The One You Feed - Changing How We Choose with David Redish
Episode Date: December 3, 2024In this episode, David Redish explains why we should be thinking about changing how we choose and explores the process of decision-making. He discusses the systems our brains use to make decisions and... how these systems interact and influence our choices, from everyday decisions to complex moral dilemmas. Key Takeaways: The three decision-making systems in our brains and how they function How the way we frame questions can dramatically alter our decisions The role of morality as a tool for fostering cooperation and mutual benefit Practical strategies for aligning our actions with our values The complex nature of addiction and its relationship to decision-making Feeling overwhelmed by holiday stress or the pressure to make everything perfect? Or maybe it’s the loneliness this season can bring. Either way, you’re not alone—and this year can be different. Join us for a free online webinar on Sunday, December 10, at 12 PM Eastern to learn a simple habit that can help you let go of stress and find peace, steadiness, and genuine connection. Give yourself this gift of support and clarity for the season. Sign up here. For full show notes, click here! Connect with the show: Follow us on YouTube: @TheOneYouFeedPod Subscribe on Apple Podcasts or Spotify Follow us on Instagram See omnystudio.com/listener for privacy information.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Are you feeling stressed about the holidays? The pressure to make everything perfect?
The tricky family gatherings? Trying to keep everybody happy? And for some of you,
it's the opposite. It's the feeling of being alone. Either way, it's a lot.
So if you're feeling overwhelmed, know that many others feel the same way.
But this year can be different. Join me and the One You Feed community for a free
online event on December 10th at 12
p.m. Eastern. You'll learn a simple wise habit that can help you let go of holiday stress
and find more peace, steadiness, and real connection. Picture yourself getting through
this season feeling grounded, present, and clear without all the pressure to be perfect. Give
yourself this free gift of support,
learning, and genuine connection with others who get it. Sign up now at oneufeed.net slash holiday.
If you're unable to attend live, we'll be sending the video to people who have registered.
So go to oneufeed.net slash holiday and change the way you experience the holidays.
holiday and change the way you experience the holidays.
It turns out that addiction is a symptom, not a disease, that it's more like fever.
There are many, many things that will lead you to addiction.
And yes, your description of a Pavlovian system that gets over associated and connected is one of those.
Welcome to The One You Feed. Throughout time, great thinkers have recognized the importance of the thoughts we have. Quotes like, garbage in, garbage out, or you are what you think,
ring true. And yet, for many of us, our thoughts don't strengthen or
empower us. We tend toward negativity, self-pity, jealousy, or fear. We see what we don't have
instead of what we do. We think things that hold us back and dampen our spirit. But it's not just
about thinking. Our actions matter. It takes conscious, consistent, and creative effort to make a life
worth living. This podcast is about how other people keep themselves moving in the right
direction, how they feed their good wolf.
I'm Jason Alexander.
And I'm Peter Tilden. And together, our mission on the Really Know Really podcast
is to get the true answers to life's baffling questions like
why the bathroom door doesn't go all the way to the floor,
what's in the museum of failure, and does your dog truly love you?
We have the answer.
Go to reallyknoworeally.com
and register to win $500, a guest spot on our podcast or a limited edition signed Jason
bobblehead. The Really Know Really podcast. Follow us on the iHeartRadio app, Apple podcasts,
or wherever you get your podcasts. Thanks for joining us. Back on the show after many years
is David Reddish, a professor of neuroscience at the University of Minnesota.
His laboratory studies learning, memory, and how animals, including humans, make decisions.
David has worked extensively on the consequences of understanding those decision processes in the
world we live in, including consequences for psychiatry, sociology, economics, and morality.
Today, David and Eric discuss his new book,
Changing How We Choose, The New Science of Morality. 2015. Wow. Okay. Well, that would have been in our second year and we're creeping up on our 10th
year. So yeah. Thank you. Thank you. You've got a new book out called Changing How We Choose,
The New Science of Morality. And we're going to discuss that in a moment, but we'll start like
we always do with the parable. In the parable, there's a grandparent who's talking with their
grandchild and they say, in life, there are two wolves inside of us that are always at battle.
who's talking with their grandchild.
And they say, in life, there are two wolves inside of us that are always at battle.
One is a good wolf, which represents things like kindness
and bravery and love.
And the other is a bad wolf,
which represents things like greed and hatred and fear.
And the grandchild stops, and they think about it for a second,
and they look up at their grandparent, and they say,
well, which one wins?
And the grandparent says, the one you feed.
So I'd like to start off by asking you what that
parable means to you in your life and in the work that you do. It's a great parable. I have to admit
that the first time I heard it was on the show in 2015. And I see it everywhere now. It became very
interesting to me. I think the parable is really about the kind of person you want to be, and importantly,
how the kind of person you want to be changes the decisions you make, which is really interesting
to me because one of the things that we now know is a lot of the decisions we make, a lot of the
actions we take, are done very quickly. That is that in the moment, there isn't a lot of the actions we take are done very quickly. That is that in the moment, there isn't a lot of processing going on.
And what that means is what really matters is how did you build up the learning to that moment?
And one of the things that's very interesting and I think is underappreciated by many people is that when we talk about learning that leads to decision making, it's not just learning like the facts of the world, but you learn your own motivations and you learn your own goals. And, you know, we can talk about learning for ourselves what to do in this
moment. And I think a lot of that parable is really about changing the information process
that's going to happen in that moment so that you take the action for the kind of person you want to be. And I think
it's a wonderful parable for really understanding that kind of complexity.
Yeah, I think that's a great way of phrasing it. And I'm really interested in getting into
these questions about how do we decide? And like you said, what is the accumulation of things that
have brought me
to this point where that's the decision? You know, in Buddhism, we would call it conditioning.
And we are all the result of countless causes and conditions.
Yeah, absolutely.
And so trying to figure out what I, you know, I'm doing air quotes, what I want
is a very complicated thing as we're going to get into some of your work that's going to show us.
So in order to get where I want to get, we're going to have to go through a couple of slightly technical areas here. So listeners just hang in there because I think where we're headed is a big
payoff, but we're going to be talking some theory here for a minute. And so I think that the first
thing we need to do is lay out. Can I respond to one thing you said that
actually I think is really interesting is the I point that you made with the air quotes. Because
one of the things that's really come to the fore, honestly, since 2015, which is interesting,
is that a lot of current theories of consciousness are based on your observation of yourself.
So humans are social creatures.
It's one of the things that makes humans particularly unique is just how social we are.
I mean, other animals are social, but they're social in other ways.
And we evolved ways to understand each other in terms of this kind of complexity and agency.
And then we observe ourselves taking action, some of which may or may not always be the
obvious one we thought we wanted.
And current theories of consciousness are that we have applied that social understanding
of others to an observation of ourselves. And so there's a
really interesting, oh, I'm the one that did that, is part of our definition of who we are. I think
that's a very interesting perspective on that idea. Right. And I think the other thing that
more modern ideas of consciousness seem to be positing, and they're talked about in different ways and referenced in different styles, is that there's really not a coherent me that's in here.
It's not one thing.
Right.
We're going to talk about the three ways that you believe decisions get made.
So that points to there's these three different deciding systems.
Yeah. that you believe decisions get made. So that points to there's these three different deciding systems. You know, there's a psychology, internal family systems, which posits that we have all these different parts and that there is one part that is sort of the core self. But there's lots
of different theories of this that strike to the heart of what Buddhism has been saying for a long, long time, which is that if you really look closely,
you can't find you as a discrete thing. What you find is lots of different processes running and
happening and competing priorities and goals and different feelings and just a lot going on in
there. And yet there is some thing that appears to be observing that. So it's interesting to me
because I think the modern psychology neuroscience is coming together and echoing some of those
more ancient ideas about what's going on inside of us. So maybe we should head from here into
talking about how people make decisions. And you talk about three, possibly a
fourth way that humans decide. So before we go into that, give me an example of the type of
decision we might be talking about here. So for me, and I think for neuroscience in general,
the question of what is a decision becomes such a ugly philosophical, you know,
rabbit hole that I want to punt on the whole question of what is a decision. And I'm going
to simply say, if you've taken an action, then I'm going to call that a decision. Okay. Any action.
So that could be lifting your arm, that could be hitting a baseball, that could be diving into the river to save a drowning child. You know, any action is going to be...
Playing solitaire versus writing your book, that kind of thing.
Absolutely. And it includes big actions, right? Big actions like, where did you go to college? It includes emotional actions like falling in love, right?
And the key is that if we re-ask the question in terms of why did you take that action?
How did you take that action?
It actually becomes a very measurable scientific question that almost un-asks all that decision-making stuff.
So what we find is that there are at least three, I guess four if you include reflexes,
there's some evidence, there may be some other ones that are more complicated, but three
big ones that really speak to most of humanity.
And those three are, first of all, what we call deliberative or
planning systems. And these are literally imagining the future and then evaluating it.
It's like running a little war game in your head or simulation, right? If you don't want to do a
war game, we can do simulation, right? A little, oh, what would it be like if I went to this college? What would my life
be like? What if I took this job, right? And you run that simulation, you imagine yourself into
that future, a mentally time travel. The term is actually called mental time travel into that
future. And then you run that simulation. So say I'm trying to make a decision. I would create a pro and con list.
Would that be the deliberative system at work?
Exactly.
Okay.
Exactly.
And people often think it's logical.
I don't want to suggest it's logical because a lot of what it is is actually emotional of, oh, that sounds like a lot of fun.
Or, oh, that sounds really boring, right?
I mean, yes, pro and con lists are definitely part of that deliberative system. But the key is that it's an actual imagined future. It is, of course,
therefore very slow, but it's very flexible, right? I can imagine, you know, doing something stupid
and then not do it, right? I don't have to actually take the action to imagine it.
The problem with that is it's very slow. If you're going to do something that's quick, like hitting a baseball or responding in the moment to playing a jazz concert, if you're doing back and forth with somebody else in trading fours or something, you don't have time to go and think through that imagined scenario, right? You say, okay, if I put my arm in just this way and I swing just this way,
you struck out, you know, you just, you literally don't have the time. So that's a second system.
That system we call procedural. People I've talked to like to say, you should think of it as muscle
memory. I'm not so fond of that term because it's not in the muscles. It's a different part of the
brain. But I think colloquially it captures some of what people think of that. It can be quite complicated, like driving to work.
If you drive to work the same way every day, then you're using that memory system,
that decision system, that procedural system.
So that procedural system isn't necessarily just incredibly fast things. It's also once something
becomes what we would think of as a true habit, meaning we do it without thinking about it.
Exactly. And in fact, sports stars often talk about being, quote, in the zone, right? And what
that means is that their procedural system is running really smoothly. So what the procedural
system is doing is recognizing
situations in the world and then responding to them with well-practiced actions. So this is,
very importantly, a completely different set of neural circuits. So if we use something like the
functional magnetic resonance imaging, which is a technology that can look inside your brain
and see which parts of your brain are active. A completely
different set of circuits will be active if you're doing something procedural than if you're doing
something deliberative. Can I ask you a question about fMRI? Because I hear criticisms of fMRI
from time to time saying, like, it's not really telling you anything. And the criticism is often
leveled at spiritual teachers who are trying to talk about something they don't understand.
I'm guilty of that from time to time.
But as somebody who's actually in the science, like works, you know, in this world, how accurate a picture do you think fMRI is giving us of what's going on?
So you have to think of fMRI as a tool.
And as a tool, it's an excellent and very powerful tool, but it's not a magic item.
And part of the problem is that it gets oversold. What fMRI is actually measuring is how active,
how much work that part of the brain is doing. So when your brain is doing certain amounts of work,
and different parts of your brain work for different things, right? You have a visual
cortex in the back of your head. So if you're looking at something
that's changing very quickly, your visual cortex in the back of your head is going to do a lot of
work. Whereas your auditory cortex, as we're talking to each other, is also doing a lot of
work right now. But honestly, your somatosensory, your touch cortices, are not doing that much work
right now. They would be if you were, say, you know, reading Brailleensory, your touch cortices, are not doing that much work right now.
They would be if you were, say, reading Braille or something, if you were feeling something,
trying to understand a certain texture, right?
So what happens with fMRI is you can measure that.
And so you can say, ah, this part of the brain is active, is doing a lot of work.
part of the brain is active, is doing a lot of work. Now, it turns out, particularly in cortical areas, that that level of work can get pretty fine-tuned. It's different in different brains,
but the kind of reading mind stuff is real to a limited extent. So, for example, I can tell if
you're thinking about faces or houses because your visualization of faces and your visualization of houses happens to be in pretty different parts of your brain.
Yeah.
Right.
It would be like think about it this way.
You're measuring the electricity going to a building. And if one building in New York City is made of Jets fans,
and another building is made of Giants fans, and the Giants are playing that day,
the building that is almost all Giants fans will have a lot more electricity.
Because all their TVs will be on. They'll all be watching the game on the fly. So I don't know how
good an analogy this is. What fMRI is measuring is the amount of electricity that's going through, in this case,
blood, right? In this case, actually, it's not blood. It's technically the parts of the brain
when it works harder, it asks for more blood. And so more blood goes to that part of the brain,
but it doesn't need any more oxygen. The parts that it needs from the blood is something else.
Honestly, it's not my field. I don't actually know what it is. I don't even know if it's known. But what it means is you
get more blood, but it uses less oxygen. So there's more oxygen in the blood. And that's what's
detectable by fMRI. Got it. So think of it as like, there's more something being supplied to
that part of the brain. Yeah. And what that means, though, is that I can ask somebody to do something procedurally
or deliberatively, and what I find is different parts of the brain are working at different times.
And when you say different parts of the brain, you use the word circuits earlier,
and I've heard people start to talk about networks, meaning it may not be just one part
of the brain. It may be parts of the
brain that are linking together in a certain way, or it's a little bit more than just like this
teensy little part of your brain is the one that's doing everything. Is that an accurate way to think
of it? Absolutely. The way I like to say it is if I took that piece of your brain out and put it in
a little dish, it wouldn't do anything, right? It's like taking the steering wheel out of the car
and putting it in the street. It's not going to steer. That doesn't mean that the steering wheel
is the same as the tires. Okay. We had gotten through, I believe, the deliberative and the
procedural, and basically different parts of the brain are working depending on what's
going on there. So what's the next type? There's a third system and it's really important because
a lot of the old psychology theories will talk about two processes and there's actually three.
It's really important because the third one is as different from either of the other two as they are
from each other. And this third one, I like to call
instinctual now, but is within the field called Pavlovian because it's what Pavlov's dogs were
doing. The problem is that it confuses everybody when they think about it because we all have this
psychology 101 we heard about Pavlov and stuff. And so I'm going to call it instinctual to separate it from that.
Again, colloquially, people talk about it as like going with the gut. But again, it's not your gut.
It's a different part of the brain. And it is a third circuit in the brain that is completely
different from the other two. And what this one is, which turns out to be much more important than people used to think. This third one is a set of species
important behaviors, some of them very complicated, that are available immediately,
but you learn when to release. So classically, fight or flight, right? But also going out to
help somebody. Diving into the river to save a drowning child
turns out to be this instinctual system. Running away from a fire, right? Running away from the
lion, right? But it turns out it's also complicated things like posture and submit,
right? If you have two people, one kind of, you know, yelling at the other. I always love that scene in the Black Panther movie, the first one, where they're in this
big battle and the guy looks over and sees that his girlfriend and all the women are
fighting with all the guys.
And he's like, this is horrible.
And he throws his sword down and kneels in front of her.
And there's this absolute iconic moment of her with her spear.
And you know, in that instant, the complete
relationship, that's completely instinctual systems. And the reason we can recognize that
is because those two behaviors are instinctual human behaviors that we are learning when to
release. And in fact, the whole mating dance, if you look at two people going back and forth in a bar, right, that is also part of this instinctual system.
So this instinctual system, might I have instinctual responses that you don't have?
Yeah.
Okay. So when we say species specific, we mean they are human, but we don't all have them in the same way and to the same degree.
And to what degree are
those, back to the term we used earlier, learned or conditioned? So it's complicated. And to be
honest, it's still a lot of science work being done here. Some of the simpler ones are pretty
straightforward, like run away or throw a punch or laughing with your friends, for example. These seem to be pretty universal. I personally am convinced that cursing is this system as compared to a conversation, which is not.
cursing is of course we all curse in our own language right yeah that the actual curse right the bad word we say we learn right in fact uh one of my kids at one point when we you know we're
trying to get him not to curse he started saying sicily as his angry word yeah yeah there's lots
of people say freaking you know so yeah exactly So there's some process in which we train that system then.
Right.
But usually the output is relatively consistent.
Like I can tell a smile on any person.
And yes, a European smile is slightly different from an American smile.
Americans tend to really smile with their mouths and Europeans think we're crazy. You know, a European will often smile primarily with their
eyes. It's a cultural difference, particularly in parts of Europe. But the basic idea of laughing
or smiling or whatever will be consistent, right? The real learning that happens in this system,
and this is kind of where honestly the one you feed turns out to be really important, is what is the condition under which you're going to release that behavior? Right. So I like to joke swinging a bat in a baseball game and in a bar are very different.
bar are very different. Right. Or in a situation, right, if you hear a crying child next door to you, right, do you react by saying, oh, no, what's wrong? Or do you react with shut that thing up?
Right. Those are both Pavlovian reactions. Those are both instinctual reactions. But which one you
release at that moment, right, emotionally is going to depend on the learning you've done
over time. That's a really interesting example of how you respond to a baby crying and that sound.
And I think the other thing that's interesting about that is that it seems to me that the way
I respond to that sound has something to do with my physiological and emotional state
at the time. Absolutely. When I'm well slept and feeling pretty good and not hungry, I might be
like, I wonder what's wrong with that baby. Somebody should give it a bottle. And if I got
two hours of sleep and I haven't eaten in several hours, I'm probably going to be more towards shut
that thing up. Right. And so even that is going to be dependent on factors within my physiology.
Absolutely. But part of the situation you're in depends not just on the physical surroundings,
but your cultural moment. Are you hungry? Are you well fed? Are you comfortable? Are you at the end
of your rope? Do you have a big exam the next day? You know, are you anxious?
All of these components feed into that situation. And the truth is that they feed into a lot of
these other situations. These decision systems interact in complex ways, right? So playing sports,
you want to be active and I want to say aggressive without being aggressive.
Right.
Right.
You want to be aggressive within the game without being aggressive in a violent, you know, interaction.
Right.
You still want to be friends at the end of the game. I'm Jason Alexander.
And I'm Peter Tilden.
And together on the Really No Really podcast,
our mission is to get the true answers to life's baffling questions like...
Why they refuse to make the bathroom door go all the way to the floor.
We got the answer.
Will space junk block your cell signal?
The astronaut who almost drowned during a spacewalk gives us the answer.
We talk with the scientist who figured out if your dog truly loves you.
And the one bringing back the woolly mammoth.
Plus, does Tom Cruise really do his own stunts?
His stuntman reveals the answer.
And you never know who's going to drop by.
Mr. Bryan Cranston is with us today.
How are you, too?
Hello, my friend.
Wayne Knight about Jurassic Park.
Wayne Knight, welcome to Really No Really, sir.
Bless you all.
Hello, Newman.
And you never know when Howie Mandel might just stop by to talk about judging.
Really? That's the opening?
Really No Really. Yeah, really.
No really.
Go to reallynoreally.com.
And register to win $500, a guest spot on our podcast, or a limited edition signed Jason Bobblehead.
It's called Really, No Really, and you can find it on the iHeartRadio app, on Apple Podcasts, or wherever you get your podcasts.
So let's take jumping into the lake to save a drowning child.
Sure.
Not everybody's going to do that, but some people will.
We would consider that a moral choice.
Like, it's good to do that.
Like, you should do that versus if you don't do that, maybe that's bad.
And again, maybe different people are going to have different moralities around that,
but most of us would view something like that as a moral
choice. So where I wanted to go is, you know, if you read about morality and I'm interested in
morality, I want to talk about that term before a bunch of people just turn off their brain.
You know, I'm interested in morality in the sense of how do I think I should live my life?
How do I decide what's important and who's the person I want to
be? So for me, it becomes a very personal thing. I'm not very interested. Well, to say I'm not
interested in others' morality is not correct, but I just want to be clear. I'm not talking about,
you know, the moral majority here or enforcing my morality on others, but that's why I'm interested
in it. Jonathan Haidt is the person that I most have read this from, but I'm sure he's
borrowed it from lots of different places, is this idea that our morals are actually feelings.
You know, you ask me a moral question, I respond, that feeling is there. And then I'm going to layer
reasons on top of that. I'm going to try and this is what I believe to be true. Now I'm believing that for
reasons that are entirely emotional. Now I'm going to try and construct an argument around that
instead of the other way around. This is one of those things where I really disagree with Jonathan
on these things. I think that he is, which is surprising given his knowledge base,
he's taking a very simple view of decision making. And so I would turn it around
and say, there's a moral question of what do we do in this situation? There are two questions that
we want to separate. One is, what is the prescriptive goal that we either as an individual or as a society want to do. And I agree with you
that I don't want to talk about imposing moral codes on anybody. What I want to understand is
how do those moral codes interact with us? The other thing, though, is that there are many
situations where each of those decision systems might be more appropriate and might be the thing
to do. So, for example, in a lot of these moral questions, I can change which of these systems
is taking control by changing how I phrase the question. An interesting story, this diving into
the river is a very interesting thing. I was on a trip with, I was at some conference and they had a afternoon thing where we got to go fishing in streams in Colorado,
which I, it was really quite wonderful. And it was with this guide who was this wonderful
talkative guide who clearly was name dropping every five minutes of all the famous people he
had, you know, guided, but he was a character. He was a wonderful, you know,
really wonderful guy. But he told this story about, he pointed out that these little streams actually run super fast. And you can be shockingly pulled under. And what looks like, you know,
two feet of water turns out it's eight feet of water and you're underneath. And he described
this story where he was driving by with somebody and they saw somebody drawn underneath and the two of them
stopped the car. The two of them got out and he told the other person, don't go, don't move.
Because he had been trained on how to go save somebody in this situation. And the other person
who, of course, in this instinctual, I'm going to go dive in the
river and be the hero and save the person, would only have made things worse. So he had been trained
procedurally how to respond in this moment. And the other person was responding instinctually.
Right. So, in fact, you could create, and I would argue that the discussion we are having right now about, do you dive into the river to save the child, is actually a deliberative one, not an instinctual one.
We're having a deliberative conversation about it.
And so I really disagree with Jonathan that these moral things start from feelings.
If you ask the question in very specific ways,
which is what he tends to do, he tends to create these very charged emotional moments in his
examples, they will activate that instinctual system. Whereas if you ask the question in a
different way, right, then you get a different answer. The famous story is the trolley problem
on this one. Yeah, I think that's really interesting and makes more sense to me because,
while I do believe there is a huge emotional component to what we think is right or wrong,
we are capable of deliberating and changing that. Right. You know, if you're open-minded enough, your moral views can shift.
And I would almost argue should shift if you're actually a very thoughtful person.
Oh, okay.
Say more.
So there are many, many cases where the instinctual is the right choice.
And there are other cases where the deliberative is the right choice. And there are other cases where the deliberative is the
right choice. A lot of the thing I think is kind of what I like to think of as, you know,
the learning I'm trying to do, and I think people should try to do is figure out when is each of
these systems best, right? Each of these systems, it's not that one is better than the other.
They've each evolved for conditions.
And like, if you're deciding who to fall in love with, making a list of pros and cons isn't really great.
You kind of want to let the chemistry happen and see what happens.
Well, as somebody who's been led astray by his chemistry a few times, I'm not sure I
agree.
I get your point. Yeah.
And the truth is, would you have been more led astray by your deliberative system?
Well, that's a whole nother conversation. So you've talked about how we ask the question changes the result that we get. So part of what you and I talked about before this interview is I'm trying
to take your work about moral systems as a whole applied to societies and groups. And I'm trying
to sort of extract that and take it down to how do I make decisions in my life about what matters
to me and what's important. So occasionally I'm sort of running aground in attempting to do
that. But if I'm trying to make a decision, I'm trying to think of what's a common moral type
decision that a human being makes. Let me see if I can bin a connection for you.
Please.
So one of the things that Jonathan's work, and in fact, all of the other science that's tried to do a science of morality
before, they have essentially punted on the question of what is the goal, right? What is
your moral goal? So we said saving babies is good. I think we all generally agree on that.
But where does that come from? Why is saving babies good? Right. If we all accept what those moral rules are, then, yes, we can talk about how do we as an individual find the best one for ourselves. rules, at which point you run into some really nastiness that I'll be honest, I'm not comfortable
with. And I think one of the things that makes that connection is what I argue in this book is
that there is actually a moral rule, a very general one, that ends up having kind of consequences.
And all the other things kind of fall from those consequences.
And I'm kind of dancing in a circle. I'm not sure if I'm getting to the right spot. So if I wander too much, stop me and we can come back. But I wonder if some of that connection of these examples comes to recognizing what the individual large-term goal is, which
is to align our work together, to find ways to work together, to create so that what's best for
you is best for me, and what's best for me is best for you. And I actually have been very intrigued
once I kind of see that. And we can talk kind of
about, as you said, about the fMRI and that about the actual science of it, that might be useful.
But once I saw that, it changes how I interact in those decisions with other people, right? I mean,
I'll be explicit about it. I'll say, oh, you know, look, I want you to understand I'm doing this because
if you succeed, I do better. I say this to my students, right? I'm a professor. A lot of my
life is teaching students. And, you know, I was talking to, I just, my class just ended this,
you know, a week ago. And I was really pleased this year. I got a lot of positive feedback from
the students. And I wonder how much of it is because I started off by saying, my goal is for you to succeed, because if you succeed and go do well, and go say to everybody, wow, I learned all this in this class. Right? That's better for me. Right? That our goals are aligned.
Yeah.
Is maybe the way to say it. say a game where I might win and you might lose, to move us collectively to games where
when we cooperate, we both win bigger and better. And that that's the purpose of a moral code is to
get more of us to play that kind of game than more of a game in which selfishness is prioritized.
Now I've oversimplified dramatically there,
but is that close? I don't think you've oversimplified at all. I think that's actually
exactly it. People always talk of morality as do the right thing, even though we know you really
want to cheat. And honestly, that's not my lived experience. I don't know about you, but my experience is most of the time
I do the moral thing because I want to do the moral thing. And what I realized is exactly that
these moral codes are tools. They're sociological and they're personal tools. Actually, an interesting
thing is to think of them as personal tools. The one you feed is a great example of a personal tool that helps you achieve behaviors that are
more likely to be in that connection. So I don't think you've oversimplified at all. I think you're
exactly right. So if I then take that idea and say, all right, I want to apply it to myself and how I decide,
let's talk about this idea that how I ask the question changes the answer.
Can you give me an example of that perhaps in our individual life?
Yeah.
So is your goal as a student to learn or is your goal as a student to get the most points,
right?
And if your goal is to get the most points, right? And if
your goal is to get the most points, then you're going to do a whole set of behaviors, right? And
if your goal is, I'm going to learn the most I can, then you're going to do a different set of
behaviors. Some people will argue, oh, in order to get into med school, you have to get an A,
you have to do this, and you have to succeed, right? And the question is, are they correct? And when you talk to the people who actually know,
and the people who actually, you know, make those decisions, they're often wrong. And it's often
the people who are helping the other people that are doing better, right? So a good example is
convincing all my students that study groups,
getting the most points while everybody else gets less points means that, yes, on the ratio of point
scales, you did better. But if you can create a study group where you all can work together,
you can all get A's, right? So they started doing study groups. So by changing the question of
saying, look, what's my goal? Is my
goal to get, I want to get a hundred points and everybody gets zero, or is my goal, I want
everybody to get a hundred points because we've all then learned, that changes it.
That goal though, the underlying goal itself.
Right.
You ask two people, one of them might say, I'm here to get A's because I'm here to then go on to grad school. And, you know, you can learn if you want. I don't much care. Not my thing. Someone
else very much saying, hey, look, you know, I really want to learn. I mean, I just had a
conversation with a guy who has a book called The Pathless Path. He was like a very successful,
I don't know if he went to Harvard. It doesn't matter. He went through the whole Ivy League
track, was working with the biggest, the best business consultancy group, McKinsey. He was on that track
and he used a phrase there called, it was a world-class hoop jumper. Meaning his job was
just to jump through these series of hoops, never thinking about why, except that it's because
that's the next hoop to get through to get to this place over here. And we could say we've got a hoop jumper on one hand, and we've got a person who wants to learn on the other hand.
The question does become, where is that goal coming from?
Even the question I ask is informed by my moral approach or my values.
That's right.
One issue is, of course, what is the individual goal, right?
But also, there is, how do you achieve that kind of greatness? And by changing the question,
you actually find that the people who are doing the most of those greatnesses are actually the
people who are working together with groups and are not the people who've elbowed everybody out
of the way. But there are other examples where we can talk about these decision
systems, you know, concretely, like, so for example, oh, here's a good one. If I'm asking
you to pick a picture to put on your wall, I could ask you to say, find the one you like,
or I could say, find the one you like, and then explain it to me. If I tell you to explain it to me,
you end up coming up with a very different answer. And there's a great experiment that did this with,
you know, college kids, as, you know, psychology experiments used to do. We do less of that now.
But, you know, classically, it was this great experiment of, you know, college kids. Some of
them, they said, pick a picture, take it home. And Some of them, they said, pick a picture, take it home.
And some of them, they said, pick a picture and come back and explain to me why you picked this
picture for your wall. And, you know, they told each of them that they were measuring all these
things. And of course, what they actually measured was who has their picture on the wall a week
later, right? And the ones who explained it, none of them had their picture on the wall. The ones
who just said, I like, I like this and put it up, right? They used a different decision system to pick the picture.
By changing the question of, are we a team, right? Are we individuals? Are we, you know,
are we a community? I was trying to think of community construction examples, things like barn building or block
parties change the definition of who we think we are.
I have a note where I started thinking about empathy as a technology, that creating empathy
with other people, which we can do through stories. And these are ways for us to
understand and change our view of people. You think of, for example, the famous napalm picture
from the Vietnam War, right, of the girl, of Kim Phuc, the young girl who, you know,
was very famously, the picture of her, you know, running from her village, you know, with Nepal, you know, being burned, changed everybody's view. It changed the question.
It was no longer a question of are we, you know, stopping this, you know, communist menace of
coming back and forth. It was, are we killing kids? It suddenly changes the question. That
technology of a photograph changes the empathy. There are
all of these examples where we can shift these decision questions by changing the questions we
ask. And I love this example of the one you feed is changing the questions you ask yourself.
That's the whole point of the parable, right? It changes the decisions you make
by pulling up different answers in different crises.
I'm Jason Alexander.
And I'm Peter Tilden.
And together on the Really No Really podcast,
our mission is to get the true answers to life's baffling questions like why they refuse to make the bathroom door go all the way to the floor.
We got the answer.
Will space junk block your cell signal?
The astronaut who almost drowned during a spacewalk gives us the answer.
We talk with the scientist who figured
out if your dog truly loves you
and the one bringing back the woolly mammoth.
Plus, does Tom
Cruise really do his own stunts?
His stuntman reveals the answer.
And you never know who's going to drop by.
Mr. Bryan Cranston is with us today. How are you, too?
Hello, my friend. Wayne Knight about Jurassic
Park. Wayne Knight, welcome to Really, No Really, sir.
Bless you all.
Hello, Newman.
And you never know when Howie Mandel might just stop by to talk about judging.
Really?
That's the opening?
Really, No Really.
Yeah, really.
No really.
Go to reallynoreally.com.
And register to win $500, a guest spot on our podcast, or a limited edition signed Jason bobblehead.
It's called Really, No Really, and you can find it on the iHeartRadio app, on Apple Podcasts, or wherever
you get your podcasts. I'm a firm believer that one of the paths to a good life is to ask yourself
questions very often about what am I doing and why? You know, what am I doing? Why? What do I want to
be doing? What matters? I mean, these are not fun questions to ask necessarily, because we often
find ourselves in, I don't know, you know, or no, I'm not living the way I wanted. But the question
itself is the important thing. You know, the question is what in and of itself, kind of what
you're saying, even asking the question starts to nudge you in a certain
direction. It's sort of like if you start tracking calories, right? Just the very active tracking
calories will shift how you eat. Absolutely. I'm not saying it'll get you all the way where you
need to go, but over and over again, we've seen studies that show just that action is going to
nudge you towards better eating. And so asking questions, I think, regularly nudges us
towards being the version of ourselves that we want to be. Yes. But also tracking calories is
a great example of, am I eating because I'm hungry? Am I eating because I'm social, right? Am I eating because I'm trying to get the right nutrition, right?
And partially there's asking questions of yourself, but partially every moment in your life,
you are answering a question, right? At every moment, you are taking an action in response to
some question in the world. And one of the points that I'm making is what that question
in the world is, like, is this food or not? Is a different question from should I throw these
mashed potatoes or not? I mean, like, if you're in a food fight, right? These are two very different
questions, and they're going to come up with very different answers. And the argument in the book is
that because we have multiple decision systems, because these decision systems learn, because these decision systems interact with the world, if we change the world, we can change those decision systems. And so we can change our behavior by changing the world, not just by changing ourselves.
not just by changing ourselves. Let me pivot off there. I'm tempted to go big. And then I'm also tempted to go small here. I'm going to go small for a second. And then maybe we zoom back out.
And the small is this, let's say that, well, let's just stick with eating. And I decide
that I want to lose weight. Okay. Now, I think what you're saying is that, A, the very question we need to be asking
ourselves is why, right? Because that informs an awful lot, right? It's because I want to look good
in a bathing suit, or it's because I want to keep my cholesterol low because my dad died of a heart
attack at 60. And I know for me, as I've reframed exercise and diet and all that in terms of well-being, not even future
well-being, but current well-being, that changed my relationship to those things dramatically.
Yeah.
So I think that's one example of the question.
Absolutely.
But let's say that I say my deliberative self, I'm assuming this would be my deliberative self,
says, I want to lose weight and I want to do it
because I want to be healthier. So there's the deliberative self. It's made its little decision,
right? And then I just don't do it again and again and again, right? This is common stuff.
It seems to me then that my Pavlovian and my procedural systems are overriding my deliberative system. Is that a way to think of it?
It is. And one of the languages, this is, of course, the language of the previous book that
we talked about in 2015, which was called The Mind Within the Brain, is one of my view is that
it's not that you are succumbing to your Pavlovian and procedural self, it's that all three of those
are yourself and you have a conflict within yourself. Yes. And that seeing that conflict
within yourself as a conflict within yourself allows you to talk about controlling it. So one
is you could try to change your Pavlovian system or your procedural system. You could learn new
things. So, for example, you could learn to cook
differently. This actually happened to be my doctor said, I need to change my diet. And so I started
buying different foods and I learned a whole bunch of new recipes because I like to cook.
And so I'm cooking very differently for myself. And it's not that I'm like at every moment saying,
oh, am I eating too much sugar? Am I eating too much of this? It's more, oh, that sounds like a good dinner.
I'm going to make this dinner tonight.
Yeah.
Right.
And what I've done is I've changed my procedural process of cooking.
And yes, I've had to learn those new recipes to retrain my Pavlovian sense of like, oh,
that smells tasty.
You know, I like that flavor combination or I don't.
I've had a few that I was like, I'm not making that one again.
You know, by thinking of this as how do I change my world that interacts with all of
these systems?
Yeah.
I don't have to be white knuckling it through saying I'm going to do this, even though I
keep finding myself not right.
I'm going to do this even though I keep finding myself not, right?
And so by thinking about changing my environment, that allows me to change those actions basically so that the systems come into alignment rather than that they're always in conflict.
I really like this sort of discussion because I think you're right.
Recognizing the internal conflicts is really important. Because until you find some way of sort of resolving those, it's one step forward, one
step back, right?
I mean, it's just you're just kind of stuck in stasis, right?
Right.
The question almost in many cases, I think, becomes, how do I change what I want?
Or how do I change what parts of me want?
Or how do I change what parts of me want?
Or how do I change the environment so that those parts of me make the right choices?
Yes. Let me say something about that and then let's go a different level.
So retraining or changing the procedural system seems pretty straightforward, right? In that, like you said, I've got a habitual way of cooking and habitual things that I buy and
habitual, you know, reach for the cookies in the cabinet, all these habitual procedural systems.
And so I'm going to change those. I'm going to learn new procedures that are going to move me
in that direction. It seems like the Pavlovian one is the harder one to work with, to change. But it does seem possible because once
upon a time I had an extraordinarily pavlovian response to drugs and alcohol, you know, in that
like it felt instinctual. And now I don't, I don't at all. And so it is possible to do that.
It wasn't as simple as like, I just decided one day with my deliberative system, drugs
are bad.
No, in the beginning, that was part of it.
And there was some white knuckling.
But over time, some of it was environmental changes, but some of it feels deeper than
that.
In this example, what are ways that we can, if we've got a sort of a conflict between
our deliberative system and our Pavlovian system, and we've decided that our deliberative system is the one we want to go with, what are ways of sort of retraining that Pavlovian system? Or even if we don't want to use that word, teaching it some new dance steps?
It's really important because although we call it instinctual, we call it Pavlovian,
it is highly learning.
It is a learning system.
And it absolutely learns.
It learns whether you could release the friendly dog or the angry dog, right?
You can release the, I'm going to binge ice cream or I'm going to have, you know, a bowl of blueberries.
cream, or I'm going to have, you know, a bowl of blueberries. And part of it is creating situations where that instinctual system recognizes the approach to, and we're talking about approach
to food, but approach to food that is good for you rather than bad, right? So you get to the point where, you know, I've found combinations of
foods that I find really enjoyable, that actually, I'll be honest, I like better than some of those
super sweet things. I had a thing at the lab where I bought donuts, there was an extra donut,
and I ate the extra donut afterwards. And I was like, I didn't really that didn't,
afterwards. And I was like, I didn't really, that didn't, that wasn't so good. Right. And I realized that I have shifted my Pavlovian drive to like, there's only so much of this high sugar
that I really want. I mean, I liked the first donut. It was the second one that was too much.
Yes, of course. Always the second donut is a mistake.
Yes. But you can teach your Pavlovian system and you can think of it, you know, yeah,
you're talking about changing the foods.
Also, you can change what's available, right?
You can pre-commit by saying, I mean, people, you know, people who have alcohol problems pour the alcohol down the drain, right?
Yep. DoorDash has been a disaster for this, by the way.
I can imagine.
Because you used to be like, well, I just don't keep it in the house.
And now, you know, DoorDash, there's even alcohol delivery services.
It's like you don't even, I mean, I'm just happy to be where I am in life and not fighting some of those battles today.
Because I'm like, oh my goodness.
It's just like, you know, those pre-commitment devices aren't as effective.
Well, you need different ones.
And one of the things that you can do is you can create other pre-commitment devices, right?
One of my favorites is suicide hotlines and alcohol sponsors. And the line is,
you can go drinking after you've called your sponsor, right? And so I knew people who had
alcohol problems. What they taught themselves to say is, and they would literally say this,
they'd say, I'm going to the bar. I'm going to call my sponsor
and tell them I'm going to the bar. Now, to be honest, at the moment, did they really believe
that the sponsor would, you know, deliberatively, maybe they knew the sponsor would stop them.
Maybe instinctually, they didn't think they could talk the sponsor out of it.
Right. But a line that I have to be honest, I never would have believed was okay. But again,
I'm not a clinician. I want to be real clear on this. I'm a, I'm a computer jock who does basic
neuroscience, fundamental science questions. And, you know, but I've been hanging out with a lot of
clinicians recently. And my understanding is sometimes the right line is call the suicide hotline after you talk to them you can
kill yourself yeah and i i mean i would not have imagined that was the thing for saying but it
turns out that can get people to call the hotline right that can get people over that line whereas
saying oh my god you really do not kill yourself because, you know,
then you get the fight back. Yeah. Right. So there are these other procedures. And sometimes the
answer is the procedural system. Sometimes what you want to do is give yourself one of these
other systems and let your procedural system beat your deliberative system, right? So this is why I don't
like the old like, oh, deliberative is in control of everything. Because sometimes the best answer
is to create a situation, a process where other things happen. I heard a thing where it turns out
that blister packs are less dangerous than screw top bottles of pills.
Because by the time some people have popped out enough bubbles to be dangerous, they're like, this is dumb.
What am I doing?
Yeah.
I mean, delay is a tremendously effective strategy.
There's a rule in behavioralism, fairly simple one, which is very much procedural to some
degree. But it's, you know, if you want to do something, make it as easy as possible to do it.
Right. And if you don't want to do something, make it as hard as possible to do it. Exactly.
Just give yourself that time for your deliberative system to sort of get its wits about it and go,
hold on a second here. And to recognize that the instinctual system is a very in-the-moment immediate system.
Yes.
So if you can move things away from it, you can make it harder for that system.
If you're fighting your instinctual system about highly sugared foods, put the sugar foods under lock and key.
Yeah.
The other thing that's interesting about this is there are some theories out there around alcohol and around addiction in general. And one of the theories
is that it's a learning disorder, meaning that for most of us, you know, if you were to drink
like I used to drink, you would have some amount of consequence that your Pavlovian system would
very quickly be like, wait a second, this doesn't make sense. Like, yeah, that was nice, but holy mackerel, this really feels terrible.
And that in addicts, for some reason, that mechanism isn't working well.
Meaning the Pavlovian system is not getting the message that this is a bad deal.
Right.
Somehow it's just simply not internalizing, oh, this isn't good anymore.
Until it's so far past the point, you know,
the deliberative system can be very clear. It's like, you're killing yourself. This is insane.
But your Pavlovian system still thinks this is a good thing.
Actually, I've done a lot of work on addiction is actually how I got started on this whole process
was actually looking at addiction from new theoretical perspectives. And it turns out that addiction is a symptom,
not a disease, that it's more like fever. There are many, many things that will lead you to
addiction. And yes, your description of a Pavlovian system that gets over-associated
and connected is one of those. But there are other ones. For example, it turns out
for a lot of people, smoking is a procedural mistake. And it is that the nicotine turns out
to activate procedural systems, even when they're unpleasant. And that just has to do with the
neurophysiology of human brains. And other ones can occur through relief of anxiety questions. Or one of my
favorites was the deliberative idea that if I did this, I would be cool. And therefore, you know,
I would do well. Right. In fact, I've been doing a lot of work with anorexia, which is kind of
different from an addiction. Again, I'm not a clinician,
but I've been working with a lot of clinicians on this. And it looks like a lot of that problem
is actually a deliberative problem of knowing exactly what you want and being wrong about your
goals. And so people who think that if they are particularly thin, they will be more socially popular or they will,
you know, be able to avoid something. And then they do these really deliberative, dangerous plans
that they stick to. And the problem isn't that the deliberative system is weak or too strong.
It's that their understanding of the goal of the structure of the world. It turns out actually
being thin doesn't affect the popularity at all in this situation.
You know, many of them, if you actually look at the data, right, that they just misunderstanding.
You know, I always think of the, you know, smoking was cool when I was a kid and it doesn't make you cool anymore.
No.
Which has actually been a very nice change because now
people don't try to be cool by starting to take up cigarettes. It is a very good change. Well,
David, we are at the end of our time. Thank you so much for coming back on the show and for a
really interesting conversation about how we decide and how we choose how to act in the world.
Thank you for having me. It's been a tremendous fun.
It's great to be back.
I really love these conversations.
If what you just heard was helpful to you, please consider making a monthly donation to support the One You Feed podcast.
When you join our membership community with this monthly pledge, you get lots of exclusive members-only benefits.
It's our way of saying thank you for your support.
Now, we are so grateful for the members of our community.
We wouldn't be able to do what we do without their support,
and we don't take a single dollar for granted.
To learn more, make a donation at any level,
and become a member of the One You Feed community,
go to oneyoufeed.net slash join.
The One You Feed podcast would like to sincerely thank our sponsors
for supporting the show.
would like to sincerely thank our? We have the answer. Go to reallynoreally.com and register to win $500,
a guest spot on our podcast or a limited edition signed Jason bobblehead.
The really,
no really podcast.
Follow us on the I heart radio app,
Apple podcasts,
or wherever you get your podcasts.