The Happiness Lab with Dr. Laurie Santos - The Happiness Lab Meets No Stupid Questions
Episode Date: August 9, 2021There are no stupid questions when it comes to discussing happiness - so Dr Laurie Santos has joined forces with fellow psychologist Angela Duckworth (host of the No Stupid Questions podcast from Frea...konomics Radio) for a special crossover episode to talk about how we all want to fit in; why we should do dinner and movie on separate date nights; why we should imagine bad things happening to our homes; and why a change for the worse in your life can actually be a happiness gift.Check out No Stupid Questions wherever you get your podcasts! Learn more about your ad-choices at https://www.iheartpodcastnetwork.comSee omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Pushkin. like the Doritos Taco Bell mashup thing. The most horrifying new one of these is Heinz just did this
crazy new thing where they're doing all these weird mayo ketchups and stuff, but they did an
Oreo mayo ketchup. That sounds terrible. But this will not be terrible because today we are doing
a cognitive science mashup of the two best cognitive science podcasts out there. Let's be
real. Laurie Santos from the Happiness Lab, along with Angela Duckworth from No Stupid Questions.
And we are using this No Stupid Questions format that my usual partner, Stephen Dubner, and I like so much, which is that one of us asks a question of the other.
And then we just have a rambling conversation.
I'm in for the rambling conversation.
That's usually what we have together when we meet up, Angela.
Let's do it.
Thank the Lord that you're here.
I'm Angela Duckworth.
And this is Laurie Santos.
And you're listening to No Stupid Questions meets The Happiness Lab.
Today on the show, why do we mirror the accents and mannerisms of the people we meet?
Monkey A observes Monkey B do something different with a banana.
I don't know if that's stereotyping.
They do like bananas.
Totally true.
Also, why can happiness seem so elusive even when things are going well?
Holy schmoly, this is the best life I could possibly be living.
Why am I not a 10 out of 10 on happiness?
is the best life I could possibly be living. Why am I not a 10 out of 10 on happiness?
I think you have the first question for me, correct?
I do. And this comes from a listener named Sabika Shaban, who hails from Qatar and is a graduate student there. This question is as follows. I have a question about mimicking
the mannerisms of people we meet, especially in our multicultural
environments.
Often in conversations with people with very marked accents different from my own, I find
my own accent taking on nuances of theirs or interjecting typical expressions from their
language culture.
Is this a typically observed behavior?
And on the flip side, are there
many whose accents and mannerisms never change regardless of who they speak to and a reason why
some do and some don't? Lori, I love this question. I am so vibing with it. Sabika, that is me. I
spent two years in Oxford and I ended up speaking almost involuntarily with this faux British posh accent. And I am from southern New Jersey, so I should not be doing that. Does this happen to you, too?
the most painful British mannerisms. Like what? Like if you told me something shocking you did,
like, oh my gosh, Lori, I just went to the pub and I had like eight pints. I would respond to that with the phrase, you walk. And I brought that back to the U.S. for like a year. And my
friends were like, stop. It'd be one thing if you just talk with a fake British accent would be bad
enough. But the fact that you're you walk all the time, it's just terrible. Wait, you knew this and you still did it, right?
It wasn't an affectation that you were doing in an ironic Brooklyn way.
Yeah.
And I think you know this, Angela, about me, but I grew up in New Bedford, Massachusetts
and had what to the uninitiated would sound like a terrible Boston accent.
You're from Boston.
You'd know it was a New Bedford accent, just like Pachyakad, Havadiyad.
Basically, I couldn't say R to the point that I actually lost a research assistant position my freshman year in college.
So I started out my freshman year working in Steve Kosslin's lab with Kevin Oxner,
who's a professor at Columbia. He was doing some study where he just needed a female voice to say
letters. And so I started recording these letters like A, B, C. And then I got to Q,
started recording these letters like A, B, C. And then I got to Q, R. And he was like, well, you can't say R. You have to say R. I was physically incapable of doing that except
sounding like a pirate. I was like, R. And he was like, no, it's just R. Anyway, so now you notice
that in my perfect podcast speak, I can say R. But the reason I think that happened was that
I ended up my freshman year being paired with a roommate from New Orleans who also had an incredibly thick, this time Southern accent.
Somehow, again, perfectly unconsciously, we took on each other's vocal cadences to the point that by our senior year,
this is back when your college room would have a single phone that someone would call and you wouldn't know who had picked up except by their voice.
And in fact, people couldn't tell me and my roommate Catherine apart by our senior year because our voices had converged so much.
You took her ours. Did she lose hers?
She stopped saying y'all as much. In fact, we both picked up another expression from
our Pittsburgh roommate, which was yins.
I like that because you guys is apparently an offensive, gendered, presumptuous,
possibly hostile sounding appellation. my own students tell me i think yin's and y'all
are better but the beauty is we just do this naturally in fact this is an evolved part of
human cognition researchers call this behavioral contagion this is the kind of thing that you see
in animals you know classic cases if you watch fish they tend to school around and it looks like
they're all kind of copying each other's behavior. There's some lovely work by this guy, Ian Cousin, who does all these detailed mathematical
network analyses of how fish school around. But the upshot is they're just soaking up each other's
behavior quite naturally. How do you know that they are really mimicking each other as opposed
to all responding to the same little piece of floating kelp. He does these incredibly detailed mathy things that I'm not going to be able to pull up on the fly.
He can actually do some predictive coding based on one fish's behavior about what's going to happen with the schooling.
And so it really does seem to be behavioral contagion.
But you don't need to look to fish.
This is something that we do quite naturally.
One of the most famous examples of this came from my colleague here at Yale, John Barge, and his colleague, Tanya Chartrand. They found out about this effect that they call
the chameleon effect. Well-labeled because it's cases where people just chameleonly copy other
people's behavior. What's an example? They had subjects get interviewed by a scary experimenter.
So there's just like high status person that you think you're being interviewed by. And what the experimenter did, unbeknownst to the subjects, was just occasionally
take on strange movements. So she would touch her face or put her arm in a particular way or cross
her legs. And what you find is that as they're being interviewed, they unconsciously copy all
these behaviors. So as the experimenter is touching her face, they touch their face more. As she's
folding her legs, they fold their legs more.
Again, totally unconsciously.
And their later work shows that this happens more in the high status direction.
So you're more likely to unconsciously copy the high status people, maybe just because you're watching them more honestly.
This is the classic work of Al Bandura also, right?
Where the little children who watch an adult take a toy like a Bobo doll and beat it
with a hammer, then when entering a room with a Bobo doll, just like they saw, they will walk
over and start beating it with a hammer. Whereas Bandura points out, they don't do that in a
control condition where they have not seen an adult model this. So I guess the question I'm asking is,
is the phenomenon that you're describing different from modeling or basically the same?
It's probably the mechanism that leads to this kind of stuff. I mean, you know this well. In
cognitive science, we often don't know the basic mechanisms that lead to other stuff down the line.
And there's lots of hypotheses that things like behavioral contagion lead to lots of nasty stuff.
In fact, Pandora is about people beating up a Bobo doll. But there's evidence from people like Francesca Gino and Dan Ariely that behavioral contag bring these subjects into the lab, college students,
and give them a super, super hard math test. It's basically impossible. And so if you're a subject,
you're experiencing like, oh my God, this is terrible. I'm never going to finish this.
And then you watch one subject raise their hand immediately, like two seconds into the experiment
and say, yeah, I'm done. What can I do? And the experimenter is like, if you're done, well,
you can just shred your answers so no one sees them and we'll pay you. And so if you're the subject, you're thinking, wait a minute,
they're not even going to check that I did them. They're just going to shred it. And what Ariely
and colleagues find is you're more likely to cheat on these problems if you see somebody else
cheating. But the neat thing is that it's not just if you see somebody else cheating,
it actually has to be somebody that relates to you. And so they manipulated this in a cute way. They're doing this stuff at Duke University.
So the Duke students are there in this study, and the person that raises their hand and cheats
is in a Duke University sweatshirt. Now, all of a sudden, cheating goes up a lot.
But if the person's in a UNC sweatshirt, who are like the losers from the other school,
now, all of a sudden, people are like, oh my god, I'm not going to cheat. It actually reinforces moral behavior. And so this is another thing we know about behavioral contagion,
which is kind of weird. We're more likely to contagiously pick up on the behaviors of people
who we see as our in-group members, who we see as high status, who we pay attention to.
So I wanted to ask you what you thought about mirror neurons. I, as more or less
an outsider to this literature, have only read articles about these specialized neurons that if
I see you doing a particular action, they are lighting up in my brain as if I were doing the
same action. So A, is that an accurate description of mirror neurons? And B, what up with mirror
neurons, Lori? I'm kind of not a fan of mirror
neurons. I'll be totally honest. There's a lot of hype around mirror neurons, but what they
actually do might not be as cool as we sometimes think. Basically, these were discovered in monkeys
in a very famous set of experiments in Italy back in the early 90s, where monkeys were watching
humans engaging in these actions and areas of the monkey motors, where monkeys were watching humans engaging in these actions
and areas of the monkey motor cortex, the spot that would fire if they were grabbing for something,
when they were watching these humans grabbing for something, they tended to fire. So it seems
super cool. Like, oh my gosh, the same neuron in me that fires when I reach, fires when I see you
reach. Maybe neurons are the code for empathy.
Maybe these neurons are the seat of our perspective, taking all the stuff.
What we know about them is they only exist in motor cortex. So it's for very specific motoric movements like grabbing and reaching. There's a couple mirror neurons in other spots.
There's some that might be in attention region. So for eye gaze turning and stuff like that,
but not as rich as you think.
And I think there is some argument that human beings are unique in their ability to learn through observation, whereas a dog or even a chimpanzee can't do it or at least can't do it as well, which is kind of weird, right?
Because the mirror neurons were mostly found in monkeys.
In terms of learning by observation, animals do do that. But what they don't learn by is imitation. Like I see
you behave in this very specific sequence and I copy all of those very mechanical behaviors
perfectly. That's literally what monkeys don't do. Wait, what do monkeys do? So monkey A observes
monkey B do something different with a banana. I don't know
if that's stereotyping. They do like bananas. Totally true. Okay, good. I'm glad that holds up.
Anyway, what does happen if it's not imitation? What is it? Here's one study that looked at this.
This is not with monkeys, but with chimpanzees. For our listeners out there, pet peeve of people
who work with primates, monkeys, not chimpanzees. Chimpanzees actually eat monkeys, so totally
different. What? It's like saying a human is a tuna fish sandwich or
something. So with chimpanzees, they have this task where there's a bunch of food outside of
some enclosure and chimp has to use a tool to get it. They give a chimp a rake, basically,
where you could try to use a rake with the tines down like we'd normally rake leaves. But if they're
tiny pieces of food, that works sort of, but not super well because the food goes through the tines down like we'd normally rake leaves. But if they're tiny pieces of food, that works
sort of, but not super well because the food goes through the tines. Whereas if you flip the rake
over and you have that part of the rake that's flat, you can scoop the food up more effectively.
So they show kids this behavior. And what you find is the kid will copy whatever the human does.
Whereas if you do the same thing with chimpanzees, they don't necessarily fully copy what the human does. They realize like,
oh, I can use a rake to try to get the food. And then they trial and error it. So they're kind of
copying the fact that you're using this tool and you can do it. But what they're not copying is
the perfect actions that go with it. It sounds smarter. Doesn't it sound a little bit more evolved, as it were?
It is smarter, yeah.
In fact, there's a wonderful bias
that is a perhaps human unique bias.
We have some evidence that you don't see it
in primates or in dogs,
which is called over-imitation,
this idea that we imitate too much.
If you see somebody doing something that's inefficient,
or in the case of this Ariely study,
we talked about bad, like immoral, you inadvertently copy it anyway. Have you read the studies of Christine
Laguerre, this developmental psychology work on children imitating others? Yeah, and she finds
with over-imitation, but part of it seems to be automatic, but part of it seems to be because
this kind of behavioral contagion is our way of showing, hey, I'm in the group like you. And this gets to another way that you could
think about switching accents in particular, this idea of code switching. So code switching is if
you're a member of a minority group and you're in a majority group situation, you sort of switch
your behavior around to match what the majority group is doing, which is sometimes considered not a great thing, but arguably, as you're pointing out, is adaptive.
Totally. If I look back at my own accent switching, my New Bedford accent wasn't going to necessarily work super well in Ivy League classes. That wasn't the way these high status, higher class people talked. It's no secret that my accent switched more towards a Ivy League vernacular
English. Right. So we were both in England, which was a higher status accent, one could argue,
certainly than my native South Jersey. So I start speaking a faux British accent. But if a British
scholar, for whatever reason, came to southern New Jersey and had to spend a summer down the shore, they would not
adopt the local vernacular, right? That would be the prediction because of status.
Status is part of it for sure. But I think also just functionally getting the inside scoop and
seeming like you belong and you're like an insider at that place. So my prediction is
Brit might do it less in southern New Jersey than this southern New Jersey or would do it in the UK, but they would to a certain extent. And this is a reason
that, again, I have lost, sadly, my New Bedford accent until I go back to New Bedford for a couple
of days. And then I all of a sudden sound like I've been there my whole life. I want to hear a
New Bedford happiness. I think you should do it full on and maybe you could record it there.
Another time it there. Another time
it happens, maybe you got this too when you were in the UK is when I'm drunk. Those more automatic
accents come back. It's weird. I haven't been drunk since I was 18, but that is probably a
different question. So I'll have to actually get data on that and come back to you. There's an
experiment we could do, Angela. Okay, so now I get to ask a question, right? Which is so cool. We don't normally do
this on the Happiness Lab. You need another person to be hanging out with. That's true.
You're welcome anytime on the Happiness Lab, Angela. Thank you. But here's question number
two. Amelia asks, why is it that so many people are restless or unsatisfied, even in terrific
or satisfying circumstances.
Her context is that she's in a really lovely place with lots of supportive and happy colleagues.
She's exceeded all these expectations she's had for herself professionally, but still finds
herself looking around at other opportunities, kind of feeling unhappy, thinking she should
switch everything. And then she goes on to ask, why can't I just wait it out? Why the need for
change? Maybe at some level, people don't want to be happy what is the deal scientifically this is such a
great question I think it is timely because as we both were very sad to know Ed Diener the scientist
who arguably more than anyone put the scientific study of happiness on the map. He passed away
very recently. So I feel like this question is a way also for us to honor the great Ed Diener,
who is amazing and whose stuff we talk about all the time on the Happiness Lab.
So I will begin by saying that I had this experience myself. I remember when I was,
I think, 18 years old, writing to Dear Abby, saying how unhappy I was. There were extenuating
circumstances. Mostly, I was an adolescent. So that's partly your job as a adolescent to be
unhappy with your circumstances. Wait, wait, wait, wait, time out. You actually wrote to Dear Abby?
Legit wrote to Dear Abby. Yes. It was the 1980s. So I wrote a letter, put it in an envelope,
1980s. So I wrote a letter, put it in an envelope, licked it, sealed it, put a stamp on it and mailed it away. And what my letter said was more or less that I felt like I had a perfect life. I had just
gotten into college. I was going to Harvard, so I was getting lots of praise from my Asian family.
And my boyfriend at the time was somebody I thought I would spend maybe the rest
of my life with. And I had a wonderful best friend and all these great things were happening to me.
And there was this contrast from the objective awesomeness of my life and my
angst. I was just like, I'm not happy. Abby, what's wrong with me?
What did Abby say?
She said to go see a therapist.
Go see a therapist are four excellent words of advice,
but I was very disappointed at the time.
And you know what, Lori?
I wish I had listened to her
because it took me a couple more decades
to realize that when I'm in the state of mind
where I'm so unhappy,
I need to write a letter to a stranger to ask them
for help, then really what I should do is go see a therapist. But there are times where you just
look around at your life. In any objective sense, you realize, holy schmoly, this is the best life
I could possibly be living. And you have this gnawing sense of dissatisfaction, like, why am I not a 10 out of 10 on happiness? Now,
I want to start our scientific discussion of this with this very famous idea of the hedonic
treadmill, which I know you probably already discussed on your podcast. Am I right on that?
Yeah, we had a fantastic guest on to talk about the hedonic treadmill, Clay Cockrell. He's a
wealth psychologist, so he's a mental health professional for the 0. treadmill. Clay Cockrell, he's a wealth psychologist. So he's a mental
health professional for the 0.001%. First off, that's telling, right? That we have to have mental
health professionals for the 0.001%. You think these people would be like, they're not so happy
that they're like, no, I'm good. Yeah. They need him. And the problems he sees in his patients are
just, I mean, if you're not in the 0.001%, you kind of get a little bit schadenfreude because they're things like, I don't know where to park my yacht. And you're
like, well, dude, maybe if you don't have a yacht. But the point is, these ostensibly objectively
terrific circumstances don't always feel terrific. And that is the hedonic treadmill. We kind of just
get used to stuff. So if you have something objectively awesome happen, you notice and you
feel that it is good and it affects your happiness for a short while, but then you kind of just get used to it.
And that's the idea of the treadmill. You like keep running and running and you stay in the
same place. But I think we would both agree that there is to some extent a phenomenon by which
through either things that we do intentionally or unconsciously, we do come back from either extreme, like too happy or the
opposite. The flip side is that we also get used to circumstances that are pretty awful. They don't
continue to affect our psychology as bad as when they first happened. So you break up or you lose
a job. Those things suck for a while. And they feel like they're going to suck forever. I think
that's one of the fascinating
things about emotions when you're in the middle of one, like when you're anxious or lonely or
extremely sad, you can't really see around the corner. Even if intellectually you realize like,
oh, yeah, I've been in this kind of place before and I've seen things get better, but it doesn't
feel that way in any visceral sense. But though we would agree with
that, I think one of the nuances here that is important to underscore is that the returning
to the set point isn't always exactly to where you were before. So the famous 1978 study of
accident victims who became paraplegics, it's often described as follow them long enough, you see that they come
back to where they were before their accident. But sadly, not quite. Yes, they adapt hedonically,
but not all the way back on average to where they were before.
Yeah. And there's a few cases like that where hedonic adaptation isn't perfect. I think another
one is in the context of unemployment. That's
another case where you go down a little bit. Actually, one that's the opposite is divorce.
You have a hit to your happiness when you first get divorced, but you actually pop up past baseline.
This is the thing about these happiness set points. They're not perfect. Sometimes you go
a teensy bit down or a teensy bit up in the good cases. But the point is, it moves. It's not like
this person is going to break up with me and I'm stuck there forever. It has to move.
Let's talk about coming down from the highs. People do, at least a lot of us, walk around
basically shooting for the 10 out of 10. Like, why can't I be a 10 out of 10 every day? Is there
something we can say about the adaptiveness of not living life at the extreme end of like everything is great?
is such that you're reckless, but you also don't want to be cowardly. And so there's something to be said for this with happiness too. Happiness is going to be elusive if you're constantly
analyzing, do I have it yet? Do I have it yet? Do I have it yet? We really want to get to a point
where we're feeling grateful, noticing the good stuff in our lives, doing everything we can to
savor what we have, but pushing, pushing, pushing might not necessarily be the best thing for your
happiness anyway. I have long pondered this Aristotle golden
mean idea in part because the things I study like self-control or grit, people always ask,
can you be too self-controlled? What if you're too gritty? What's the dark side of excessive grit?
And when I think about what Aristotle is saying, I'm a little confused. What is the deeper reason why something in between the extremes is as a rule better, not just in the case of bravery and cowardice, but as a general truth about human nature? And I wonder whether there is some cost to being at the 10 out of 10, which makes us not want to be there all the time? Well, I think one is if you were always at a 10 out of 10 and you never change,
you wouldn't notice any change. And I think this actually gets to Amelia's question. She's asking,
like, why the need for change? The need for change is that we don't notice our absolute
objective status. We only notice when we change from it. People who live in Southern California
don't appreciate the weather because it's just perfect all the time. But when you live in the
Northeast, you get enough sucky days that all of a sudden when
it's sunny out, you're like, oh my gosh, it's sunny and 80.
Thank you.
You know, whatever divinity you're praying to.
The other thing is, I think you're totally right on the cost.
Sometimes if you're pushing happiness too much, that can be costly.
And I think we see that in the context of clinical disorders like mania.
Those people would report, I'm 10 out of 10 on a happiness scale, but they're gambling and wrecking their car and hurting their family and things like that. And so I think that
Aristotle might not have been perfect with the middle road, but he was on to something. And
the something I think he was on to that's most relevant to Amelia gets back to this idea of
the power of change. When we're just consistently good, we kind of don't notice it. The consequence
of that is what Danny Kahnman and Amos Tversky
referred to as diminishing sensitivity. We can get small changes that objectively feel good,
but we just don't notice them, which is sort of sad. The example I give my students is I try to
be hip, right? Like I try to know what a crossover is and know all the new songs. And there's this
DJ Khaled song called All I Do Is Win. Do you know
this song? No, of course I don't. Tell me. It's like, all I do is win, win, win, no matter what.
The idea is he just wins all the time. And I tell my students that is like a crappy way to live a
life because if you're literally winning all the time, you don't actually notice the subtle changes.
What is the optimal design then of a good life? Maybe it's just 99 great days and one really bad one to make you appreciate the rest of the 99. What do you think? because another feature of this diminishing sensitivity, I mentioned Danny Kahneman and Amos Dversky, it comes from their famous idea about prospect theory, which is this idea that
we don't evaluate prospects or things in our lives in terms of absolute values. We recognize them and
represent them in terms of changes. You know, if I was like, Angela, right now, the Happiness Lab
is going to give you $1 million, you'd be like, that's amazing. But if I was like, right now,
the Happiness Lab is going to give you $2 million. I mean, that's better, but you're not like twice as happy. And so that is diminishing
sensitivity. And that sucks. It means for you to get that extra happiness benefit from the extra
million in the 2 million, you would have wanted to go back to baseline first. So it feels like
two separate gains instead of one big gain. That's another reason not to obsess about being a 10.
Exactly. You think
going from 9 to 10 is going to be just as good as going from 8 to 9 or 7 to 8. But according to
diminishing returns, like it's better, not much better as it was to go from 7 to 8. That gives us
some hints about how to do it better. So one is split your gains. You don't want 2 million at
once. You want 1 million and then come back a couple months later and get another million. This is something I
actually try to do. How? How do you do it? Sometimes my husband and I will have a date
night and we're like, all right, we're going to see the movie we really wanted and get the dinner
we really wanted and get ice cream too. And it's like, wait, let's do the nice dinner and then do
the ice cream tomorrow. A really stupid way I do this is sometimes when I'm buying stuff,
this is not very ecologically savvy. So maybe this is not helping with my climate change goals,
but you know what it's like. You get the package of a bunch of stuff that you bought on Amazon.
It's not as fun as if you got the shirt one day and then the next day you got the shoes and you're
like, oh yeah. So you split your gains. I have a proposal that may or may not have as severe
consequences getting two Amazon packages. You know, vacations are
something that I don't know how to take very well. But according to the principles we've been
discussing, rather than taking seven days off and cramming in all of your dinners out and your
extra desserts and your walks around whatever city you're in with iced coffee, that would be my
preferred vacation. Why don't we have seven three-day weekends? I think that
that could result in a massive global gain in happiness without any obvious downside.
I love it. My other tip on this, I don't know if you like Hostess cupcakes. I'm from Philly. We
have Tasty Cakes. Oh, I think Tasty Cakes are similar. But the key to the Hostess cupcake is
that you get two of them. Like Hostess could have made that much chocolate cakiness in a single big cupcake. But if you got that cupcake, you'd just
plow through it. They had the insight to break those up. And like what happens is you eat the
first one, you wait, and then you come back to it. You're kind of at baseline again. You get more
happiness. Wow. You think that the hostess people really had behavioral science down?
get more happiness. Wow. You think that the hostess people really had behavioral science down? They read prospect theory. They're like, wait a minute. This is why people like the mini
black and white cookies better than the one huge black and white cookie. You can pause in between
them and you go back to happiness baseline, no cookie. And then you're like, oh my gosh,
another cookie. And then spike back up. I don't know how many people, by the way,
eat the one hostess or Tasty Cake. In
the Tasty Cake version, there's no white swirly line across the top, but it's basically the same
cupcake. But there's two, right? There is two. And I do think spacing out our gains could be
helpful, just as you recommend. And maybe just reframing the inevitable bad days as like, I mean,
here's a trivial example. Last night I made cashew, you know, the buckwheat
thing. Oh, yeah. I followed the recipe to the letter because I had a friend. She's like,
I'm going to call my grandmother. We're going to get this exactly right. And then I left the
pot on the stove, not even thinking. And it just burned to a crisp and it was horrible and both
mushy and burned at the same time, which I didn't think was physically possible. So that was a bad experience. I grieved a bit, but maybe if I
reframe that as hooray for the burned cashew, now it'll make the next non-burnt batch all that much
more delicious and appreciated. Totally. And in fact, this gets back to a different form of ancient
wisdom. This was exactly the strategy that the Stoics had. So the Stoics thought you should
every morning do what they called negative visualization. You wake up and you say,
my kasha is going to get burned. My husband's going to leave me. I'm going to lose my job.
I'm going to trip and break my leg. You don't ruminate on that forever, but you do that as
little kind of five, 10 minute meditation. And you go to your day and you're like, oh my gosh,
my kasha didn't burn today. So the Stoics were really into this idea that you don't necessarily
have to have the change to notice the change. You could just imagine the change. And it gives you a
lot of gratitude for the stuff you have. One technique I use in some of my talks is I look
out in the audience and I say, all of you people who have kids, imagine whatever the last time you saw them was, that was the last time. It's over. You're
never going to see them again. And the idea is the next time you hug your kids, you're going to hug
them much more tightly. You didn't have to have a horrible thing happen to them. The reference
point didn't have to change in a bad way for you to get the appreciation. I had a shudder. I just
had to say, Lori, that was rough. But now you're going to be so nice to your kids today. Even if they're annoying, you'd be like, but I'm so happy they're alive.
You're like, thank the Lord that you're here. Okay. You have given us one thing you could do.
You could wake up and think of three bad things and they are just imaginary. And then the rest
of your day is going to go better. But I recall the study that you and I did wake up and think
of three good things, right? The classic gratitude exercise. These are opposite recommendations. So should people wake up and think of three good things or should they wake up and think of three bad things? I'm going to vote for the three good things. thought about the kasha. Thank God the house didn't burn down because I did discover the pot
of burning buckwheat in time to prevent a fire. Yay. And then I thought of a couple of other
things. My daughter got home safely. I really love this collaborator. And built in is a contrast
to the counterfactual, like my collaborator could be a jerk, but they're not. And my daughter
could have not gotten home safely. So maybe the
Stoics had a good idea, but I think it's improved upon by this much more positive experience of
thinking about three good things. To be fair, I think that's what the Stoics mean. They don't
mean like, oh, my God, my house is going to burn down. They think you should do that because
immediately afterwards, you're going to think about the positive thing, too. You're going to
be grateful for your kids leaving the stuff on the floor because you had that moment of thinking about
what it could be like to not have kids at all. I think naturally in the way the Stoics are talking
about them, they focus on the negative side, but they're hoping you're going to get to the
blessings really fast. And I think the negative side is important when you're feeling really down.
Like the example of breaking your leg, because I'm clumsy, this actually happens to me with
reasonable frequency. Like I recently broke my knee.
You literally mean this happens to you with frequency?
You injure yourself in a serious way?
Yeah, this was the second time I'd broken the same kneecap when I fell on it.
Oh, my God, Lori.
That's terrible.
Yeah, I was like, woe is me.
I broke my kneecap.
This sucks.
And then I actually went back to the Stoics because I knew I needed hardcore people who were going to help me with this. And I read a book by this
current practicing Stoic, Bill Irvine, and he went through like, let's talk about some cases
that you could have. He's like, you could be a shut in. These are people who have some sort of
accident happen who are fully conscious, but so paralyzed that they can't move any part of their
body. They have to like blink an eye to communicate with people. And I was like, okay, well, at least I don't have that. But sometimes
if you get the right negative visualization, you're like, wait a minute, I can actually be
grateful for the broken knee too, because at least it's not X, Y, and Z. And I think this is a nice
way to solve Amelia's problem. You don't necessarily have to get the change from your real actions.
You can make your current reference points seem good just through these
imaginations. Do you think that would change Amelia's set point? Do you think that if she
chronically were comparing her pretty awesome life, she says she has a lovely department,
she's doing really well. If she regularly did these mental counterfactuals that she would be enduringly happier?
You know, if every morning she could have the counterfactual of like, what if I didn't have
this lovely, supportive job with my interesting colleagues, as she mentions? What if my colleagues
suck? That bumps up the appreciation you have. It breaks your hedonic adaptation. So I actually do
think it would be kind of a nice strategy. I think we need Amelia to agree to be a pilot subject in a study with only one subject. So, Laurie, you want Amelia
to wake up every day for a week and what, think of three bad things? Of the things she loves about
her life and her job, imagine that those weren't there. I would propose the second week be that
she tried the three good things exercise.
And after a month, we could all get together and find out which week was better. I want a third
condition where she does both, where she imagines the bad thing and then thinks, oh my gosh, I am
so lucky to have these colleagues. Because I think if you just do bad, then it could be ruminating.
And to be fair to the Stoics, that's not really what they meant. Okay, now we need six weeks of
your life, Amelia, right?
Love it. To be continued.
The Happiness Lab is co-written and produced by Ryan Dilley.
Our original music was composed by Zachary Silver, with additional scoring, mixing, and mastering by Evan Viola.
The Happiness Lab is brought to you by Pushkin Industries and me, Dr. Laurie Santos.
No Stupid Questions is part of the Freakonomics Radio Network, which also includes Freakonomics
Radio, People I Mostly Admire, and Freakonomics MD. This episode was produced by me, Rebecca Lee
Douglas. For our Happiness Lab listeners who are new to No Stupid Questions, this is the time in the show where I do a quick fact check of the conversations.
Early on in the episode, Lori and Angela say that they're horrified by the idea of Heinz Oreo mayonnaise.
I'm sure they will be thrilled to hear that this is not, in fact, a real product.
In June of 2021, the Instagram account Dr. Photograph posted a convincing photo of
May Oreo sauce that immediately went viral, but the image was later proven to be altered.
For those who are disappointed that this crossover product doesn't actually exist,
fret not. Plenty of other Heinz mashups are actually real. The company now produces Mayo Chup, a combination of mayonnaise and ketchup,
Mayo Must, a mix of mayonnaise and mustard, and Crunch, a blend of ketchup and ranch, among others.
Later, Angela and Lori discuss recently deceased psychologist Albert Bandora's seminal Bobo doll
experiment. I was unfamiliar with the concept of a Bobo doll, and I surmised
that many listeners would be as well. I found that the toy isn't really a doll at all, but rather a
large, inflatable, plastic clown with a heavy, rounded bottom. When it's pushed over, the clown
temporarily wobbles but quickly bounces back to center, making it a perfect toy for children and adults to beat up in Bendora's experiments.
Finally, Angela says that Tasty Cakes, like Hostess Cupcakes, come two to a package.
Standard Tasty Cake boxes do include six packs of two cupcakes, but you can also opt for
a single package of three cupcakes.
Either way, if you have the self-restraint, you can still enjoy the happiness that comes
with more dessert later that day.
That's it for the Fact Check.
No Stupid Questions is produced by Freakonomics Radio and Stitcher.
Our staff includes Allison Craiglow, Greg Rippin, James Foster, Joel Meyer, Trisha Bobita, Emma Terrell, Lyric Bowditch, Jasmine Klinger, and Jacob Clemente.