The Knowledge Project with Shane Parrish - Daniel Kahneman: Algorithms Make Better Decisions Than You

Episode Date: July 22, 2025

Daniel Kahneman won the Nobel Prize for proving we're not as rational as we think. In this timeless conversation we discuss how to think clearly in a world full of noise, the invisible forces that c...loud our judgement, and why more information doesn't equal better thinking. Kahneman also reveals the mental model he discovered at 22 that still guides elite teams today.  Approximate timestamps:  (00:36) – Episode Introduction   (05:37) – Daniel Kahneman on Childhood and Early Psychology   (12:44) – Influences and Career Path   (15:32) – Working with Amos Tversky   (17:20) – Happiness vs. Life Satisfaction   (21:04) – Changing Behavior: Myths and Realities   (24:38) – Psychological Forces Behind Behavior   (28:02) – Understanding Motivation and Situational Forces   (30:45) – Situational Awareness and Clear Thinking   (34:11) – Intuition, Judgment, and Algorithms   (39:33) – Improving Decision-Making with Structured Processes   (43:26) – Organizational Thinking and Dissent   (46:00) – Judgment Quality and Biases   (50:12) – Teaching Negotiation Through Understanding   (52:14) – Procedures That Elevate Group Thinking   (55:30) – Recording and Reviewing Decisions   (57:58) – The Concept of Noise in Decision-Making   (01:01:14) – Reducing Noise and Improving Accuracy   (01:04:09) – Replication Crisis and Changing Beliefs   (01:08:21) – Why Psychologists Overestimate Their Hypotheses   (01:12:20) – Closing Thoughts and Gratitude Thanks to MINT MOBILE for sponsoring this episode: Get this new customer offer and your 3-month Unlimited wireless plan for just 15 bucks a month at MINTMOBILE.com/KNOWLEDGEPROJECT. Newsletter - The Brain Food newsletter delivers actionable insights and thoughtful ideas every Sunday. It takes 5 minutes to read, and it’s completely free. Learn more and sign up at ⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠fs.blog/newsletter⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠ Upgrade — If you want to hear my thoughts and reflections at the end of the episode, join our membership: ⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠fs.blog/membership⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠ and get your own private feed. Watch on YouTube: ⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠⁠@tkppodcast Photograph: Richard Saker/The Guardian Learn more about your ad choices. Visit megaphone.fm/adchoices

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Delay your intuition. Don't try to form an intuition quickly, which is what we normally do. Focus on the separate points and then when you have the whole profile, then you can have an intuition and it's going to be better. Welcome to the Knowledge Project. I'm your host Shane Parrish. In a world where knowledge is power, this podcast is your toolkit for mastering the best for what other people have already figured out. So you can use their insights in your life. Before we get into the interview, I want to tell you about a moment that didn't make it into the episode. I first came across Daniel Kahnman's work in the early 2000s. His impact on me and so many people around the
Starting point is 00:00:49 globe has been unbelievable. By the time I sat down with him in his New York City home in 2019, I had so many questions for him. Condon won a Nobel Prize in Economic Sciences in 2002, yet he never took an economics course. His central message was very simple. If we want to make better decisions, we need help. Danny died last year on March 27, 2024. He was 90.
Starting point is 00:01:14 This conversation is now one of the final opportunities to hear directly from one of the most influential thinkers of our time. I get messages about this episode every week. People come away with new insights on everything from life to decision making. I re-listened to it recently, and it's timeless. That's exactly why I'm republishing it. Consider loss aversion, one of his most important discoveries. Why does losing $100 hurt twice as much as gaining $100 feels good?
Starting point is 00:01:45 The asymmetry affects everything. It affects your stock portfolio, your golf game. Check your portfolio when it's down, and you'll start making emotional decisions. A golfer putts better for power than for Bertie. But here's what happened near the end of our interview. Danny's phone rang, and it was loud. He'd forget to turn it off. And we're almost done the interview at this point, but he answered.
Starting point is 00:02:07 And someone obviously wanted him to give a talk or review a book. He ended the call with words that have stayed with me since then. My rule is I never say yes on the phone. I'll get back to you tomorrow. I wanted to discuss that on air, but we ran out of time. As I packed up my gear, I asked him, about that. This rule was a trick to avoid saying yes intuitively. It gave him time to think. He's always bombarded with requests, and he often says yes when he didn't want to. At first, he would try
Starting point is 00:02:36 saying no, that date doesn't work, that timeline doesn't work, but what happened in those moments was it turned into a negotiation. What about another date, another timeline? So he hit on this rule, and to me, this is his most practical discovery. Most people don't even know about it. This rule lets you reprogram your unconscious mind. Your desired behavior becomes your default behavior. And that's incredibly powerful. It changed my life. I now exercise every day. It's actually easier than three times a week. The activity, duration, and scope can change, but working out and exercising doesn't. I think I've missed five days in five years at this point. And I talk about this in my book, Clear Thinking, and the concept has changed so many lives, including my great friend, Brent Bishore.
Starting point is 00:03:21 in episode 196, we talk about this a little. Several parts of this conversation stuck out when I was re-listing to it. First, we talk about happiness versus satisfaction. Happiness is feelings. It's mostly social. Am I with the people who love me and whom I love back? Satisfaction, on the other hand, is how you feel about your life, your job, your career, conventional aspects.
Starting point is 00:03:45 Danny argued people want satisfaction more than happiness. Second, changing behavior. Make good behavior easier and bad behavior harder. The insight, all behavior is equilibrium. Rather than pushing people to change, ask why they aren't doing it already. Third, behavior is situational. Want to understand behavior? Look at the situation.
Starting point is 00:04:09 When someone acts in ways that don't make sense, ask yourself, what would the world have to look like for that behavior to make sense? Fourth, agents making decisions on your behalf beat you, at certain types of decisions. They have no sunk cost. They have no emotions. Brian Johnson talks about this in episode 188. He turned his health decisions over to effectively an algorithm because that algorithm
Starting point is 00:04:36 makes better decisions than he does. Fifth, our beliefs are formed by people more than facts. We agree with people we like, despite the facts. It's easier to believe a lie from someone you like than a truth from someone you dislike. We form identity beliefs, liberal, conservative, Democrat, Republican, they can do no wrong. If they're wrong, we're wrong, and we can't handle that. Finally, intuition. Danny had talked about this so much, his answers sounded repetitive.
Starting point is 00:05:05 So I framed my question on this to include his typical answer in the question, forcing him to think a little deeper. Whether this is your first listen or your third, you'll come away with ideas that you can use in life. Daniel, I'm so happy to give that a chance to talk to you. Well, I'm happy to have you here. What was your childhood like? What were you like as a child? Oh, my God.
Starting point is 00:05:49 That was a long time ago. I was an early child, as you might expect, I suppose. I thought I'd be a professor when I was like three or four years old because people told me it would be because they probably spoke with long words and stuff like that. And then the rest of my childhood, I mean, I was five when World War II began. And I was a Jew in France. I've had a difficult childhood, but from that point on,
Starting point is 00:06:24 but what I was, was I like, yeah, I was a, I was a nerdy child. I was quite inept physically, very fortunately for me when I finally moved to Israel at age 12, they held me up a grade and then, and that was all right, but that's, that's what I was like. If there are any particular lessons or memories that stand out for you? There are two of them that I speak about. So one is that I was a psychologist very early on that was very clear. I wrote an essay, before I was 11, I remember where, because it was a German counterattack. It was during that period we were in Paris.
Starting point is 00:07:09 And I wrote an essay about faith and religion, and it was a very pompous essay. I had a little book that was titled what I write about what I think, something pompous like that. But the essay started with another pompous thing that I was quoted Pascal. My sister had passed her exams and I had read,
Starting point is 00:07:36 no, she studied some Pascal and I had read it. And Pascal had said that Faith is God made sensible to the heart. And, you know, little me, I said, how true. That's my essay said. And then, but then I said, but faith is really hard to get. You don't say it's God all the time. So that's what religious pomp is for.
Starting point is 00:08:07 Cathedrals, organ music, they give you, and I call that Ozat's face. sort of substitute faith because it's a it's a similar feeling it's got to do with god and that's what you must do with that's that's a psychologist so it's clear that's you know that was my calling man so that's one significant memory of my childhood so you wanted to be a psychologist i think so i think so i mean i you know it's always had that that point of view that later as a teenager I was, you know, interested in all the philosophical issues like, you know, does God exist and good and bad and stuff like that? And why shouldn't we masturbate, you know, serious questions? But I discovered that actually I was less interested
Starting point is 00:09:05 in the question of whether or not God exists, then in why do people believe that he exists? that I thought was interested and I wasn't particularly interested in the question of what's good or bad but I was really interested in what makes people angry and indignant so I've had the psychological point of view since
Starting point is 00:09:25 terms of since my childhood was there anybody that sort of influenced you to go on to study this I mean it's one thing to have these dreams as like a 12, 13 14 year old boy it's another to turn this into you know probably the most
Starting point is 00:09:40 eminent career that's ever happened for a psychologist? No, I'm not the most of a career. You know, and I wasn't sure, actually, that I would do psychology. When I took a vocational exam to tell me what I was good at, and psychology and economics turned out, but, you know, that was unexpected. I was, and then I took psychology as an undergraduate and mathematics, at which I was not particularly good, so. And, no, it's not.
Starting point is 00:10:10 that I knew at the time that, you know, I had that calling to be a psychologist. It didn't occur to me. I thought, you know, I thought I'd be a professor in one thing or another. I mean, I thought I'd be an academic, but not psychology-specific. You worked with Amos Tversky for a long time. Are there any particular stories that you remember about working with him that bring a smile to your face? Almost everything about working with him brings a smile to my face.
Starting point is 00:10:40 You know, he was a very unusual person. Most people who knew them thought that he was the smartest person that ever met. And in fact, the famous psychologist, Nick Nesbett said that it's sort of an intelligence test. When you said that when you are with Amos, how long does it take you to figure out that he's smarter than you are? And the faster you figure that out, the smarter you are. So, yeah, he was super bright and very, very funny. He joked a lot. He laughed a lot at his own jokes, and that was infectious.
Starting point is 00:11:18 When I was with him, I was very funny, too. More than half of my life, with my lifetime of that during the 10 years I worked with him. You know what doesn't belong in your summer plans? Getting burned by your old wireless bill. While you're locking in itineraries, your wireless bill should be. the last thing holding you back. That's why I recommend the switch to Mint Mobile. No gimmicks, just a smarter play at most people overlook. You make smart trade-offs everywhere else in life, why not here? All plans come with a high-speed data and unlimited talk and text
Starting point is 00:11:53 delivered on the nation's largest 5G network. Use your own phone with any Mint Mobile plan and bring your phone number along with all your existing contacts. It's a small switch that compounds over time, just like any great decision. Most people leave money on the table. This is how you stop doing that. If I had needed this product, it's what I'd use. The savings are unbeatable. This year, skip breaking a sweat and breaking the bank.
Starting point is 00:12:18 Get this new customer offer and your three-month unlimited wireless plan for 15 bucks a month at mintmobile.com slash knowledge project. That's mintmobile.com slash knowledge project. Up front payment of $45 required, equivalent to $15 a month. Limited time, new customer offer for first three months only. Speeds may vary about 35 gigabytes on unlimited plan, taxes and fees extra. See Mint Mobile for details. You have an interesting distinction between happiness and satisfaction.
Starting point is 00:12:47 Can you walk us through that? Yeah, sure. I mean, the word happiness is so ambiguous and it means so many things to many people. But one sensible interpretation of it is that it's got. to do with your emotions, with how you feel with the emotional tone of your life, whether it's a happy life, you know, it's pleasant to be you. Life satisfaction is a completely different thing. I mean, life satisfaction is how you feel about your life when you think about your life. And most of the time you don't think about your life, you just live. But, you know, some triangle
Starting point is 00:13:26 sort of look. And that's when you determine how satisfied you are. That's, that's when you determine how satisfied you are. That's life satisfaction. It's not satisfaction. It's life satisfaction. Should we balance the two? How would you think about them? Should we be more happy when we're younger, more satisfied when we're older?
Starting point is 00:13:45 That thought that never occurred to me. When I began to work on those, I started out thinking that happiness in that sense of how you feel when you live, that was reality and that life satisfaction was just stories that people, people tell themselves and the important thing was to be happy in real time. But later, when we did more research, it turned out that the circumstances that make
Starting point is 00:14:15 people happy and the circumstances that make them satisfied with their life are not the same. So happiness is mostly social. It's, you know, it's being with people you love and who love you back. That's a lot of what happiness is. Life satisfaction is much more conventional, it's to be successful. And, you know, so it's money, education, prestige, that sort of thing is what satisfaction is about. So those are two very different things.
Starting point is 00:14:46 I thought that life satisfaction is irrelevant. You know, that's how I began. And we had a research program where we were trying to, you know, to show that this is the case. But then, after a few years, I realized that people really want in their life is they don't seem to care about how happy they'll be. They seem to want to be satisfied with their life. They seem to want to have a good story about their life. And then I was in the position of saying that to define well-being in a way that people didn't seem to care particularly about. So that was not a tenable position.
Starting point is 00:15:26 So I dropped back into saying that I had no idea how to deal with it. Was this the result of the research? You did some research that was, I think it said above 70,000, you don't become happier. But do you become more satisfied? No. The research I did with Angus Deaton at Princeton, famous economists, we showed that in terms of happiness, in terms of emotional tone, positive and negative, Having a lot of money doesn't make you happier,
Starting point is 00:15:59 but being poor makes you miserable. So that's above the threshold that was like $70,000 approximately in the U.S. Then extra money didn't make you emotionally happier. But with life satisfaction, it was a different story. With life satisfaction, that doesn't satiate, so it's always good to have more. But because basically, I think, money is a proxy for success
Starting point is 00:16:27 and it's a proxy for subjective success in many cases. So it's not necessarily but spending it or doing something with this. It's just a measure. Just getting it. I mean, you know, you look at all those people, all those billionaires working their heads off, and they're clearly not doing this because they need more money. They're trying to get more money.
Starting point is 00:16:47 And they're trying to get more money because that will be an indication that they're good at what they do. I think mostly it's approximately. Do either of those variables correlate to longer living, happiness or satisfaction? Both, apparently. But, you know, it's hard to separate. And I haven't been followed. You know, shortly after deciding that I didn't know what well-being was,
Starting point is 00:17:11 I sort of stopped doing research on this, so I haven't been following. But I think there's clear evidence that being effectively happened. be, you know, is very good for you. And you do live longer and you make better and so on. And life satisfaction works in the same direction. Whether it's separable, which of them, you know, is it more important that I don't. I want to switch gears a little bit and talk about behavior. And I'd love your insider expansion upon the idea of we can change behavior and how do we
Starting point is 00:17:47 go about changing our behavior. Well, you know, I'm not sure I buy the premise. I think changing behavior is extremely difficult. There are a few tips on, you know, a few guidelines about how to do that, but anybody who is very optimistic about changing behavior is just deluded. It's hard to change other people's behavior. It's very hard to change your own. Not simple.
Starting point is 00:18:12 This is what marriage is all about, right? Among other things, you know, people, when, you know, married people are tried to change each other's behavior. It's a lot of dissatisfied. They are not on their way to a good marriage, I think. We'd all be happier with lower expectations. Yes, I mean, and even if you have
Starting point is 00:18:30 expectation, don't try to change because, you know, it's very unlikely to work in a significant way. I can think of the common ways that we would sort of go about behavior change and it would be, you know, making good behaviors more easy or negative behaviors harder.
Starting point is 00:18:48 Then that's the main insight, you know, when you want to influence somebody's behavior, that's a very big insight. I've always thought that this is the best psychological idea ever, you know, so far as I'm going to say. But it's that when you want somebody to move from A to B in terms of their behavior, you can think of it that there are two ways of doing it. You can push them. Or you can ask the question, why aren't they doing it? B already, which is an unusual question, but you know, why? So then when you ask why not, why aren't they doing B, they ought to, as I think they ought to, then you get a list of what's both Lewin, that's the psychologist who, my guru
Starting point is 00:19:38 and many people's hero, he spoke of restraining forces. I mean, so there are reasons why they're not where you want them to be. So he spoke of behavior as an equilibrium. There are forces that are pushing you one way, forces that are pushing you the other way. So how loud you speak, how fast you drive. It's easy to think of it as an equilibrium. And what we tend to do when we want to move people from A to B is we push them. We add to the driving forces.
Starting point is 00:20:13 And Kutluen's insight was that this is not what you should do. You should actually work on the restraining forces and try to make them weaker. And that's a beautiful point. And he showed, he had that image that, you know, I've had since I was an undergraduate. And I'm not sure, actually, whether it was his image or something that I drew from reading him. But it's like you have a plank and it's being held by two sets of springs. You know, you wanted to move one direction and so you could add another spring that would push it that way or could remove one of the springs that are holding it back. And the interesting thing, and that's the striking outcome, is when it moves, if it moves because of the driving force, you've added to the driving force, then at equilibrium it will be in a higher state of tension than it was originally.
Starting point is 00:21:11 that is because you've compressed when spring and such pushing back harder. But if you remove a restraining force at equilibrium, there'll be less tension on the system. I must have been 20 years old. I thought that's just so beautiful. What do you wish that everybody knew about psychology that you don't think that they do? If that was class one, what's class two? You know, class two, which is a development from class one. You know, it's the same idea extended.
Starting point is 00:21:42 Class two is that behaviors don't necessarily reflect the personality, but behaviors have a lot to do with the situation. And so if people behave in strange ways, look at the situation they're in, and what are the pressures in the situation that make them act as well? So there is a bias that a social psychologist, well-known social psychologist, called the fundamental attribution error.
Starting point is 00:22:10 And that means that when you see people acting in some way, you think that it's because of their personality that they do it. That may not be the case. It's quite likely that the situation is making them do it. I'd like people to know that motivation is complex and that people do good things for a mixture of good and bad reasons and they do bad things for a mixture of good and bad reasons. And I think that there is a point to educating people in psychology
Starting point is 00:22:40 is to make them less judgmental. Just have more empathy and more patience and being judgmental doesn't get you anywhere. When you talk about a situation, one of the things that comes to mind is it's so easy for us to give our friends advice. But if we were in that situation, we might not necessarily see it.
Starting point is 00:23:00 Why is that the case? Why is it so much easier to give other people advice? I mean, feelings get in the way of clear thinking. There is a phenomenon that we call the endowment effect, which is that when I'd ask for more money to sell you my sandwich than I'd pay to get it, I mean, that's essentially the endowment effect. And our explanation of it, there are many explanations, but a story I like to tell about it is that it's more painful to give something up
Starting point is 00:23:29 than to get something. But there is an interesting result that if you have an agent, making decisions on somebody's behalf, that agent doesn't have loss of those. So that agent sells and buys at the same price, which is the economically rational thing to do. Where this goes into policy and governments and really important things, that governments are like agents or people who think about the good of society. And agents, they take the economic view. They take the view of what things will be like at the end.
Starting point is 00:24:01 They don't figure out that there are some people who are going to be. be losing because of the reform that they make. And it turns out that you can really expect losers, potential losers, to fight a lot harder than potential winners. And that's the reason that reforms are frequently fair and that when they succeed, they're almost always way more expensive than anticipated. And they're more expensive because you have to compensate the losers. and that frequently is not anticipated. So that's an example of a story about that incorporates behavior change
Starting point is 00:24:39 and the difference between perspective, between being, you know, in the situation, feeling the pain of giving up the sandwich and not feeling the pain of giving up the sandwich. That would have huge public policy sort of implications, too, right, that we don't tend to think about or discuss. That's a really interesting angle there. I want to come back to sort of situational decision making based on sort of like what we see is all there is and we have these feelings
Starting point is 00:25:08 that we can't sort of disassociate with. How does environment play a role, like the physical environment, in sort of what we decide or does it? I mean, you know, there are sort of obvious thing that we know. People are hot and bothered and distracted and, and, and there is a lot of noise and so on, and they'll think less well. And that we know, that's... But even there, there are puzzles. I mean, many people think and work a lot better in cafes,
Starting point is 00:25:40 you know, where there is actually ambient noise and activity around them, and it helps them concentrate better. So there isn't a very simple story of the environment. But certainly, you can make the environment tough enough so that people won't be able to think properly. That's feasible. Are the things that we could do to, I guess, push the environment to be more conducive to clearer thinking, the physical environment in this case?
Starting point is 00:26:09 Oh, there are all sorts of, you know, odd findings, you know, the color of the color of the room. Some colors are better than others. And you would expect that some colors are more calming than others. So you wouldn't want to be in a red room. Making decisions. making decisions, but those are extreme and minor effects. I want to come to intuition and noise later. Is there anything else that stands out that gets in the way of clear thinking
Starting point is 00:26:40 that we can sort of bring to the surface now? Well, you know, what gets in the way of clear thinking is that we have intuitive views of almost everything. So as soon as you present a problem to me, I have, you know, has some ready-made answer. And what gets in the way of clear thinking those ready-made answers, and we can't help but have them.
Starting point is 00:27:04 So that's one thing that gets in the way. Emotions get in the way. And I would say that independent, clear thinking, is to a first approximation impossible? I mean, in the sense that, you know, we believe in things most of the time, not because we have good reasons to believe them. If you ask me for reasons, I'll explain you.
Starting point is 00:27:28 I'll always find a reason. But the reasons are not the causes of our beliefs. We had beliefs because mostly we believe in some people and we trust them. And we adopt their beliefs. So we don't reach our beliefs by clear thinking, unless you're a scientist or doing something like that. But even then, it's probably a very narrow... But that's very narrow.
Starting point is 00:27:54 and there is a fair amount of emotion in your neuroscientists as well that gets in the way of clear thinking. You know, commitments to your previous views, being insulted that somebody thinks he's smarter than you. I mean, lots of things get in the way than the neuroscientist. So I'd say there is left clear thinking than people like to think.
Starting point is 00:28:16 Is there anything that we can do at the belief formation stage? Like it sounds almost as though when you say that we're reading a newspaper, we read this op-ed, and it's well-constructed and fits with our view of the world, therefore we adopt that opinion, and we forget the context that we didn't learn it through our own experience or reflection. We learned it sort of from somebody else, so we don't know when it's sort of likely to work or not work, but we just proffer that as our opinion, is there? That's how I believe in climate change. You know, I believe,
Starting point is 00:28:52 in the people who tell me there is climate change. And the people who don't believe in climate change, they believe in other people. But similarly, there's like fake news and all this other stuff that we would have the same reaction to you. You know, but I'm much more likely to believe fake news on my side than the fake news on the other side. I mean, it's true that there is a huge degradation
Starting point is 00:29:18 in public discourse. and in the recent 10, 15 years in the United States. I mean, there used to be an idea that facts matter. What would be your hypothesis as to why that is playing it? Were they getting into politics because I don't want to talk politics? But like, why is that? Well, I mean, it's hard to answer that question without politics because the general political polarization has had a very big effect.
Starting point is 00:29:49 and the fact that people can choose the sources of information. Let's switch gears a little bit and talk about intuition. I think one of the things that strikes me the most about some of the work that you've done is the cases where we're likely to trust our intuition and when we're not. And so if I'm, correct me if I'm getting this wrong, so it's sort of like a stable environment, repeated attempts and rapid feedback. It strikes me that most decisions made in organizations do not fit that environment. And yet, we're making a lot of these decisions on judgment or experience.
Starting point is 00:30:37 What are the ways that we can sort of make better decisions with that in the context? Well, in the first place, I think, you know, you shouldn't expect too much. And back to low expectations. I didn't think through, yeah, should have low expectations about improving decisions. I mean, there is, you know, one basic rule is slow down, especially if you have that immediate conviction, slow down. There are procedures, you know, there are ways of reaching better decisions, but reaching better deference and we can talk about them. I would love to hear. If you really want to improve the quality of decision making, use the way.
Starting point is 00:31:18 algorithms. I mean, wherever you can, if you can replace judgments by rules and algorithms, they'll do better. There's big social costs to trustee, allowing an algorithm to make decisions, but the decisions will likely it to be better. So that's one thing. If you can't use algorithms, then you slow yourself down. And then there are things that you can do for certain types of problems and there are different types of problems. So one class of problems are forecasting problems. A friend Phil Tetlock has that book on super forecasters where he identifies with people who are good at forecasting the future, what they do. That makes them good. And, you know, it tries to train people and they can improve people. So that's one class of problems. I'm interested specifically
Starting point is 00:32:15 in another kind of problem, judgment problems, where basically you're considering options or you're evaluating a situation and you're trying to give it a score. There is advice, I think, on how to do it. For me, it goes back to something I did in the Israeli army when I was like 22 years old. So that's a long time ago, like 63 years ago.
Starting point is 00:32:42 I was a psychologist in the Israeli owned. And I was assigned the job of setting up an interviewing system for the army. That's ridiculous, but, you know, this was the beginning of the state of Israel. So people were improvising all over the place. So I had a BA and I was, I think I were the best trained psychologist in the army. My boss was a chemist. Brilliant. But anyway, and the existing system was one where people would interview and try to
Starting point is 00:33:15 to form an intuitive, global image of how well that recruit would do as a combat soldier, which was the object of the interview. And because I had read a book at Paul Neal, I took a different talk. And the different tact was I identified six traits that I sort of made up, and I had them ask questions and evaluate each of these traits independently and score it and write down the score, them go on to the next trade. And they had to do it for all six traits. And that's all I asked them to do. And the interviewers, who were about one year younger than I, all recruits, but very,
Starting point is 00:33:58 very smart, selected for being good at it, they were furious with me. And they were furious with me because they wanted to exercise their intuition. And I still remember that one of them said, they're turning us into robots. So I compromised with them. And I said, okay, you do it my way. And I told them, you try to be reliable, not valid. You know, I'm in charge of validity. You'd be reliable, which was pretty arrogant,
Starting point is 00:34:26 but that's how I presented it. But then when you're done, close your eyes and just put down a number, how good a soldier is that guy who over to be? And when we validated the results of the interviewed, it was a big improvement on what had gone on before. But the other surprise was that the final intuitive judgments added it was good.
Starting point is 00:34:55 It was as good as the average of the six straight and not the same. It added in formation. So actually, we ended up with a score that was half, was determined by the specific ratings and the intuition got half the way and that by the way stayed in the Israeli army for where over 50 years I don't know whether it's I think probably some version of it
Starting point is 00:35:18 was still being forced but around 15 years ago a visit of my old base and the commanding officer of the research unit was telling me how they run the interview and then she said and then we tell them close your eyes. So that had stayed for 50 years. Now, the close your eyes and that whole idea is now the basis of the book that I'm writing. So actually, I have the same idea, really, that when you
Starting point is 00:35:50 are making decisions, you should think of options as if they were candidates. So you should break, it up into dimensions, evaluate each dimension separately, then look at the profile, and the key is delay your intuition. Don't fight to form an intuition quickly, which is what we normally do. Focus on the separate points. And then when you have the whole profile, then you can have an intuition and it's going to be better. Because people make form intuitions too quickly and the rapid intuitions are not potentially good. So if you delay intuition until you have more information, it's going to be better.
Starting point is 00:36:31 I'm curious how we delay intuition. You delay intuition by focusing on the separate problems. So our advice is that if you have, you know, the board of directors make your decisions about an investment, we tell them you do it that way. Take the separate dimensions and really think about each dimension separately and independently. And don't allow, you know, if you're the chair, don't allow people. to give their final judgment, say, we wait until we cover the whole thing.
Starting point is 00:37:08 I mean, if you find a deal breaker, then you stall. But if you haven't found a deer breaker, wait to the end and look at the profile, and then your decision is almost certainly going to be better. Does that include weighting the different aspects of the problem differently? Do you highlight that in advance? Yeah, I mean, it makes you see the trade-offs more clearly. Otherwise, when we don't follow that discipline, there is a way in which people form impressions, very quickly you form an impression, and then you spend most of your time confirming it instead of collecting evidence.
Starting point is 00:37:48 And so if accidentally your impression was in the wrong direction, you're going to confirm it and you don't give yourself a chance to correct it. Independence is the key because otherwise when you don't take those precautions, it's like having a bunch of witnesses to some crimes and allowing those witnesses to talk to each other. They're going to be less valuable if you're interested in the truth than keeping them rigidly separate and collecting what they have to say. What have you seen work in a repeatable way? It may be a particular organization or cross organizations to not only reliably surface disconfirming evidence,
Starting point is 00:38:37 but then place a value on what is surfaced instead of being dismissive. Is there a framework for that? Is there? Well, yeah, there are many, you know, there are many procedures like red team, blue team, a devil's advocate to mean this. There have been, you know, many attempts. In general, you know, if you are the head of the group that makes decisions,
Starting point is 00:39:02 one of your missions would be to protect the dissenters because they're very valuable and you should make it painless to be sent or as painless as possible because it's hard to be said. It's painful and costly. So protecting the centers is important. I'm curious about the distinction between intuition and judgment. You had mentioned intuition, judgment, intuitive judgment. Can you walk me through some of how those differ?
Starting point is 00:39:37 It's a bit hard to separate. And judgment is what you do when you integrate a lot of information informally into score. of some kind. We speak, we being my co-author Zunai in the book we're writing. We speak of judgment as measurements, but it's measurement where the measuring instrument is your mind. But you do it informally. And because you do it informally,
Starting point is 00:40:06 people are not necessarily going to agree. So wherever we say it's a matter for judgment, we're allowing for differences, for variability. Now, judgment can be more or less slow, more or less systematic. So at one end, you have pure intuition where you allow the judgment to go very quickly and so on. And at the other end, you try to delay intuition. But ultimately, if you're making it by judgment, you're going to have a judgment
Starting point is 00:40:37 and it's going to be like an intuition and you're going to go with it. So, the more or less deliberate judgment, intuition is always involved at one point or another. You're either sort of like listening to it or fending it off? Yeah. And our recommendation is fend it off. Are there ways to judge the quality of somebody's judgment? Oh, sure. I mean, some of them would be unique to the actual scenario, but what are the sort of other ways that we could?
Starting point is 00:41:08 Well, I mean, you may require people to explain. their judgments and evaluating the quality of the explanation is, you know, whether it's logical, whether it uses the evidence, whether it uses all the evidence,
Starting point is 00:41:24 whether it is strongly influenced by wishes, whether the conclusion was reached before the judgments supposedly is made. You know, there are lots of, there are lots of ways for judgment
Starting point is 00:41:40 to fail that can be recognized. So it's harder to recognize very good judgment, but it's fairly easy to see, you know, what goes wrong. And there are quite a few ways for judgment to go wrong. And I think some of those ways are the cognitive biases, like overconfidence and sort of using small or extrapolating from small sample sizes. And one of the interesting things that I've heard you say in interviews before, so correct me if I'm off here, is that you've studied cognitive biases effectively. your whole life and you're no better at avoiding them than anybody else. Yeah, certainly not much better, no. What hope do the rest of us have?
Starting point is 00:42:23 Not much. I mean, I never, you know, I think, you know, the quality of people's judgment is affected by education. But so in general, you know, more educated people, I have better judgments, I think, on average. but people decide I'm going to make better judgments. I don't think that's very hopeful. I'm much more hopeful about organizations
Starting point is 00:42:49 because organizations think more slowly and they have procedures for thinking and so you can control the procedures. Individual judgment is really hard to fix. Not impossible. One of the things that I see people do in response to cognitive biases and trying to account for them is to sort of make a list of them
Starting point is 00:43:12 almost like a checklist and then go through that checklist and explain or rationalize why those things don't apply in this situation. It also strikes me that the more intelligent you are, the more stories you'd be able to conjure up about why you're avoiding this. I really think that's not very hopeful
Starting point is 00:43:31 because there are so many biases and the biases work in different directions anyway. So sometimes, you can recognize a situation as one in which you're likely to be wrong in a particular way. So that's like illusions.
Starting point is 00:43:51 If you recognize a particular pattern as something that gives rise to a visual illusion, then you don't trust your eyes. You know, you do something else. And the same thing happens when you recognize this is a situation
Starting point is 00:44:06 where unlikely to make an error. So sometimes you can recognize the importance, for example, of what we've called an anchor. So you're going to negotiate a price with somebody. They start very high, and that has an effect. So you know or you should know that the person who moves first in a negotiation has an advantage. Because the first number changes everybody's gene.
Starting point is 00:44:40 what is considered plausible. So it moves things in that direction. That's a phenomenon. People can learn that. And they can learn to resist it. So when I was teaching negotiations, I would say somebody does that to you, comes up with the number that's absurd.
Starting point is 00:45:00 I would say, lose your temper, make a scene. Say, I will not start the conversation from that number. It's an absurd number. I don't want to. That's erase that number. So that's something that, you know, you can improve if you recognize it. I think people are aware of the fact
Starting point is 00:45:19 that you shouldn't make a decision about road safety within a short interval of the terrible accidents. You know, so you should allow things to settle down and cool down. There is a more subtle error and harder, harder to fix, but that the best prediction, the best guess, is always less extreme than your impression. Intuitive prediction is, as we say, not regressive. It doesn't recognize regression to the mean. But statistics is statistics.
Starting point is 00:45:55 In statistics, things are less extreme. Should I give you my favorite example of the bias? Yeah, please. I have been unable to think of a better one. But the story is about Julie, that's part of the story, that's her name. She is a graduating senior at university, and I'll tell you one fact about her, that she read fluently when she was four, but she's a GPA. And the interesting thing here is that everybody has a number.
Starting point is 00:46:26 As soon as I told you that thing, her number came to mind. Now, we know where that number came from. We really, that's one of the few things that I'm reasonably sure. I understand perfectly. And this is that when you hear she read fluently at age four, you get an impression of how smart she is, of how precocious she was at eight four. And you could put that in percentiles.
Starting point is 00:46:51 You know, where did that put her on a percentile for sort of aptitude, ability? And it's high. It's not, you know, if she had read fluently at age two and a half, it would be more extreme, but age four is pretty high. So say it the 90th percentile. And then the GPA that comes to your mind is around the 90th percentile in the distribution of GPA.
Starting point is 00:47:17 So you pick something, your prediction is as extreme as your impression. And it's idiotic, statistically, completely stupid, because clearly the age at which a child learned to read is not. all that diagnostic with respect to GPA. So it's better than nothing. If you didn't know anything, you would predict the mean GPA, whatever it had 3.1, 3.2.
Starting point is 00:47:46 Now, she's bright, so probably a little higher, but not 3.7. You don't want to. So that's called, that's a bias. That's non-regressive prediction. And that's very hard to resist. Sometimes I'm able to resist it, but never when it's important. You know, when I'm really involved in something, I don't think about it, but sometimes I will recommend, oh, you know, that's a situation.
Starting point is 00:48:17 I should moderate my prediction. And if you're conscious of it, that's an example of one you can sort of talk yourself out of? Yeah, you can talk yourself into. Although, you know, you usually will find a way to cheat. and end up with your intuition. It's remarkable, you know, when you've been in an academic life a long time, so you've been in many situations
Starting point is 00:48:44 where people discuss a job candidate. And absurdities of that kind are very common. So somebody, a job candidate gives a talk, and people evaluate the talk. And this is something happens, you know, at Berkeley when I was teaching there, that somebody gave a talk wasn't a very good talk. Stammed a bit.
Starting point is 00:49:08 Now, that person had teaching prizes, and yet what was said about him in the discussion, he can't teach. You know, we heard the talk. So that's a mistake. But the funny thing is you can point out to people that that's a mistake. They still don't want to hire him
Starting point is 00:49:27 because he gave a lousy talk. So it's hard to resist. it's interesting one of the I think one of the ways I probably got my job is using psychology in the interview which is asking why I was there and then reinforcing those beliefs throughout the inner feet
Starting point is 00:49:43 I want to come back just one second to the immediacy of sort of having a stimulus and then making a decision so we use the example of roads and a tragic accident happens and you're rethinking sort of policy
Starting point is 00:49:58 or laws around the roads how much of that do you think is social pressure? And I'm wondering if we could even extrapolate that a little more to, we're taught to answer questions on a test right away, right? So we see this question and we answer it. We're taught that we, or maybe it's reinforced, Todd is probably the wrong word, that politicians need to have an immediate response to, and even if they know the best thing to do is like, okay, like let this settle, take some time. It's society at large seems to demand it.
Starting point is 00:50:35 Like the environment is not conducive. I think it's pretty clear that people prefer leaders who are intuitive and were overconfident. Leaders who deliberate too much are viewed with suspicion. So I think Obama was at a certain disadvantage relative to George Bush. Because he was seen as more deliberate. Yeah, it was more deliberate. And then when you're very deliberate,
Starting point is 00:51:05 you look as if you don't know what you're doing. But when you act with confidence, so people want leaders who are intuitive, I think, by and large, provided they've been able to. I was just working my way back through some of these rabbit holes that we've gone down. You taught negotiations. I'm curious what would be in your sort of syllabus
Starting point is 00:51:26 for negotiating. that everybody should learn about negotiations when it comes to your work in psychology? Well, you know, that goes back to a theme that we started with, the essence of teaching negotiations, that negotiations is not about trying to convince the other guy. It's about trying to understand them. So again, it's slowing yourself down. It's not doing what comes naturally because trying to convince. them is a prime pressure.
Starting point is 00:52:00 Arguments, promises, and threats are always of applying pressure. And what you really want is understand, you know, what you can do to make it easy for them to move your way. Very non-intuitive, that's a surprising thing when you teach negotiation. It's not obvious. You know, we are taught to apply pressure. I mean, socialize that way.
Starting point is 00:52:24 You mentioned that there was procedures for thinking in organizations. Are there any that stand out in your mind that we could use to elevate thinking and if not elevate but give feedback on the quality of thinking to improve it? Well, I think one of the ideas that people like the most
Starting point is 00:52:43 is an idea by Gary Klein that he calls the pre-morton and that's a universal winner. People really like that idea. And this is that when you're about to make a decision, a group, not quite, because if you've made it, it's too late, but you're approaching you. And then you get people in a room, can be the people who are making the decision. And you said, suppose it's two years from now.
Starting point is 00:53:14 And we made the decision that we're contemplating. And it turned out to be a disaster. Now, you have a page in front of you, write the history of that disaster in bulletproof. That's the premortals. And it's beautiful as an idea. It's beautiful because when people are coming close to a decision, it becomes difficult to raise doubts or to raise questions. People who are flowing the group down when the groups is nearing a decision.
Starting point is 00:53:46 Our procedure is really, you know, she's annoying. You know, you want to get rid of them. And the premortem legitimizes that sort of dissent. and that sort of doubts, not only legitimizes it, you know, it rewards it. And so that's a very good idea. I don't, you know, I don't think that it's going to prevent people from making mistakes, big mistakes, but it could certainly, it will alert people to possible loopholes to things that they ought to do to make a safer decision.
Starting point is 00:54:20 So that's a good procedure. And there are many others. What comes to mind? What comes to mind is to make intelligence, in the collection of information, independent of the decision-makers' wishes, and you really want to protect the independence of the people collecting the evidence.
Starting point is 00:54:44 And I would add to a procedure that really people don't like, but if it were possible to implement it, I think, would be good. And that's that when you're going to be discussing a topic and it's known in advance on people in sense and material to think about the topic, that you may want them to write down the decision
Starting point is 00:55:10 they are in favor of before the discussion starts. That has many advantages. It's going to give you a broader diversity of points of view because people tend to converge very quickly in a group discussion and it forces people to be better prepared except people
Starting point is 00:55:32 don't want this so I don't know whether it's even possible to implement it but clearly if you could would be a good idea what are the reasons people don't want it it's too much work
Starting point is 00:55:46 right forces you to do a lot rather than the signaling you can sort of get away with yeah and then you know there's somebody who is going to prepare the case And so I glanced at the material and then, you know, so a lot of meetings are tremendous to think for wasted time. And improving the quality of meetings would be a big thing. Do you have any insights on how to do that?
Starting point is 00:56:13 Keeping them short. You know, I'm not a professional at fixing meetings. So I have a few ideas, but not an complete view. The question of structuring the meetings to be discussing topics one at a time, that I think is really useful. I'll give you an example. I mean, it's something that I suggested when I was consulting, but for some reason people didn't buy that suggestion. So when an investment is being discussed, say, by an investment firm, some staff people, if it's a big investment, staff people, will prepare a briefing book with chapters.
Starting point is 00:56:58 Now, our recommendation would be that the staff should end each chapter with a score. How does that chapter taken on its own, independently of anything else, affect the likely decision? And then you could structure the meeting that discuss of this and the meeting of the board, say, to discuss these scores, one at a time. That has the effect that I was talking about earlier, making the decision, making the judgments about the dimensions, we call them mediating assessments is a jargon tube.
Starting point is 00:57:36 The mediating assessments come first, and then you have a profile of them, and then you make a global judgment. And you can structure it. So if the staff has presented a score and you discuss in the board, do we accept their score. You're forcing people to have a look at the evidence. And think about why they would have to reject and then they feel like they have to construct an argument that might be less intuitive. That's it. So, you know, there are ways of doing this, but if you're going to be too rigid about it, it won't work either. I'm curious what other advice you gave as a consultant
Starting point is 00:58:19 that nobody followed. Oh, I mean, virtually all the advice they get, people long follow. I mean, you know, I think that's not, you shouldn't, you know, you're not going to be a consultant if you expect your advice to be taken. You have to give the best advice you can. What would be other examples of something you think could be widely applicable that you would have advised people and you just sort of like saw them drop the ball? Well, I mean, you know, I would advise people who make a lot of decisions to keep track their decisions and of how they turned out so that later you can come and and evaluate your procedures and see whether there is anything that is in common
Starting point is 00:59:06 with those decisions that turned out well and didn't not so well and so on people hate doing this why do you think people hate doing it oh because because retrospectively they may look foolish some of them or all of them or in particular the leader. So they really don't like keeping track. I mean, there are exceptions. Ray Dalio and his firm and where everything is explicitly. Bridgewater.
Starting point is 00:59:34 Yeah, Bridgewater. But in general, in my experience, I haven't consulted in Bridgewater, they don't mean me. But in general, when I suggested that, never went anywhere. What are the variables that you would recommend people keep track of? Like, what would your decision journal look like? Oh, I mean, my decision journal would be a mess. I don't, I'm not putting myself as an example. So obviously, the outcome, but you've got to do that post after?
Starting point is 01:00:05 Yeah, but no, no, you would want to say what were the main arguments pro and calm? What were the alternative that were considered? No, no, it doesn't have to be very detailed, but it should be. enough so that you can come later and debrief yourself. Should you have a calibration? Like what degree of confidence you are? That would be good. Then, you know, it would depend on something that you could evaluate late. It strikes me that decision journals and premortems are a way to identify people that are sort of perhaps suppressed by their manager, where you have somebody who's actually a better
Starting point is 01:00:49 better at exercising judgment than the person that is, you know, that they're working for, and this would be a pain-free sort of way to calibrate that score over time and identify the quality of judgment in a consistent way. Oh, yeah, I mean, that strikes me as worth a lot of money to an organization. Yeah, but also very costly. And you will see that certainly anything that threatens the leader is not going to be a adopted, and leaders may not want something that threatened their subordinates either. People are really very worried about embarrassment.
Starting point is 01:01:28 You're writing a book now on noise. Yeah. Tell me about noise and decision making. Can you explain the concept? Yeah. I can really explain it by saying what, you know, was the beginning of it, and which was a consulting assignment in an insurance company, where we, I have the idea of running.
Starting point is 01:01:49 attests to see whether people in a given role who are supposed to be interchangeable agree with each other. So, you know, when you come to an insurance company and an underwriter gives you a premium, the underwriter speaks for the company. And so it's, you expect that any underwriter, that it doesn't matter which underwriter you get for the premiums. And the company has that expectation. It shouldn't make much difference. So we tested that and they constructed some cases and then we had some like 50 underwriters assess a premium for the case. With the same information.
Starting point is 01:02:31 Yeah, with a really very realistic, we didn't construct it. They constructed the cases and they conducted the experiment. But now the interesting question is how much variation you expect there to be? So we asked the executives the following, I'm supposed to take two underwriters of random. By what percentage does they differ? I mean, you look at the difference between their premium, divide that by the average premium, what number do you get? And people expect 10%. By the way, it's not only the executives in that company, for some reason. People expect 10%. And it was roughly 50%. 5.0. So that's, you know,
Starting point is 01:03:14 that's what made me curious about noise. That and the fact that the company was completely unaware that it had noise. It took them completely by surprise. So now we're writing a book because there's a lot of noise. So wherever a rule is that wherever there is judgment, there is noise and more of it than you think. So that's the pattern. Are there procedures to reduce noise? And conversely, it strikes me that the variation would be good, but maybe only in an evolutionary concept. Well, we call that noise is useless. variability. I mean, variability can be very useful if you have a selection mechanism and some feedback. So evolution is built a variability, but of course it's useful. But noise among
Starting point is 01:04:04 underwriters is useless. There's nothing. Nothing gets learned. There's no feedback. It's just noise. And it's costly. The first advice, of course, would be algorithms, as I said earlier. So I Algorithm are better than people on judgment. That's not intuitive, but it's really true. And after that, then, you know, the procedure that I mentioned earlier for making decisions in an orderly way by breaking it up into assessments, that's the best that we can do. And there is one very important aspect that I haven't mentioned.
Starting point is 01:04:44 And this is training people in what the scale is. So there is one piece of advice that you'd have for underwriters that they should always compare the case to other cases. And if possible, if you can have them share the same frame of reference with other underwriters, you're going to cut down on the noise. Oh, that's a clever idea, yeah. So controlling the scale, and that exists in human resources where performance evaluation, which is one of the scandals of modern commerce,
Starting point is 01:05:24 how difficult it is, but performance evaluation, they have a thing that's called frame of reference training, which is teaching people how to use the scale. There's a lot of variability in the scale. And a part of what the super forecasters do, they make judgments in probability units, and they teach them to use the probability scale. So learning the scale is a very important aspect of the new signals.
Starting point is 01:05:55 I know we're coming out to the end of our time here. What have you changed your mind on in the past 10 years? Oh, large. Anything big? Yeah. There's been a replication crisis in psychology, and some of the stuff that I really believed in when I wrote Thinking Fast and Slow,
Starting point is 01:06:14 some of the evidence has been discredited. So I've had to change my mind. What are the, what's the biggest? Some of the sexiest stuff, priming and unconscious priming. So this hasn't held up in replication. And I believed it and I wrote it as a fit word true because, you know, the evidence suggested it. And in fact, I thought that you had to accept it because that was published evidence.
Starting point is 01:06:43 And I should have, I blame myself. for having been a big gullible, that as I should have known, that you can publish things even if they're not true, but I just didn't think that through. So I changed my mind. I'm now much more cautious
Starting point is 01:07:02 about spectacular findings. I mean, very recently, I think I have a theory about why psychologists are prone or social scientists generally are prone to exaggerate to be overconfident about their hypotheses. So I've done quite a bit of learn.
Starting point is 01:07:22 What's the theory? Well, the theory, one element of the theory, is that all these hypotheses are true in what sense? That, you know, if I, there's a famous study that you mention wrinkles to people, and then you measure to speed it with, they walk and they walk more slowly. Turns out that hasn't held up.
Starting point is 01:07:45 up in replication, which is very painful. It's some of the favorite studies. But actually, you know that if you mentioned wrinkle and it's going to have any effect on the speed of walking, it's not going to make people faster. It has any influence. It's going to make them slower. So directionally, all these hypotheses are dream.
Starting point is 01:08:07 But what there is is what people don't see is that then huge number of things factors that determine the speed at which individuals walk and the differences in the speed of walking between individuals. And that's noise and people neglect noise. And then there is something else, which touches on both philosophy and psychology. When you have intuitions about things, there are clear intuitions and there are strong intuitions. Another say, so clear intuition is if I offer you a trip to Rome, a trip to Rome and an ice cream call, you know what you prefer, it's easy.
Starting point is 01:08:52 But it's very weak, of course, I mean, the amount of money you would pay to get a trip to Rome and a trip to Rome and an ice cream call nothing. But when you are a philosopher, and I should add one thing, to see clear intuitions, you have to be in this kind of situation that psychologists call within subject. that you have both with the ice cream cone and without the ice cream code. So in a within subject situation, that's an easy problem. In a between subject situation, it's an impossible problem.
Starting point is 01:09:27 But now if you're a philosopher, you're always in a within subject situation. But people live in a between subject situation. They live in one's condition. And the same thing is true for psychologists. So psychologists live in a, when they cook up their hypotheses, they're in a within-subject situation. But then they make guesses about what will happen between subjects.
Starting point is 01:09:53 And they're completely lost between clear intuitions and strong intuition. We have no way of calibrating all sorts. So that makes us wildly overconfident about what we know and reluctant to accept that we may be wrong. That's a great place to end this conversation, Danny. Thank you so much. Thanks for listening and learning with us. Be sure to sign up for my free weekly newsletter at fs.blog slash newsletter.
Starting point is 01:10:24 The Farnham Street website is also where you can get more info on our membership program, which includes access to episode transcripts, my repository, add-free episodes, and more. Follow myself and Farnham Street on X, Instagram, and LinkedIn to stay in the loop. Plus, you can watch full episodes. on our YouTube channel. If you like what we're doing here, leaving a rating and review would mean the world. And if you really like us, sharing with a friend is the best way to grow this community. Until next time.

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.