The Peter Attia Drive - #127 - AMA #3 with sleep expert, Matthew Walker, Ph.D.: Fasting, gut health, blue light, caffeine, REM sleep, and more
Episode Date: September 7, 2020In this special episode, Matthew Walker returns for his third AMA episode to provide his expert insight into numerous sleep-related questions directly from listeners. He explains how he adjusted his ...hypotheses on topics like blue light and caffeine, and why he is more bullish on the importance of REM sleep. Matt also answers questions about sleep wearables, how fasting affects sleep, how sleep deprivation impacts gut health, and magnesium as a sleep aid. Finally, Matt reveals what he believes is the next evolution in sleep science and technology. If you’re a subscriber, you can now listen to this full episode on your private RSS feed or on our website on the show notes page. We discuss: Matt’s framework for changing his mind when faced with new information [1:30]; Blue light—How Matt shifted his thinking [5:45]; Caffeine—How Matt has adjusted his hypothesis [12:00]; REM sleep—Why Matt is more bullish on the importance of dream sleep [16:30]; How to increase REM sleep [27:30]; Sleep tracking wearables—criteria for evaluation, and why Matthew favors Oura [35:00]; Does the electromagnetic force of devices have any impact on sleep? [40:15]; The relationship between fasting and sleep [46:15]; Restless leg syndrome [58:10]; Magnesium supplementation as a sleep aid [1:03:00]; The relationship between sleep deprivation and gut health [1:08:30]; The next evolution in sleep science and technology [1:16:30]; Questions Matt would like to explore if money was no issue [1:24:15]; and More. Learn more: https://peterattiamd.com/ Show notes page for this episode: https://peterattiamd.com/matthewwalkerama3 Subscribe to receive exclusive subscriber-only content: https://peterattiamd.com/subscribe/ Sign up to receive Peter's email newsletter: https://peterattiamd.com/newsletter/ Connect with Peter on Facebook | Twitter | Instagram.
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hey everyone, welcome to a sneak peek, ask me anything, or AMA episode of the Drive Podcast.
I'm your host, Peter Atia.
At the end of this short episode, I'll explain how you can access the AMA episodes in full,
along with a ton of other membership benefits we've created, or you can learn more now by going to peteratia MD dot com forward slash
subscribe.
So without further delay, here's today's sneak peek of the Ask Me Anything episode.
Welcome to a special follow up, ask me anything interview with my guest Matthew Walker.
This is part of a two part interview, the first part being last week.
In this AMA episode, Matthew and I discuss the following.
What has Matthew changed his mind on, or what does he now believe to be true or untrue based
on emerging evidence?
And in particular, he brings up topics around blue light, caffeine, REM sleep, and sleep
wearables.
We also dive a little bit into the topic of sleep and electromagnetic force.
We discuss how fast and can impact sleep.
We talk about restless leg syndrome or RLS.
We talk about magnesium in particular as a sleep aid.
We talk about how sleep can impact the gut.
And we end with an interesting conversation around what Matthew believes is the next step
function evolution when it comes to sleep. Just as a quick reminder of Matt's credentials,
he's a professor at UC Berkeley in the Department of Neuroscience and Psychology and he's the founding
director for the Center of Human Sleep Science. He's trained in the UK and he is the author of the International
Best Seller Why We Sleep. So without further delay, please enjoy this special AMA
with Matthew Walker.
So Matt, I don't know a few months ago, Bob Kaplan and I, I think it was actually for
our 100th episode, did a special AMA
that was titled Something Along the Lines of Strong Convictions Lusely Held or High Convictions
Lusely Held.
Strong belief, Lusely Held.
Yeah, strong belief, Lusely Held.
All basically a flavor of the same theme, which is, what are things that you believe with
great conviction, but you're willing to hold them loosely enough
and in the presence of new information you've changed your mind.
And I can ask this question a lot.
In fact, I find it one of the most enjoyable questions people ask me, which is Peter,
what do you believe today that you did not believe five years ago?
And conversely, what did you believe five years ago that you categorically did not believe
today?
So let's start with either of those, take either one or feel free to not even separate them.
But basically, where is your belief system today, Matt, discordant from where it was,
I don't know, four or five years ago, when you were saying the throws of writing your book,
which is probably the most you thought about this problem.
The way I think about this as a scientist, not from this question perspective, but I usually
have sort of three buckets in my mind, evidence that's coming in, that's helping reinforce
a belief that I have. So I'm more bullish, things that I believed that now the evidence
has added to, which means that I haven't changed my mind on the original belief, but I've
had to add a new construct in that equation of the belief.
And then the third one is contradiction, where I've got now evidence against that hypothesis
that I once believed there was evidence to support it.
And now I'm rejecting that hypothesis.
So it's either getting stronger, more nuanced,
or you're moving towards rejecting.
Correct, yeah.
So I'm now trying to think of examples
in maybe all of those three.
Well, you're thinking of that, Matt.
Can I just give you a minute to think about that,
but also an opportunity to go on one of my favorite
rants about science, which is sort of a pet peeve of mine
is the way that science
has been represented in the press as anything but probabilistic.
And one of the problems with the world we live in is most people don't have the opportunity
to be educated scientifically.
And therefore they, especially in a manner where it's experimental.
Not that there's anything wrong with disciplines of science that don't have as much experimentation,
but if you're privileged enough to get to cut your teeth a little bit in an experimental
discipline of science, you start to realize that it's really all probabilistic.
I mean, data are very messy, and therefore the best you can do when an experiment or many
experiments are concluded
is increase your confidence in the probability of something being true or being false.
And I remember a mentor of mine once explaining that because I had a background in mathematics,
he said, look, you just have to get used to the fact that there's no proving anything
in science.
Your last proof was done when you left the faculty
in mathematics.
You are now going to spend the rest of your life
looking at high, high, very high low intermediate
probability events, but the days of this is proved,
or this is disproved, are really over.
And so, one, I hope that little soliloquy
is helpful for folks to understand how it is
that in science, you can walk back from things you believed or add nuance to things you believe or walk forward on things.
And hopefully, if you could drone out my voice, as I said that, Matt, it also gave you time to think of that, because I know that's a tough question to get asked.
to get asked. I didn't tune out. I love what you're saying. I'm very actually envious of mathematicians because I think it is the only deterministic discipline where you, if one to prove is a proof,
it's proof for the most part forever. Whereas with science, all we ever really do is hopefully
disprove what we think it isn't and we're never certain about what it is. But we're getting a little
bit more certain, hopefully, or less certain, but we can never prove anything what it is, but we're getting a little bit more certain, hopefully,
or less certain, but we can never prove anything
like a one or a zero with mathematics.
So with that said, it's not necessarily that hard
of a question because I'm constantly trying to run
these calculations about all of my scientific sleep beliefs
and think about these three buckets.
I think the thing that I've probably changed my mind
on most or had a reversal on
is the effects of blue light on our sleep. And in fact, in the book, I spoke about a study at the
time that had been done out of Harvard, which I still think is valid, where they'd used an iPad,
where you read on an iPad for an hour versus you read a book under dim light. And they show that
that iPad had this detrimental effect
on sleep. It had delayed the release of melatonin. It had caused a reduction in REM sleep.
And even when they stopped reading the iPad, it had a blast radius to it where the sleep quality
was still bad for a couple of days afterwards. And it was a compelling study in publishing a
good journal. But over the years, I think there's been some
research that's pushed back on that, and there's been some great work from a university in Australia
called Flinders University. And Michael Gratazar has done some just great work on this at Flinders.
He has changed my mind. I'm less bullish now about the idea that these devices that we use are sleep disruptive because
of the blue light, I still think that has an effect.
But what I think he's shown in some elegant work is that it's less about the light, it's
more about the fact that these devices are just so activating.
That these devices are designed to trigger alertness and what we call physiological arousal in the brain.
And in other words, what happens when we use these devices,
the reason that they're so disruptive to our sleep,
is less about their blue light.
It's more the fact that we are masking our sleepiness
with this overriding artificial activation from the devices. In other words, let's say
that all of a sudden it's 10pm and you think, and wide awake, I'm on my computer, I'm
working, I've got my phone next to me, I'm checking it, it's pinging, it's dinging, all
of a sudden all of the lights go out, there's a massive electromagnetic pulse that curses
across your environment. It knocks out all of the devices. You've got no phone, no iPad, no electricity. I suspect that within about 15 or 20 minutes you'd start
to feel sleepy. And it's not because of the blue light effects. It's the fact that you are,
and you were all along, sleepy, but these devices, because they're so activating, was creating a competing force that hit the mute button on the sleepiness, and it activated you.
So I've actually down-regulated my belief in the effects of blue light, and I've introduced this new mental framework regarding the effects of the invasion of technology into our evening lives and our bedrooms. And I'm much more now enamored
with this idea that they are mentally stimulating, rather than blue light emitting.
Forgive my ignorance for this question, but has the experiment not been done where you've taken
groups of subjects and you've subjected one group to just a blue light? So an actual blue light
that's hitting, I forget how many nanometers that
is, but the actual right in the short of that wavelength. Yeah. Yeah. And then you have
another group that is just being blasted with red light. And then you have another group
that is just being blasted with a regular LED and white light. So you're getting the
same intensity of light, but you're moving the wavelength, and therefore you're nullifying the
stimulatory effect of what's being read or looked at.
I mean, to me, that experiment would eloquently demonstrate whether or not blue light per
say is the problem.
Has that not been done?
It has.
So people have played around with the wavelength of the light.
And what we believe is that it's the cooler blues, the shorter wavelength light that are
most detrimental and the reason that screens were blamed is because they are LED
based, which is enriched in the lower visible light spectrum, the shorter wavelength, in other
words, the cool blues. And that's why the blame came because it was stamping the brakes on melatonin,
especially powerfully. And those studies were done, they were done by Chuck Seisler and Steve Lockley from Harvard years ago and that led to this sort of belief. And I still think there's good
validity in that. And by the way, there was a couple of studies that came out in animals that
were now suggesting at least I think it was in rats or mice, I could be wrong, or if it was in fruit
flies, I apologise, where they actually found the opposite opposite where they found that the warmer colour lights had stronger blocking effects on melatonin and it began this sort of controversy
had we got it wrong about blue light.
And then this work from Flinders University from Michael was coming online regarding this
cognitive component and it really sort of made me shift my belief system.
So yes, those studies have been done and
they principally looked at melatonin. I think they studied less, a full night of sleep
with polysomnography and really asked the downstream consequences. They were just simply
saying, how does it affect your melatonin, which is maybe one step short of saying, how
does it then affect as a consequence of that change in melatonin, your subsequent sleep,
without necessarily doing a much more sophisticated study, which I think are now being done, where
you do the Coke Pepsi challenge of same amount of light stimulation.
So light is standardized.
Right.
Light becomes the variable.
That's the only independent variable.
That's right. And then you start measuring
independent variable at polysum and melatonin for what it's worth and do everything.
And then you do a second round of studies where light actually becomes the constant stimulus where
you maintain the same light exposure, but in one condition, you're doing something cognitively
activating, like building a Facebook account or checking that, versus
you're simply just there in front of the blue light. But there's no cognitive stimulation
to really do the two-by-two disambiguation of that. I think those studies are coming. But
that's one of the things where I've definitely changed my mind, I think, and I felt compelled to
now speak more about that and less about the
blue light. The other place where I think I've changed my mind and maybe even some of my
behavior is around coffee and caffeine. Thank you for listening to today's Sneak Peak
AMA episode of The Drive. If you're interested in hearing the complete version of this AMA,
you'll want to become a member. We created a membership program to bring you more in-depth exclusive content without relying on paid ads.
Membership benefits are many, and beyond the complete episodes of the AMA each month, they include the following.
Redeculously comprehensive podcast show notes that detail every topic, paper, person, and thing we discuss on each episode of the drive.
Access to our private podcast feed. The qualities which were a super short podcast typically
less than five minutes released every Tuesday through Friday, which highlight the best
questions, topics, and tactics discussed on previous episodes of the drive. This particularly
important for those of you who haven't heard all of the backup episodes, it becomes a great way to go back and filter and decide which ones you want to listen to
in detail.
Really steep discount codes for products I use and believe in, but for which I don't get
paid to endorse, and benefits that we continue to add over time.
If you want to learn more and access these member-only benefits, head over to peteratia-md.com
forward slash subscribe.
Lastly, if you're already a member but you're hearing this, it means you haven't downloaded
our member-only podcast feed where you can get the full access to the AMA and you don't
have to listen to this.
You can download that at peteratia-md.com forward slash members.
You can find me on Twitter, Instagram, and Facebook, all with the
ID, Peter Atia MD. You can also leave us a review on Apple podcasts or whatever
podcast player you listen on. This podcast is for general informational purposes
only. It does not constitute the practice of medicine, nursing, or other
professional health care services, including the giving of medical advice.
No doctor-patient relationship is formed.
The use of this information and the materials linked to this podcast is at the user's own
risk.
The content on this podcast is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical
advice, diagnosis, or treatment.
Users should not disregard or delay in obtaining medical advice from any medical condition they have,
and they should seek the assistance of their healthcare professionals for any such conditions.
Finally, I take conflicts of interest very seriously. For all of my disclosures in the companies I
invest in or advise, please visit peteratiamd.com forward slash about where I keep an up to date and active list of such companies. you