:43:18 It absolutely is. I even know of trials testing this directly, where the same intervention, because even treatments, kind of normal biological treatments, they partially work from a placebo effect. So actually they could be boosted potentially by the kind of communication that you're talking about. So I think in these trials, they're tweaking like what exactly patients get given
Starting point is 01:43:41 in manuals, but also probably what the clinicians say to them. So they're tweaking just expectations. Yes. So just as 10 people with depression could have 10 different driving factors that's causing their depression, which might mean they all need subtly different treatments, right? It also makes you think then as a clinician, and there's loads of doctors who listen to the show, pharmacists, nurses, physiotherapists, right, and osteopaths and beyond. Actually, when we think about the tools
Starting point is 01:44:12 that we have in our toolbox to help a given patient, we probably need to upgrade our view of our manner, our emotional state, how are we, you know, how much are we smiling, how much are we being positive and empowering, right? It's not just about the blood test and then the prescription we can write. It's the way we write it, the way we communicate also has to be upgraded to be thought of, I would say, at a similar level. I think so. And I think it actually, it explains a really interesting phenomenon in medicine.
Starting point is 01:44:49 And this is kind of outside of mental health. But if you look at many disorders, things like multiple sclerosis, stroke, ideas where you have exactly, you have a good prediction of how disabled someone should be based on their pathology. Actually, it corresponds poorly. There's a relationship, but it doesn't account
Starting point is 01:45:10 for all the variability in how disabled someone is by their specific lesion, by their specific degeneration. And in my opinion, that's at least partially because of this introspective sense that we have, the perception of the internal state of the body, which is powerfully modulated by expectations, of course, because that's how we know what to attend to, what's important, what's meaningful, what's negative, what's positive. And that is the bit that I think clinicians are working on in this manner in this relationship that you're talking about. It's changing the way you interpret your symptoms.
Starting point is 01:45:50 I want to talk about interoception and this idea that our expectations or our understanding of the world alters the way we interpret that symptom. So a very simple example might be, and actually this is hypothesized to be one of the reasons why exercise is so good for us is because it enhances our interoception, right? That's, I don't know if you've seen that research, certainly I find that fascinating. This idea that if you don't move your body regularly,
Starting point is 01:46:21 or you don't move it much at all, if you have an elevated heart rate for whatever reason, it may not feel good. You may think, what's going on? Am I sick? You start to feel anxious, right? Exactly. If you're running on a treadmill in the gym, your heart rate's going up, but you associate it positively. It's like, oh yeah, my heart rate's up. Like, yeah, I'm working my body. I'm moving it. So maybe not quite the same thing's happening, but from an increased heart rate, you're interpreting
Starting point is 01:46:50 it in two completely different ways because of your perception. So what is interreception? How is it related to our mental health? And what can we do to improve it? Interception is the sense of the internal condition of the body. So extraception is the sense of the outside world. So hearing is an example of extraception, seeing, even tasting is an extraceptive sense because you're tasting something you put on the tongue. But as soon as something is inside our body, whether that is our heart beating or something we've eaten that we're digesting or a bladder,
Starting point is 01:47:28 these are all sending signals up through the brain stem into the brain and we're processing these signals all the time, like a cacophony of signals, and that is interception. But interception is more than just listening to the body. It actually is predicting what the body will feel, processing those signals with knowledge of how the body has felt in the past and what those signals have meant. So that's how we can predict if we're about to get ill. So we're interpreting even things like inflammation in the body, a very generic sense of all around us, we're interpreting that with meaning. We might interpret those same signals slightly differently.
Starting point is 01:48:10 I don't know if we just run a marathon. So you might say, oh, I'm not getting ill. My body feels this way because I've just run a marathon. But I think in your example of how important interception is and why exercise might help kind of harness that interception. There is an additional thing happening. Yes, you get a new context for an increased heart rate. It's not necessarily bad. It's not necessarily anxiety, fear, or heart attack. It's something, maybe something good. But you also often get the ability to control that signal. So as you exercise you experience
Starting point is 01:48:46 these heightened physiological signals and you also experience the ability to maybe slow your breathing, maybe slow your pace and actually you discover that you have probably better control of your bodily signals than someone who's experiencing that increased heart rate purely as a reactive signal to the world around them. So, do we know from the research that people who struggle with their mental health have poorer levels of interoception? Does that make sense? Can you even say poorer levels? Yes, so there is a general decrease in your accuracy of interoception in a number of mental health conditions. So that might mean, for example, that you're slightly less good at telling when your heart
Starting point is 01:49:32 beats, or you can do this in breathing as well. So some of the experiments in my lab, we restrict people's breathing very subtly and we ask them when they can feel it. So people might be slightly less kind of able to detect these subtle differences. But to be honest, I think that's actually just one end of the spectrum. I think some people have very, very good interception and that's not necessarily for benefit. So I think if you have a really kind of harnessed sense of your body, but perhaps something awry in your interpretation of that body.
Starting point is 01:50:07 So you might be perfectly able to detect your heartbeats and other symptoms from the body, but your interpretation still might be somehow dysfunctional. That is also an interceptive problem, but it's not necessarily good or bad interception. It's at that other level, at that interpretative level. Yeah. It reminds me of a conversation I had a couple of years ago with this wonderful chap who runs a company called Physiology First in America. And he basically trains children and teenagers, adolescents. He gets them in saunas, cold plunges, he gets them on exercise bikes, he teaches them breathing techniques.
Starting point is 01:50:47 And one of the things he tries to do is teach these teenagers what an increased heart rate feels like and how you can control it. And then teach them how with their breathing, they can control their sensation of pain and discomfort. of pain and discomforts. And I guess one of the things he must be doing is helping them with their interoception. I think that's exactly right. He's essentially doing a kind of interceptive training where very kind of old psychological therapy they might have exposed someone with panic disorders to carbon dioxide inhalation, because that's what happens when you're having a panic attack.
Starting point is 01:51:28 And if you experience it and you learn to kind of control it and control your breathing, control your response to it, that can kind of stop a panic attack in its tracks, but you have to train for it. So that's what I think he's doing. Yeah. And incredibly since that podcast, he got contacted by so many people in the UK. He's actually opened up his first centre in the UK in sale, which is not far from here. And he's pretty incredible what he's trying to do.
Starting point is 01:51:52 And I think it's some of it at least is via this interoception mechanism. Given what you know about interoception and people want to learn more, there's a whole chapter on interoception in the balanced brain. What's your take on certain practices that can perhaps train us in interoception? So body scans, meditation, yoga, I believe you're a keen practitioner of yoga. Do you think those things are generally helpful for most people as a way of improving their mental health? I do. So we're actually running a big study at the moment looking at whether interception is
Starting point is 01:52:37 the crucial bit of mindfulness based therapies that makes them work for anxiety and depression. So we're very lucky that Wellcome invested in this long-term study to look at whether or not interception is kind of the key of this intervention. And if it is, which we think it might be, then actually boosting interception by a very simple things like training people to attend to their heartbeat could actually help things that we already know work for many people, like mindfulness-based therapies. But I also think kind of outside the maybe more clinical realm that other things also affect interception.
Starting point is 01:53:12 So I don't even think I'd heard of interception when I started practicing yoga regularly, but it really had a profound effect on my interpretation of signals from my body, partially because when I started, I was practicing hot yoga. So it's really, it's like incredibly hot. You're sweating buckets and your heart is going out of control. You really feel like you might pass out. And I'd actually, as a teenager, I'd had a couple panic attacks, which were really scary. I didn't know what they were.
Starting point is 01:53:39 And then in my kind of early 20s, late teens, early 20s, I started doing hot yoga and I realized I felt just like those panic attacks, but I never passed out. Actually, I could kind of keep going and it wasn't the end of the world and it actually wasn't necessarily negative. It was kind of a cool thing that I could overcome. So I really feel that was a sort of
Starting point is 01:54:03 introspective training that I experienced. In that case, it was a sort of introspective training that I experienced. In that case, it was a kind of introspective interpretation training, but also probably control because one of the things yoga teaches you is how to control your breathing and so on. Yeah, there's just, it's so fascinating interception, that sixth sense that maybe many of us have lost touch with. And there's some really good studies that I've seen on drug and alcohol addicts. And I think those who were trained to increase their interoception through, I think mindful breathing practices, reduced their rates of relapse and reduced their rates of depression
Starting point is 01:54:40 as well, which is pretty remarkable. So I think interoception is something that's very, very important. One thing I think a lot about is this idea that these days we've lost the art of paying attention to our internal signals and our bodies. I think one of the reasons is because it's never been easier to distract ourselves, right? So it's easy now to wake up in the morning and instead of paying attention to anything internal and in terms of how you're feeling, your mood, your heart rate, your breathing, any tightness, we can distract straight away on these devices, emails, podcasts, music, news, whatever it is. So my feeling is that we're constantly out there in the world. You know, so I often say that even if you're consuming really good
Starting point is 01:55:34 content, like I hope this podcast is for people, even if you're doing that, if you're doing that all the time and you're not having any time to listen to yourself, I think that's a problem. You're a neuroscientist at Cambridge. What's your take on that? I think people fall on that end of the spectrum more and more, perhaps. The end of the spectrum where actually a deficit in interpreting internal signals of the body can get you to a really dysfunctional place. That might mean a place that leads to anxiety or interpreting sort of physiological signals of anxiety kind of hyperactively, but it also, you know, we interpret these physical signals constantly to suggest really critical things like should we eat? Are we full?
Starting point is 01:56:25 These are signals that end up having huge repercussions for our physical health, but they depend on our interceptive ability. So if you're someone who isn't really listening to hunger or fullness, well, you might well not be eating when you should, how much you should and so on. Yeah, and you talk about that in the book,
Starting point is 01:56:43 don't you, about being hangry, right? So let's talk about being hangry, because it's something I think many people experience. And that is in some ways, I think, interoception gone wrong? Yeah, or right. It's the nature of interoception is that we use the insula, a region in the brain that helps us interpret signals from the body, also to tell us what we're feeling. So that means that signals from the body are one way of telling us our emotions. And so the very same signals that are telling us that we're hungry could actually, in another
Starting point is 01:57:18 context, have been telling us that we're distressed. So this region is saying there's this homeostatic disruption, this disruption to the balance of my body. And actually that could have been because of some kind of emotional disruption. So when someone feels hungry, so, you know, for people who don't know that term, you actually are hungry, your body wants food and let's say needs food, but you don't know that. That's right. You don't know it yet. And so you're moody and you're angry, hence hangry, but you don't realize that actually
Starting point is 01:57:55 it's not really anger. Well, I guess it is, but actually your mood is because you haven't eaten. I'm saying that's interoception gone wrong. You're saying it could be that it's gone right. Can we just tease that out? Because it's fascinating that. So what I'll say about hangry is that actually not everyone feels anger. It's often closer to irritability. Maybe you feel easily frustrated by things.
Starting point is 01:58:20 Maybe you actually just feel kind of quite emotional or sad. So these are all things that you can kind of mistakenly interpret the symptoms of hunger for. And the reason for that is that the way you interpret hunger, the signals that are sent to the brain, the way the brain interprets that is actually very similar to the way the brain interprets how it's feeling emotionally. So if I ask you how you're feeling emotionally, you're using these introspective senses to listen to your body. So that's the way that it's going right.
Starting point is 01:58:53 Because if you were to say, well, right now I'm feeling a bit anxious, you would have been listening, using your brain to listen to your heart, to maybe your muscle tone, it's feeling very tense right now. So that will have been what the brain is doing to tell you how you feel or disgust,
Starting point is 01:59:12 one of my favorite emotions actually, not to experience, but to experiment with. That involves signals being sent from the stomach. So the brain to interpret something as disgusting, part of what it's doing is it's listening to the stomach. For someone who then experiences hangar regularly, who ends up in a situation where they feel irritable, frustrated, moody, whatever it is, whereas actually it's, let's say their blood sugar is dropping a little bit and they actually need to eat. Can they retrain that? So enhance their interreceptive ability to
Starting point is 01:59:53 detect what's going on and how do they do that? I definitely think people can get better at detecting the sort of true source of hangar. Some of it is with sort of knowing it's been a kind of certain amount of time since your last meal. But some of it is probably through getting slightly better at detecting the sort of subtle physiological differences between feeling hunger and feeling irritable. For example, in this case,
Starting point is 02:00:19 also the involvement of the stomach. So if you feel angry and irritable, but you're good at telling my stomach is quite empty right now, it's maybe rumbling a little bit, that's going to help you tell you the source of those feelings and that the source is hunger and not in fact your emotions. Yeah, you're studying interoception a lot. So I'm going to again, put something else to you, which I've discovered, health trackers and wearables. Okay, so I don't think that for everyone,
Starting point is 02:00:47 I think some people can have quite a dysfunctional relationship with them, become anxious, every little figure that goes slightly wrong, it can stress them out. I definitely think that is real and people can become so dependent on them that they can't function without them. At the same time, I think there are some people
Starting point is 02:01:04 who thrive wearing health trackers and health wearables. So let's take a CGM, for example, so continuous glucose monitor that traditionally has only been used for people with type one and type two diabetes. More commonly now in health and wellness, some people are using it to help them understand
Starting point is 02:01:24 how they respond to different foods. Now what I've seen with some patients and what I've experienced myself is that wearing a CGM now and again actually can help you with your interoception because you start to then go, oh wow, oh I can see that the sugar's dropping there and that correlates when I feel tired and a bit low. And again, to be really clear, I think things can be overused, but I think if they're used intentionally and carefully, I believe that they can enhance your interoception and your
Starting point is 02:02:00 ability to detect what's going on. I've been experimenting with this Woot band for a few months. I spoke to Kristin Holmes, the chief scientific officer there a few months ago, had a great conversation with her on the show and I thought, yeah, you know what, let me try this. It's been really fun for me and I can't say that's for everyone, but for me, I feel I understand my body signals now much better than before using this. So heart rate variability, this marker of the state of my nervous system essentially and how much stress it is under, how well recovered it is. I am starting to pick up now, like I can predict quite nicely, not 100% of the time, but I
Starting point is 02:02:43 would say, I would probably say 90, 95% of the time, I can predict roughly where my HRV is going to sit because of what I've learned. That's very cool. You've done essentially some kind of biofeedback training on yourself where you've like begun to tell what the signals are that are leading something to respond in that way.
Starting point is 02:03:02 I love that, also I love the example of the CGM, something that people, you know, has a clear medical purpose, but has also kind of given you information that you use crucially when you're not even wearing a CGM. So you kind of learned what it feels like thanks to the CGM, which is very cool. Yeah. So anyway, maybe that can factor into some of your research in the in the coming years, because I'm always thinking, well, how do you help people with their interoception? Wearables are here and they're here to stay.
Starting point is 02:03:29 I don't think they're necessarily for everyone, as I say, but I think for some people, if you're quite intentional, I think they can be pretty profound. So let's see where that field goes. I also think it depends what information your wearable gives you. So we were having a chat in my lab the other day about if your wearable gives you kind of like maybe more negative information, it says like, oh, you had a really bad quality sleep last night, sometimes even more than you would have perceived that actually could have negative repercussions. So you might want your wearable to be like a little bit biased.
Starting point is 02:03:58 Yeah, that study has been done. Actually, there was one study where they took people, where there's been multiple studies on this, they took people into a lab and I think from recollection, one half, one group were slept for five hours, the other group was slept for eight hours, but then the group who slept for eight hours, they told had only slept for five hours and vice versa and you could actually see a difference later on in the day in their subjective feelings and I think some physiological findings, which again speaks to what you've been saying about the placebo response and how powerful our mind is and our expectation. I'm slightly annoyed that someone else has already done that experiment because I wanted
Starting point is 02:04:40 to, but that is really cool. Well, do it. Repeat it. I'll show you the link to that so you can see it. But maybe you can repeat it. I think it'll be fascinating, that sort of stuff. Okay. Just before we got into the studio, you were talking to me about some new things that you've
Starting point is 02:04:54 been really excited about recently. Metabolism and our mental health. Can you share anything about that? Yeah. My lab has taken this slightly surprising turn recently because we've become interested in, we've always been interested in kind of signals from the gut, but actually the signals that we get from the gut are very complex. It's not kind of, there's, you know, some electrical signals that you get just from
Starting point is 02:05:17 the contraction of the stomach, which we've studied before. These are the signals that are involved in things like disgust, nausea, fullness. But actually, another kind of signal that we get are the sorts of signals we get eventually from the gut, from the things that we eat that then get transmitted to the kind of large metabolic system all over our body. And what we found is that there may be interesting consequences of having metabolic differences in the body. So as people, we looked at people's kind of diabetes risk score across people with and
Starting point is 02:05:53 without diabetes, and we found that people with a higher risk for diabetes and then people with type two diabetes show differences in that effort cost decision-making that I was discussing. So there's actually a kind of a bias towards conserving energy in people with you might think worse metabolic health. So the kind of, the better your metabolic state, the more likely you are to engage in effortful behavior that involves some kind of effort cost. So just quite interesting, a sort of brain implication of your metabolic health.
Starting point is 02:06:28 Yeah, and I guess if we think about that theme of bi-directional communication, do we know which one comes first? No, we don't. So, I think it's totally plausible that these might be natural brain differences in the population that lead us to behave differently, eat differently, exercise differently, and then that leads to metabolic consequences. It's absolutely fascinating. Wow. I mean, look, Camilla, there's just so much in your book we haven't even got to yet. And I don't think we'll have time in this conversation. I'm sure there's enough for a part two at some point later on in the year.
Starting point is 02:07:05 We were talking about drugs before. One drug we didn't mention was cannabis. And I think it's worth just pausing on that for a moment because there's a lot of people out there who were smoking weed. I know a lot of parents are concerned with how much their children or their teenagers are smoking weed and you know cannabis. What is the latest research saying about the effects of cannabis on our brain and our risk of mental health problems?
Starting point is 02:07:35 I think it's such a relevant question because more and more places are legalizing cannabis and what consequences will that have on health? I think in some ways we don't entirely know. What we do know is that there are kind of quite important chemical differences between the cannabis that people are smoking today and the cannabis that people were smoking several decades ago. And that has to do with the kind of constituent parts.
Starting point is 02:08:02 THC versus cannabidiol, which are the kind of two chemicals that people focus on in cannabis, have a particular ratio. And as time has gone on, the ratio of THC has gone up relative to cannabidiol. And this seems quite important when it comes to abuse of cannabis, but also possibly side effects. So I even know of a clinical trial treating cannabis dependence with cannabidiol, so with one of the ingredients. And so that's how significant these kind of two constituent parts are. People have heard of CBD.
Starting point is 02:08:42 CBD, yes, sorry, I should have said that. No, no, I'm just trying to make sure people are following along in the sense of cannabidiol is shortened to CBD because I think people are seeing that that's coming out more and more for pain and other reasons. So you're saying the balance has changed over time so colloquially the weed of the 1960s is not the same as the weed of 2025. Yeah, totally. But what can this tell us? It's not just a bad thing.
Starting point is 02:09:08 It could actually help us understand how even the same drug could be more negative or more positive depending on how it's made. So in places like states in the US that have legal cannabis, they should be using this information to inform sort of what kind of cannabis is legal. I don't know if they are, but that would be sort of one thing I would do
Starting point is 02:09:31 based on that line of research. And I think the other thing to bear in mind about the risks of cannabis, of which there are definitely some, and they're worth talking about, is that sometimes those risks should be looked at relative to other legal drugs like alcohol. So alcohol is actually quite a dangerous drug
Starting point is 02:09:49 and just because it's legal, people sort of neglect that. And I suppose in my book, I could be accused of being a bit like pro recreational drugs, but that's only relative to alcohol. So if we have a standard of legality with alcohol, I think we need to kind of change which other drugs are legal. And my hope would actually be that this would reduce people's
Starting point is 02:10:11 consumption of alcohol, which can have really long-term negative consequences, but also immediate negative consequences on health. Yeah. I don't think you could be accused of that. I see what you're saying. I think you've managed that chat so really well. And you know, you mentioned Professor David Nutz, who I hope to speak to later this year on the podcast and in terms of his views on drugs and alcohol and how we've kind of got a bit messed up with how we've classified these things legally and you know, alcohol really is, it has a huge potential for harm, yet it's legal. Given what you said about cannabis, and then obviously the logical line from that is when we talk about cannabis and its impact on the brain and mental health, we can't really because
Starting point is 02:10:57 we have to specify what percentages are we talking about because actually that balance is going to have a different effect. Understanding and acknowledging that complexity, are you able to give a more broader guidance for anyone who's concerned either with their own cannabis intake or their children's or family members, for example, of which I know there are many. Are you able to sort of give a general take on what you think regular smoking of cannabis is doing to our risk of mental health problems? Or is it too hard to say? No, it depends.
Starting point is 02:11:35 I cannot say completely blanket, but there are some very interesting possible risks, especially depending on the age that you smoke cannabis. So people have, for a long time, and I'm sure many of your listeners will be aware that there is a relationship between smoking cannabis and development of symptoms associated with schizophrenia like psychosis.
Starting point is 02:11:57 So this is a really important association to understand, but the association does actually run in both directions. So one of the reasons that correlation exists and is relatively strong is because if you have a genetic propensity for psychosis, you are more likely to smoke cannabis and to do it earlier than people without. So it does run in both directions, but if anything, it's kind of stronger in the, if you have a risk for psychosis, you smoke cannabis direction. So it's not something worth neglecting.
Starting point is 02:12:30 It's something worth bearing in mind when we talk about that association. I would also say dependence on lots of different substances is bad. It's not the substance itself necessarily. It's not like somehow being addicted to cannabis is worse somehow being addicted to cannabis is worse than being addicted to alcohol.
Starting point is 02:12:47 You could probably argue the other way around, but that actually if you feel you have a problem with functioning because of a substance, any substance, that's something you need to seek help for you or a loved one. Yeah, it's not the drug, it's your relationship to the drug. Yeah. Yeah. Okay, look, we've covered so many different things,
Starting point is 02:13:04 these kind of brain pathways of pleasure, pain, motivation and learning. We've covered this idea that there's no one size fits all. We've gone through various drug treatments, antidepressants, how they potentially might work, the power of the placebo response, interoception, you know, all the different causes. I mean, I think, I think we've covered a lot. Is there anything that we haven't covered that you think is really important for people to know?
Starting point is 02:13:30 I suppose there is one thing we haven't talked about, probably because it's weird and many people won't have heard of it or thought about it in the context of mental health. I'm excited already. Yeah, all right. And that's brain stimulation. So this is an area that I think emerges mired in controversy in mental health because people think of ECT, electroconvulsive therapy. But actually, the way that it's used today very, very rarely is something like ECT used
Starting point is 02:13:58 only in the most extreme cases, and for them it is often effective. But for most people, they wouldn't be appropriate for ECT, but they might not be responding to other kinds of treatments. And that's where I think non-invasive brain stimulation approaches like transcranial magnetic stimulation or TMS hold really quite significant promise. There are now substantial studies, big meta-analyses, looking at all those studies' effects, showing that something like TMS has about the same effect as antidepressants, meaning it doesn't work for everyone,
Starting point is 02:14:36 but it works for a large proportion of people for depression. And it feels like a sort of quite intense tapping on your head. What it does is it increases the activation of brain cells underneath where you're putting this, it's called a TMS coil. So it's a quite localized effect,
Starting point is 02:14:51 although it also kind of changes the circuitry related to that location. And it sounds weird and sci-fi and so on, but what I can say is that I think the evidence is convincing enough that more people should know about it and be able to look into it and access it should they not respond to other treatments. And is this commonly available now with psychiatrists? I think not commonly available enough.
Starting point is 02:15:14 So there are a small number of clinics, some NHS clinics, but at least in the UK, there really isn't widespread availability despite kind of quite high efficacy rates. Absolutely fascinating. If we were to have a conversation in 12 months, I'm fascinated to see where this field would have moved on, where all this research that your lab is doing and other labs are doing is gonna be. Honestly, I think you've written a wonderful book.
Starting point is 02:15:41 It's so rich in information. It's quite a unique take on various aspects of the brain, which really got me thinking. And I've tried to explore a lot of those ideas with you today. To finish off then, if anyone's listened to this conversation, Camilla, and is thinking, well, I need to, not I need to, I'd like to do a little bit more for my mental health, dare I say, my mental wellbeing. With the understanding that everyone's different, do you have some general guidance for people given that you're a neuroscientist and you've been in this field for a long, long time now? Yeah, I would tell you what I would do,
Starting point is 02:16:25 which is I think one of the most important messages you can take from my book is not the sort of depressing generalization that nothing works for everyone, but actually that I think something works for everyone. And that's something I hope for you, it already exists. But it is possible that that is still in development, that we still need more science to discover that. But it's coming.
Starting point is 02:16:51 It's coming quickly. There are huge resources dedicated to developing new innovative mental health treatments working as we speak. So I think that kind of diversity of treatments is a good thing, as as, you know, sometimes a bad thing. Yeah, very empowering. And are there two or three kind of general practices that you think
Starting point is 02:17:12 actually whilst you can't say for sure this is going to work for you, these are generally good things to be doing for your mental well-being and your physical well-being. I guess from your research, you know, where would you go if you were going to recommend some general practices for people? Yeah, the two biggest things I would say, one is obvious, one is less obvious. So one is to focus as much as you can on things like sleep hygiene and also exercising if you can. These sort of basic things can often be the difference between resilience in the face of great mental health challenges that we all sadly experience at some point in our life and not. So I think focusing on those kind of crucial fundamental aspects of health if you can. And then the second
Starting point is 02:18:00 thing I would say is pleasure. Even if you feel that a life of austerity is like on a pinnacle, something we all need to be aspiring to, I think actually you need to look at what is pleasurable in your life and don't neglect it. Yeah, I love it. Camilla, you're doing great work. Thank you for writing such a wonderful book and thank you for coming up to the studio. I can't wait for our next conversation. Oh, thank you so much for having me. Really hope you enjoyed that conversation. Do think about one thing that you can take away and
Starting point is 02:18:37 apply into your own life. And also have a think about one thing from this conversation that you can teach to somebody else. Remember when you teach someone, it not only helps them, it also helps you learn and retain the information. Now, before you go, just wanted to let you know about Friday 5. It's my free weekly email containing five simple ideas to improve your health and happiness.
Starting point is 02:19:03 In that email, I share exclusive insights that I do not share anywhere else, including health advice, how to manage your time better, interesting articles or videos that I've been consuming, and quotes that have caused me to stop and reflect. And I have to say, in a world of endless emails, it really is delightful that many of you tell me it is one of the only weekly emails that
Starting point is 02:19:25 you actively look forward to receiving. So if that sounds like something you would like to receive each and every Friday, you can sign up for free at drchattyjee.com forward slash Friday five. If you are new to my podcast, you may be interested to know that I have written five books that have been bestsellers all over the world, covering all kinds of different topics, happiness, food, stress, sleep, behaviour change, movement, weight loss, and so much more. So please do take a moment to check them out. They are all available as paperbacks, ebooks, and as audiobooks, which I am narrating. If you enjoyed today's
Starting point is 02:20:04 episode, it is always appreciated if you can take a moment to share the podcast with your friends and family, or leave a review on Apple podcasts. Thank you so much for listening. Have a wonderful week, and please note that if you want to listen to this show without any adverts at all, that option is now available for a small monthly fee on Apple and on Android all you have to do is click the link in the episode notes in your podcast app and always remember you are the architect of your own health making Lysol changes always worth it because when you feel better you live
Starting point is 02:20:43 more Because when you feel better, you live more.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.