The Peter Attia Drive - #270 ‒ Journal club with Andrew Huberman: metformin as a geroprotective drug, the power of belief, and how to read scientific papers

Episode Date: September 11, 2023

View the Show Notes Page for This Episode Become a Member to Receive Exclusive Content Sign Up to Receive Peter’s Weekly Newsletter Andrew Huberman, Professor of Neurobiology at Stanford Universi...ty and host of the Huberman Lab podcast joins us in a special journal club episode. Peter and Andrew each present a recent paper that sparked their interests, delving into the findings, dissecting their significance, discussing potential confounders and limitations, and exploring remaining questions. Importantly, they share their methodologies for comprehending research studies, providing valuable insights for listeners to navigate this process independently. Peter presents an epidemiological study reevaluating a noteworthy metformin result that intrigued the anti-aging community, leading to discussions on metformin's geroprotective potential (or lack thereof) and the current lack of aging biomarkers. Andrew introduces a paper examining how our beliefs about the medications we take influence their biological effects, distinguishing the "belief effect" from a placebo effect and highlighting its exciting implications for the future. We discuss: The motivation behind this journal club conversation [2:45]; Why Peter chose a paper on metformin, how metformin works, and why it generated excitement as a longevity-enhancing agent [9:00]; Defining insulin resistance and its underlying causes [16:15]; Metformin as a first-line treatment for type 2 diabetes, and Peter’s evolving interest in metformin as a geroprotective drug [22:00]; Defining the term “geroprotection” [24:45]; The 2014 study that got the anti-aging community interested in metformin [26:00]; Peter presents the 2022 paper that repeats the analytical approach from the 2014 Bannister study [33:15]; Greater mortality in the metformin group: how results differed between the 2022 paper and the 2014 paper [40:00]; Understanding statistical significance, statistical power, sample size, and why epidemiology uses enormous cohorts [51:45]; Interpreting the hazard ratios from the 2022 metformin study, and the notable takeaways from the study [56:45]; Drugs that may extend lifespan, why Peter stopped taking metformin, and a discussion of caloric restriction [1:08:45]; Current thoughts on the use of metformin for longevity [1:21:00]; Could there be any longevity benefit to short periods of caloric restriction? [1:22:45]; Peter and Andrew’s process for reading scientific papers [1:26:45]; The biological effects of belief, and how “belief effects” differ from placebo effects [1:32:30]; The neurobiology of nicotine: a precursor conversation before delving into the paper Andrew chose [1:39:45]; Andrew presents a paper that demonstrates the impact of belief [1:45:30]; Analyzing the fascinating results of the Perl paper [1:54:30]; Exciting implications of the findings about “belief” reported by Perl and colleagues [2:03:15]; and More. Connect With Peter on Twitter, Instagram, Facebook and YouTube

Transcript
Discussion (0)
Starting point is 00:00:00 Hey everyone, welcome to the Drive Podcast. I'm your host Peter Atia. This podcast, my website, and my weekly newsletter all focus on the goal of translating the science of longevity into something accessible for everyone. Our goal is to provide the best content in health and wellness, and we've established a great team of analysts to make this happen. It is extremely important to me to provide all of this content without relying on paid ads. To do this, our work is made entirely possible by our members, and in return, we offer exclusive member-only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free.
Starting point is 00:00:46 If you want to take your knowledge of this space to the next level, it's our goal to ensure members get back much more than the price of a subscription. If you want to learn more about the benefits of our premium membership, head over to PeterAtiaMD.com forward slash subscribe. Welcome to a special episode of the drive. This episode is actually a dual episode with Andrew Huberman, where we are going to be releasing our conversation on both the Huberman lab podcast and on the drive.
Starting point is 00:01:15 In this episode, Andrew and I have a journal club where we each present and talk through a paper that we have found interesting in the previous couple of months. Now, I hope this will help people not only understand the results of the specific papers we go through, which is part of the exercise, but also to give people an idea of how to read and interpret a paper that you might read. And really, in some ways, I think that's equally if not more important as part of this exercise. For my paper, we looked at a study on Metformin by Keys et al, which looked back at the 2014 study by Bannister et al, that initially got everyone really interested in Metformin as a possible zero protective molecule.
Starting point is 00:01:53 Through looking at this paper, we discussed Metformin as a possible zero protective drug, but also had a general discussion around zero protection in the current lack of biomarkers of aging. Andrew then presented a paper that addressed how our beliefs of the drug we take impacts the effect they have on us at a biological level. So not looking at placebo effects, but actual belief effects in what this could mean going forward. As a reminder, Andrew is an associate professor of neurobiology at the Stanford University School of Medicine and the host of the very popular Huberman Lab podcast. He's also a former podcast guest on
Starting point is 00:02:30 episode 249. So without further delay, please enjoy my conversation with Andrew Huberman. Peter, so good to have you here. So great to be here, my friend. This is something that you and I have been wanting to do for a while. And it's basically something that we do all the time, which is to peruse the literature and find papers that we are excited about for whatever reason. And oftentimes that will lead to a text dialogue or a phone call or both. But this time we've opted to try talking about these papers that we find particularly exciting in real time
Starting point is 00:03:12 for the first time as this podcast format. First of all, so that people can get some sense of why we're so excited about these papers, we do feel that people should know about these findings. And second of all, that it's an opportunity for people to learn how to dissect information and think about the papers they hear about in the news, the papers they might download from PubMed, if they're inclined. Also, just to start thinking like scientists and clinicians and get a better
Starting point is 00:03:38 sense of what it looks like to pick through a paper, the good, the bad, and the ugly. So we're flying a little blind here, which is fun. I'm definitely excited for all the above reasons. Yeah, no, this is you and I've been talking about this for some time. And you know, actually, we used to run a journal club inside the practice where once a month, one person would just pick a paper and you would go through it in kind of a formal journal club presentation.
Starting point is 00:04:08 We got in a way from it for the last year just because we've been a little stretched then. I think it's something we need to resume because it's a great way to learn. And it's a skill. People probably ask you all the time because I know I get asked all the time, hey, what are the do's and don'ts
Starting point is 00:04:24 of interpreting scientific papers? Is it enough to just read the abstract? And then, you know, usually the answer is, well, no. But the how-to is tougher. And I think the two papers we've chosen today illustrate two oppositions of the spectrum. You know, you're going to obviously talk about something that we're going to probably get
Starting point is 00:04:41 into the technical nature of the assays, the limitations, et cetera. And the paper, ultimately, I've chosen to present, although I apologize, I'm surprising you with this up until a few minutes ago, is actually a very straightforward, simple epidemiologic paper that I think has important significance. I had originally gone down the rabbit hole on a much more nuanced paper about ATP binding cassettes in cholesterol absorption, but ultimately I thought this one might be more interesting to a broader audience. By the way, I got to tell you a funny story.
Starting point is 00:05:09 So I had a dream last night about you. And in this dream, you were obsessed with making this certain drink that was like your elixir. And it had all of these crazy ingredients in it. Suck, tons of supplements in it. But the one thing I remembered when I woke up, because I forgot most of them, I was really trying so hard to remember them.
Starting point is 00:05:30 One thing that you had in it was due. You had to collect a certain amount of due off the leaves every morning to put into this drink. It was just sounds like something that I would do. And so, but here's the best part. You had, you had like a thermos of this stuff that had to be with you everywhere. And all of your clothing had to be tailored with a special pocket that you could put the thermos into so that you were never without the special Andrew drink.
Starting point is 00:06:00 And again, you know how dreams when you're having them seem so logical and real and then you wake up and you're like That doesn't even make sense like why would he want the thermos in his shirt? Like that I would warm it up like you know all these but but boy It was a realistic dream and there were lots of things in it including do Spass special do off the leaves every morning. I love it. Well, it's not that far from reality. I'm a big fan of yerba mate. I'm drinking it right now, in fact. That's my. In its many forms, usually the loose leaf.
Starting point is 00:06:33 I don't tend to drink it out of the gourd. My dad's Argentine, so that's where I picked it up. I started drinking it when I was like five years old or younger, which I don't recommend people do. It's heavily caffeinated. Don't drink the smoked versions either, folks. I think that was potentially carcinogenic. But this, I mean, you describe of caring around the thermos close to the body, if you are ever in Uruguay, or if you ever spot grown men
Starting point is 00:06:55 in a restaurant anywhere in the world, caring of thermos with them and to their meals and hugging it close, chances are they're Uruguay. And they're drinking your bimote. They drink it usually after their meals. It's supposed to be good for your digestion. So it's not that far from reality. I don't carry the thermos, but I do drink mate every day. And I'm going to start collecting dew off the leaves. Just a few drops every morning.
Starting point is 00:07:20 It's just over. Oh my. Some other time we can talk about dreams. Recently, I've been doing some dream exploration. I've had some absolutely transformative dreams for the first time in my life. One dream in particular that has, that allowed me to feel something I've never felt before and has catalyzed a large number of important decisions in a way that no other experience waking or sleep has ever impacted me.
Starting point is 00:07:46 And this was drug-free, et cetera. And do you think you could have had that dream? We don't have to get into it if you don't want to talk about it now, but was there a lot of work you had to do to prepare for that dream to have taken place? Oh, yes. Yeah. At least 18 months of intensive analysis type work with a very skilled psychiatrist.
Starting point is 00:08:09 But I wasn't trying to seed the dream. Yeah, yeah. It was just I was at a sticking point with a certain process in my life. And then I was taking a walk while waking and realized that my brain, my subconscious, was going to keep working on this. I just decided it's going to keep working on it.
Starting point is 00:08:28 And then two nights later, I traveled to a meeting in Aspen, and I had the most profound dream ever where I was able to sense something and feel something I've always wanted to feel as so real within the dream. Woke up, knew it was a dream dream and realized this is what people close to me that I respect have been talking about, but I was able to feel it and therefore I can actually access this in my waking life. It was absolutely transformative for me. Anyway, sometimes I can share more details with you or the audience, but for now, we should talk about these papers very well. Who should go first?
Starting point is 00:09:12 I'm happy to go first. This one is this one's this is a pretty straightforward paper. So so we're going to talk about a paper titled reassessing the evidence of a survival advantage in type 2 diabetics treated with metformin compared with controls without diabetes, a retrospective cohort study. This is by Matthew, Thomas, Keys, and colleagues. This was published last fall. Why is this paper important? So this paper is important because in 2014, Bannister published a paper that, I think in many ways, kind of got the world very excited about
Starting point is 00:09:46 Metformin. So this is almost 10 years ago. And I'm sure many people have heard about this paper, even if they're not familiar with it, but they've heard the concept of the paper. And in many ways, it's the paper that has led to the excitement around the potential for zero protection with Metformin. And I should probably just define for the audience what zero protection means. When we think of- Probably also, sorry to interrupt what Metformin is just for the uninformed. That's a great point.
Starting point is 00:10:10 So I'll start with the latter. So Metformin is a drug that has been used for many years, depends where it was first approved, I think was in Europe. But call it directionally 50 plus years of use as a first-line agent for patients with type 2 diabetes. In the US, maybe 40 plus years. So this is a drug that's been around forever, trade name, Glucophage, or brand name, but again, it's a generic drug today. But again, it's a generic drug today. The mechanism by which metformin works is debated,
Starting point is 00:10:47 hotly, but what I think is not debated is the immediate thing that metformin does, which is it inhibits complex one of the mitochondria. So again, maybe just taking a step back. So the mitochondria, as everybody thinks of those, is the cellular engine for making ATP. So the most efficient way that we make ATP is through oxidative phosphorylation, where we take either fatty acid pieces or a breakdown product of glucose
Starting point is 00:11:15 once it's partially metabolized to pyruvate. We put that into an electron transport chain. And we basically trade chemical energy for electrons that can then be used to make phosphates onto ADP. So you think of everything you do. Eating is taking the chemical energy and food, taking the energy that's in those bonds, making electrical energy in the mitochondria, those electrons pump a gradient that allow you to make ATP. To give a sense of how primal and important this is, if you block that process completely, you die. Everybody's probably heard of cyanide.
Starting point is 00:11:52 Cyanide is something that is incredibly toxic, even at the smallest doses. Cyanide is a complete blocker of this process. If my memory serves me correctly, I think it blocks complex four of the mitochondria. I don't know if you recall if it complex three or complex four. I know a lot about toxins that impact the nervous system, but I don't know a lot about the ways that it might have come. But if ever you want to have some fun, we can talk about all the dangerous stuff that animals make and insects make and how they kill you.
Starting point is 00:12:17 Yeah, like the tropotoxin and all these things that block sodium and natural toxins and bone growth. I really geek out on this stuff because it allows me to talk about neuroscience, animals, and scary stuff. It's like, combines it. So we could do that sometime for fun. Maybe at the end, if we have a few moments. So, you know, something like cyanide that is a very potent inhibitor of this electron transport chain will kill you instantly. People understand that, of course, a drop of cyanide and you would be dead literally instantaneously. So Metformin works at the first of those complexes, I believe there are four of my memory serves correctly, four electron transport chain complexes.
Starting point is 00:12:52 And, but of course, it's not a complete inhibition of it. It's just kind of a weak blocker of that. And the net effect of that is what? So the net effect of that is that it changes the ratio of adenosine monophosphate to adenosine diphosphate. What's less clear is why does that have a benefit in diabetics? Because what it unambiguously does is reduces the amount of glucose that the liver puts out. So hepatic glucose output is one of the fundamental problems that's happening in type 2 diabetes. You may recall, I think we talked about this even on previous podcast.
Starting point is 00:13:28 You and I sitting here with normal blood sugar have about five grams of glucose in our total circulation. That's it, five grams. Think about how quickly the brain will go through that within minutes. So the only thing that keeps us alive is our liver's ability to titrate out glucose. If it puts out too much, for example, if the glucose level was consistently two teaspoons,
Starting point is 00:13:52 you would have type 2 diabetes. The difference between being metabolically healthy and having profound type 2 diabetes is one teaspoon of glucose in your bloodstream. The ability of the liver to tamp down on high glucose output is important. Metformin seems to do that. So can I just ask, oh, one question. Is it fair to provide this overly simplified summary of the biochemistry, which is that when we eat, the food is broken down, but the breaking of bonds creates energy that then ourselves
Starting point is 00:14:24 can use in the form of ATP. And the breaking of bonds creates energy that then ourselves can use in the form of ATP. And they might have conduit our central of that process. And that metformin is partially short circuiting the energy production process. And so, even though we are eating, when we have metformin in our system, presumably, there is going to be less net glucose. The bonds are going to be broken down. We're chewing, we're digesting, but less of that is turned into blood sugar glucose. Well, sort of. I mean, it's not, it's not depriving you of ultimately storing that energy. What it's doing is changing the way the body partitions fuel. That's probably a better way to think about it to be a little bit more accurate. So, for example, like it's not depriving you of the calories that are in that glucose.
Starting point is 00:15:11 That would be, you know, fantastic. But that was the, that was the cholesterol. That's right. You'll have to throw the cholesterol from the 90s. Alestra folks, for those of you who don't remember, by the way, if you ever ate this stuff, you'd remember, because it was a fat that was not easily digested, it had sort of analogous to plant fiber or something like that. So it was being put into potato chips and whatnot. And the idea is that people would simply excrete it. And I don't know what happened,
Starting point is 00:15:42 except that people got lost stomach aches. And well, the anal seepage. The anal seepage. We know that. The anal seepage is what happened except that people got lost stomach aches and well the anal seepage in the world. We know that the anal seepage is what really did that product. The only seepage only a physician because after all Peter's a clinician for physician and MD and I'm not could find it a an appropriate term to describe. Yeah, when you have that much when you have that much fat malabsorption,
Starting point is 00:16:06 you start to have accidents. Wow. And so that did away with that product. Right. It was either that or the diaper industry was going to really take on. Okay. That's why you don't hear about a less drug.
Starting point is 00:16:16 That's right. So we've got this drug. We've got this drug metformin. It's considered a perfect first line agent for people with type two diabetes. So again, what's happening when you have type 2 diabetes? The primary insult probably occurs in the muscles and it is insulin resistance. Everybody hears that term.
Starting point is 00:16:34 What does it mean? Insulin is a peptide. It binds to a receptor on a cell. So let's just talk about it through the lens of the muscle because the muscle is responsible for most glucose disposal. It gets glucose out of the circulation. High glucose is toxic, we have to put it away and we want to put most of it into our muscles. That's where we store 75 to 80 percent of it. When insulin binds to the insulin receptor, tyrosine kinase is triggered inside. So just ignore all that, but a chemical
Starting point is 00:17:01 reaction takes place inside the cell that leads to a phosphorylation, so ATP donates a phosphate group, and a transporter, just think of like a little tunnel, like a little straw, goes up through the level of the cell, and now glucose can freely flow in. So I'm sure you've talked a lot about this with your audience. Things that move against gradients need pumps to move them, things that move with gradients don't. Glucose is moving with its gradient into the cell, it doesn't need active transport, but it does need the transportor put there. That requires the energy. That's the job of insulin. By the way, I did not know that. I mean, I certainly know active and passive transport as it relates to neurotransmitter and ion flow.
Starting point is 00:17:45 But I'd never heard that when insulin binds to a cell that literally a little straw is placed into the membrane of the cell. Yeah. The glucose doesn't need a pump to move it in because there's much more glucose outside the cell than inside. But the energy required is to move the straw up to the cell. So biology is so cool. Yeah, it is. So what happens is as and Gerald
Starting point is 00:18:08 Schoelman at Yale did the best work on elucidating this as the intramuscular fat increases. And I intramuscular, I mean intracelular fat, triacyl and diacylglycerides accumulate in a muscle cell, that signal gets interrupted. And all of a sudden, I'm making these numbers up. If you used to need two units of insulin to trigger the little transporter now, you need three, and then you need four, and then you need five. You need more and more insulin to get the thing up. That is the definition of insulin resistance.
Starting point is 00:18:43 The cell is becoming resistant to the effect of insulin and therefore The early mark of insulin resistance the canary in the coal mine is not an increase in glucose. It's an increase in insulin so Normal glycemia with hyperinsulinemia, especially post-prandial meaning after you eat hyperinsulinemia Is the thing that tells you, hey, you're five, 10 years away from this being a real problem. So fast forward many steps down the line, someone with type 2 diabetes has long passed that system. Now, not only are they insulin resistant where they just need a boatload of insulin, which is made by the pancreas, to get glucose
Starting point is 00:19:21 out of the circulation. But now that system's not even working well. And now they're not getting glucose into the cell. So now their glucose level is elevated. And even though it's continually being chewed up and used up, because again, the brain alone would account for most of that glucose disposal, the liver is now becoming insulin resistant as well. And now the liver isn't able to regulate how much glucose to put into circulation, and it's overdoing it. So now you have too much glucose being pumped
Starting point is 00:19:49 into the circulation by the liver, and you have the muscles that can't dispose of it. And it's really a vicious brutal cascade, because the same problem of fat accumulating in the muscle is now starting to happen in the pancreas. And now the relatively few cells in the pancreas called beta cells that make insulin are undergoing inflammation due to the fat accumulation
Starting point is 00:20:10 within the pancreas itself. And so now the thing that you need to make more insulin is less effective at making insulin. So ultimately way, way, way down the line, a person with type two diabetes might actually even require insulin exogenously. Could you share with us a few of the causes of type 2 diabetes of insulin resistance? I mean, one, it sounds like is accumulating too much fat.
Starting point is 00:20:31 Yeah, so energy imbalance would be an enormous one. Inactivity or insufficient activity is probably the single most important. So when Gerald Schulman was running clinical trials at Yale, they would be recruiting undergrads to study, obviously, because you're typically recruiting young people. And they would, you know, be doing these very detailed mechanistic studies where they would require actual tissue biopsies. So, you know, you're going to biopsy somebody's quadriceps and actually look at what's happening in the muscle. Well, I remember him telling me this when I interviewed him on my podcast. He said, the most important criteria of the people we interviewed is that because they were still lean,
Starting point is 00:21:06 these weren't people that were overweight, but they had to be inactive. You couldn't have active people in these studies. So exercising is one of the most important things you're going to do to ward off insulin resistance. But there are other things that can cause insulin resistance, sleep deprivation has a profound impact on insulin resistance. I think we probably talked about this previously, but if you notice some very elegant
Starting point is 00:21:27 mechanistic studies where you sleep deprived people, you let them only sleep for four hours for a week, you'll reduce their glucose disposal by about half, which is, I mean, that's a staggering amount of. You're basically inducing profound insulin resistance in just a week of sleep deprivation. Hypercortisolemia is another factor, and then obviously energy imbalance. So when you're accumulating excess energy, when you're getting fatter, if you start spilling that fat outside of the subcutaneous fat cells into the muscle, into the liver, into the pancreas, all those things are exacerbating it.
Starting point is 00:21:59 Got it. Okay. So, enter metformin, first line drug. So every drug you give a person with type 2 diabetes is trying to address part of this chain. So some of the drugs tell you to make more insulin. That's one of the strategies. So here are drugs like sulfonia rias. They tell the body, make more insulin.
Starting point is 00:22:20 Other drugs like insulin just give you more of the insulin thing. Metformin tackles the problem elsewhere. It tamps down glucose by addressing the glucose, the hepatic glucose output channel. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also make more insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin. GLP1 agonist or another drug, they increase insulin sensitivity, initially causing you to also That's ozemic. Yes. Yeah. And is it true that burberry is more or less the poor man's metformin? Yeah. Yeah. It's a from a tree bark. It just happens to have the same properties of yeah. And by the way, reducing mTOR and reducing blood glucose. Yeah. And metformin, by the way, occurs from a lilac plant in France, like that's where it was discovered. So it's also,
Starting point is 00:22:59 metformin is also based on a substance found in nature. So you need a prescription for metformin. You don't need a prescription for burberry. Correct. But we can talk about burberry a little bit later. I had a couple great experiences with burberry and then a couple bad experiences. Interesting. Burberry. Yeah.
Starting point is 00:23:16 So, maybe taking one step back from this. In 2011, I became very interested in metform and personally just reading about it, obsessing over it, and just somehow decided like, I should be taking this. I actually began taking Metformin. I still remember exactly when I started. I started it in May of 2011, and I realized that, because I was on a trip with a bunch of buddies, we went to the Berkshire Hathaway Shareholder meeting,
Starting point is 00:23:41 which is the Buffett shareholder meeting. And it's kind of like a fun thing to do. And I remember being so sick the whole time because I didn't titrate up the dose of Metformin. I just went straight to two grams a day, which is kind of like the full dose. And we went to this. Is that characteristic of your approach to things?
Starting point is 00:24:01 Yes, I think that's safe to say. Next time I'll give you a thermos of this do that I collect in the morning. Oh, you agree that. So I remember being so sick that the whole time we were in Nebraska or Omaha, I guess, I couldn't, we went to Dairy Queen because you do all the Buffet things when you're there, right? Like I couldn't have an ice cream at Dairy Queen. You couldn't, I mean, I couldn't. I'm so nauseous. Oh, because I would say, if you've got metformin in your system, you're gonna buffered glucose. You get a four-eyes cream cone. It's a bad way to get out. Except I couldn't put, I couldn't. I'm so nauseous. Oh, because I would say if you've got metformin in your system, you're going to buffer glucose. You get four ice cream cones in your body. It's that I couldn't put. I couldn't keep anything down. I mean, it was so nauseous. So, so clearly metformin has this side effect initially, which is a little
Starting point is 00:24:34 bit of appetite suppression. But regardless, that's the story on metformin. There were a lot of reasons I was interested in it. I wasn't thinking true zero protection. That term wasn't in my vernacular at the time, but what I was thinking is, hey, this is gonna help you buffer glucose better. It's gotta be better. And this was sort of my first foray into self experimentation. Do you wanna define zero protection?
Starting point is 00:24:55 Yeah, yeah. It's a good term to define. Georeiatric, zero. Yeah, so, yeah, zero from, from geriatric old protection. So protection from aging. And when we talk about a drug like Metformin or RAPA-MISON or even NADNR, these things, the idea is we're talking about them as Giroprotective to signal that they are drugs that are not targeting a specific disease of aging.
Starting point is 00:25:20 For example, a PCSK9 inhibitor is sort of zero protective, but it's targeting one specific pathway, which is cardiovascular disease and dyslipidemia. Whereas the idea is a zero protective agent would target hallmarks of aging. There are nine hallmarks of aging, please don't ask me to recite them. I've never been able to get all nine straight, but people know what we're talking about, right?
Starting point is 00:25:42 So decreased etythology, increased senescence, decreased nutrient sensing or defective nutrient sensing, proteomic instability, genomic instability, methylation, all of these things, epigenetic changes. Those are all about nine hallmarks of aging. So a zero protective agent would target those deep down biological marks of aging. And in 2014, a paper came out by Bannister
Starting point is 00:26:08 that basically got the world focused on this problem, by the world, I mean, the world of anti-agent. So what Bannister and colleagues did was they took a registry from the UK and they got a set of patients who were on metformin with type 2 diabetes, but only metformin. So these were people who had just progressed to diabetes. They were not put on any other drug, just metformin. And then they found from the same registry a group of matched controls.
Starting point is 00:26:40 So this is a standard way that epidemiologic studies are done because again, you don't have the luxury of doing the randomization. So you're trying to account for all the biases that could exist by saying we're gonna take people who look just like that person with diabetes. So can we match them for age, sex, socioeconomic status, blood pressure, BMI, everything we can. And then let's look at what happened to them over time. Now again, this is all happening in the future, so you're looking into the past.
Starting point is 00:27:14 It's retrospective in that sense. And so let me just kind of pull up the sort of table here so I can kind of walk through. And this is not in the paper we talked about, but I think this is an important background. So they did something that at the time I didn't really notice. I didn't notice what they did. I probably did and I forgot, but I didn't notice this until about five years ago when I went back and looked at the paper. And they did something called informative censoring. So the way the study worked is, if you were put on metformin, we're going to follow you. If you're not on metformin, we're going to follow you.
Starting point is 00:27:51 And we're going to track the number of deaths from any cause that occurred. This is called all cause mortality or ACM. And it's really the gold standard in a trial of this nature or a study of this nature or even a clinical trial. You want to know how much are people dying from anything? Because we're trying to prevent or delay death of all causes. Informative censoring says, if a person who's on Metformin deviates from that inclusion criteria,
Starting point is 00:28:18 we will not count them in the final assessment. So how are the ways that that can happen? Well, one, the person can be lost to follow-up. Two, they can just stop taking their metformin. Three, and more commonly, they can progress to needing a more significant drug. So all of those patients were excluded from the study. So think about that for a moment. This is, in my opinion, a significant limitation of this study. Because what you're basically doing is saying, we're only going to consider the patients who were on met form and stayed on met form and never progressed through it. And we're going to compare those to people
Starting point is 00:28:59 who were not having type 2 diabetes. So an analogy here would be, imagine we're going to do a study of two groups that we think are almost identical, one of them are smokers, and the other are identical in every way, but they're not smokers. And we're gonna follow them to see
Starting point is 00:29:13 which ones get lung cancer. But every time somebody dies in the smoking group, we stop counting them. When you get to the end, you're going to have a less significant view of the health status of that group. So with that caveat, the Bannister study found a very interesting result, which was the
Starting point is 00:29:34 crude death rate was, and by the way, the way these are done, this is also one of the challenges of epidemiology, is the math gets much more complicated. You have to normalize death rate for the amount of time you study the people. So everything is normalized to thousand person years. So the crude death rate in the group of people with type 2 diabetes who were on metformin, including the censoring, was 14.4. So 14.4 deaths occurred per thousand patient years. If you looked at the control group, it was 15.2. This was a startling result. And I remember reading this in, again, 2014 and being like, holy crap, this is really amazing. Is there, could you explain why?
Starting point is 00:30:24 Because I hear those numbers and they don't see that striking. It's a difference of about a year and a half. Now, of course, a difference of about a year and a half in lifespan is remarkable. It doesn't even translate to that. So taking a step back, type two diabetes on average will shorten your life by six years. I see.
Starting point is 00:30:42 So that's the actuarial difference between having type two diabetes and not all comers. But you're right, this is not a huge difference. It's only a difference of a little less than one year of life, per thousand patient years studied. Okay, and by the way,
Starting point is 00:30:55 appears just point out my math was wrong when I said about a year and a half. But the point here is, you would expect the people in the Metformin group to have a far worse outcome, i.e. to have a far worse crude death rate. And the fact that it was statistically significant in the other direction.
Starting point is 00:31:13 And it turned out on what's called a Cox proportional hazard, which is where you actually model the difference in lifespan. The people who took Metformin and had diabetes had a 15% 1,5%, 15% relative reduction in all-cause death over 2.8 years, which was the median duration of follow-up. Well, that seems to be the number that makes me go, wow. Yeah. Right, because it could you repeat those numbers again? Yeah.
Starting point is 00:31:44 So, 15% reduction in all cause mortality over 2.8 years. That's a big deal. It is. And again, there's no clear explanation for it unless you believe that metformin is doing something beyond helping you lower blood glucose. Because the difference in blood glucose
Starting point is 00:32:07 between these two people was still in favor of the non-diabetics. So again, the proponents of metformin being a zero protective agent, and I put myself in this category at one point, I would put myself today in the category of undecided, but at the time, I very much believed this was a very good suggestion that Metformin was doing other things.
Starting point is 00:32:29 You mentioned a couple already. Metformin is a weak inhibitor of mTOR. Metformin reduces inflammation. Metformin potentially tamps down on senescent cells and their secretory products. There are lots of things Metformin could be doing that are off-target. And it might be that those things are conferring the advantage. So, fast forward until a year ago, and I think most people took the Bannister study as kind of the best evidence we have for
Starting point is 00:32:58 the benefits of Metformin, and I'm sure you've had lots of people come up to you and ask you, should I be on Metformin? Should I be on Metforma? I mean, I probably get asked that question almost as much as I'm asked any question outside of due. I mean, people definitely want to know if you should be consuming due, but after that it's metforma. Fresh off the leaves. Has to be. While I'm viewing morning sunlight.
Starting point is 00:33:16 So, okay, so let's kind of fast forward to now the paper that I wanted to spend a few more minutes on. Yeah, and thanks for that background. I'm still dazzled by the insertion of the straw by way of insulin. I don't think I've ever heard that described. I need to go get a better textbook. It's a pretty short straw in fairness.
Starting point is 00:33:38 You know, it's just a little tricky. Yeah, I'm just to give people a sense of why I'm so dazzled by I am always fascinated by how quickly how efficiently and how specifically Biology can create these little protein complexes that do something really important I mean you're talking about an on-demand creation of a little of a portal, right? I mean these are cells engineering their own machinery and real time in response to chemical signals.
Starting point is 00:34:06 But, but it's, it's great. Yeah, but I'm, I'm sort of rusty on my neuroscience, but an action potential works in reverse the same way. Like, you need the ATP gradient to restore the, to restore the gradient, but once the action potential fires, it's passive outside, right? Yeah. So what peers are referring to is the way that neurons become electrically active is by the flow of ions across the cell, from the outside of cell to inside of cell.
Starting point is 00:34:31 And we have both active conductances, meaning they're triggered by electrical changes in the gradients, by changes in electrical potential. And then their passive gradients where things can just flow back and forth until there's a balance equal inside and outside the cell. I think what's different is that there's some movement of a lot of stuff inside of neurons when neurotransmitters like dopamine binds to its receptor and then a bunch of, you know, it's like a bucket brigade that gets kicked off internally. But it's not often that you hear about receptors getting inserted into cells very quickly. Normally, you have to go through a process of, you know, go through a process of transcribing
Starting point is 00:35:05 genes and making sure that the specific proteins are made. And then those are long, slow things that take place over the course of many hours or days. What you're talking about is a real on-demand insertion of a channel. And it makes sense as to why that would be required. But it's just so very cool. It's cool.
Starting point is 00:35:21 So keys and colleagues came along and said, we would like to redo the entire banister analysis. And I think their motivation for it was the interest in this topic is through the roof. There is a clinical trial called the tame trial that is, I think, pretty much funded now and may be getting underway soon. The tame trial, which is an important
Starting point is 00:35:45 trial, is going to try to ask this question prospectively and through random assignment. So this is the targeting aging with metformin trial. That's correct. Okay. Near Barzoli is probably the senior PI on that. And I think in many ways, the banister study, along with some other studies, but of lesser significance, probably provided some of the motivation for the tame trial. So they said, okay, like we're going to do this.
Starting point is 00:36:12 We're going to use a different cohort of people. So the first study that we just talked about, the banister study used, I believe it was, like roughly they sampled like 95,000 subjects from a UK bio bank. Here, they used a larger sample. They did about half a million people sampled from a Danish health registry. And they did something pretty elegant. They created two groups to study.
Starting point is 00:36:36 So the first was just a standard replication of what Bannister did, which was just a group of people with and without diabetic that they tried to match as perfectly as possible. But then they did a second analysis in parallel with discordant twins. So same sex twins that only differed in that one had diabetes and one didn't. I thought this was very elegant because here you have a degree of genetic similarity and you have similar environmental factors during childhood that might give you, you know,
Starting point is 00:37:08 allow you to see if there's any sort of difference in signal. So now turning this back into a little bit of a journal club, virtually any clinical paper you're gonna read, table one is the characteristics of the people in the study. You always wanna take a look at that. So when I look at table 1 here, you can see, and by the way, just for people watching this, we're going to make all these papers and figures available.
Starting point is 00:37:31 So if you're, you know, don't, you know, we'll have nice show notes that'll make all this clear. So Table 1 in the keys paper shows the baseline characteristics. And again, it's almost always going to be the first table in a paper. Usually the first table in a paper. Usually the first figure in the paper is a study design. It's usually a flow chart that says these were the inclusion criteria, these were all the people that got excluded. This is how we randomized, etc. And you can see here that there are four columns. So the first two are
Starting point is 00:37:59 the singletons. These are people who are not related. And then the second two are the twins who are matched. And you can see, remember how I said they sampled about 500,000 people? You can see the numbers. So they got 7,842 singletons on Metformin, the same number then they pulled out matched without diabetes. On the twins, they got 976 on Metformin with diabetes. And then by definition, 976 co-twins without them. And you look at all these characteristics.
Starting point is 00:38:29 What was their age upon entry? How many were men? What was the year of indexing when we got them? What medications were they on? What was their highest level of education, marital status, et cetera? The one thing I want to call out here that really cannot be matched in a study like this.
Starting point is 00:38:44 So this is a very important limitation, is the medication. So look at that column, Andrew. Notice how pretty much everything else is perfectly matched until you get to the medication list. Yeah, it's all over the place. Yeah, it's just, it's not even close. They're nowhere near matched, right? In other words, just to give you a couple of examples, right? Let's just talk about the singletons,
Starting point is 00:39:05 because it's basically the same story on the twins. If you look at what fraction of the people would type to diabetes are on lipid lowering medication, it's 45.6% versus 15.4% in the matched without diabetes. It's a 3x difference. What about anti-platelet therapy? That's 30% versus 14%. Anti-hypertensive, 65% versus 63% versus 31%.
Starting point is 00:39:28 Because people who have one health issue and are taking metformin' are likely to have other health issues. Exactly. So this is, again, a fundamental flaw of epidemiology. You can never remove all the confounders. This is why I became an experimental scientist so that we could control variables.
Starting point is 00:39:46 That's right. Because without random assignment, you cannot control every variable. Now you'll see in a moment when we get into the analysis, they go through three levels of corrections, but they can never correct this medication one. So just keep that in the back of your mind. Okay. So the two big things that were done in this experiment, in this survey or you know study to differentiate it from banister was one the twin trick which I think is pretty cool. The second thing that they did was they did a sensitivity analysis with and without informative censoring. So one of the things they wanted to know is hey does it really matter if we don't count the metformin patients who progress?
Starting point is 00:40:28 So, let's see kind of what transcribed. So the next figure, figure two, pardon me, the next table, table two, walks you through the crude mortality rate in each of the groups. So the most important row, I think, in this table is the one that says crude mortality per thousand person years. Now you recall that in the previous study, in the Bannister study, those were on the ballpark of about 15 per. Okay. So let's look at each of these. So in the
Starting point is 00:41:14 Single the singletons with without so the non twins who were not diabetic it was 16.86 and could you put a little more Contour on what this thousand person years what what it is I tell you about pooling the lifespans of a Of a bunch of different people until you get to the number 1000. Yeah, because you're normalizing not, so it's not who's gonna live a thousand years because everyone's expecting that. So you've got some people that are gonna live 76 years, 52 years, 91 years, and you're pooling all of those until you hit a thousand. And then that becomes kind of a,
Starting point is 00:41:43 it's like a normalized division. You're basically like, so let's say the control group, you're asking if there were a thousand person years available to live, how likely is it that this person would live another 15 years? Yeah, so a couple of ways to think about it. So taking a step back, we always have to have some way of normalizing. So when we talk about the mortality from a disease like cancer in the population, we would, we report it as what's the mortality rate per, and it's typically per 100,000 persons. Okay. That's a much more intuitive way to express it. It is, but the reason we can do it that way is because we're literally looking at how many people died this calendar year
Starting point is 00:42:26 and we divide it by the number of people in that age group. So it's typically what you're doing when you look at aged groups and buckets of like decades. So that's why we can say the highest mortality is like people 90 and up. Even though the absolute number of deaths is small, it's because there's not that many people there, though the absolute number of deaths is small, it's because there's not that many people there, right? The majority of deaths in absolute terms probably occur in the seventh decade. But as you go up, because the denominator is shrinking,
Starting point is 00:42:57 you have to normalize to it. So we just normalize to the number of people. Here are all the people that started the year, here are all the people that ended the year. What's the death rate? Why are these done in a slightly more complicated way? Because we don't follow these people for their whole lives.
Starting point is 00:43:10 We're only following them for a period of observation. In this case, roughly three years. So to say something like, you know, we have a crude death rate of five deaths per thousand person years. One way to think about that is, if you had a thousand people and you followed them for one year, you'd expect five to die. If you had 500 people and you followed them for two years, you'd expect five to die. If you have a thousand people and you follow them for
Starting point is 00:43:40 one year, you'd expect five to die. Those would all be considered equivalent mortalities. for one year you expect five to die. Those would all be considered equivalent mortalities. Great, thank you for clarifying that. No, no, this stuff is, I mean, like I find epidemiology when you get in the weeds is way more complicated than following the basics of experimental stuff where you just, you get to push all this stuff into the garbage bin
Starting point is 00:44:02 and just say, we're gonna take this number of people, we're gonna exclude this group, we're gonna randomize, we're gonna to take this number of people, we're going to exclude this group, we're going to randomize, we're going to see what happens. Yeah, that's what the paper will talk about next. Yep. So when you adjust for age, and they don't show it in this table, it's only in the text, when you adjust for age, a very important check to do
Starting point is 00:44:22 is what is the crude death rate of the people on Metformin who are not twins versus who are twins. Now in this table, they look different because it's 24.93 for the Metformin group and 21.68 for the twin group. That's on Metformin. When you adjust for age, they're almost identical. It goes from 24.93 to 24.7. One other point I'll make
Starting point is 00:44:47 here for people who are going to be looking at this table is you'll notice there are parentheses after every one of these numbers. What does that offer in there? Those parentheses are offering the 95% confidence interval. So, for example, to take the number, you know, 24.93 is the crude death rate of how many people are dying to take metformin. What it's telling you is we're 95% confident that the actual number is between 23.23 and 26.64. If a 95% confidence interval does not cross the number zero. It's statistically significant. Okay, so the first thing that just jumps out at you, I think when you look at this is there's clearly a difference here between the people who have diabetes and those who don't. It complicates the study a little bit because it's basically two studies in one, but you're comparing 95, pardon me, 24.93 to 16.86, which by the way
Starting point is 00:45:50 remains after age adjustment, when you go to the twin group, it's 24.73 to 12.94. So maybe just to zoom out for that, what you're describing, if I understand correctly, is this crew deaths per 1,000 person years. Let's just talk about the singletons, the non-tales, is 16.86. So 16.86 people die, and some people will probably think, how can 0.86 of a person die? Well, it's not always whole numbers,
Starting point is 00:46:16 but there's a bad joke to be made here, but call it 17 versus 25. Right, 17 deaths per 1 per thousand versus 25 deaths. Yep. And the 25 is in the folks that took Metformin. Now, that to the naive listener and to me, means, oh, Metformin basically kills you, right? Not a faster, or you know, you're more likely to die,
Starting point is 00:46:42 but we have to remember that these people have another, they have a major health issue that the other group does not have. That's right, because people weren't assigned drug or not assigned drug. It wasn't placebo drug. It's let's look at people taking this drug for a bad health issue and compare to everyone else. That's right. So now you have to go into, and I'll just sort of skip the next figure, but the next figure is a Kaplan Meyer curve. I think it's actually worth looking at it because they show up in all sorts of studies. So if you look at figure one, it's a Kaplan Meyer curve,
Starting point is 00:47:16 which is a mortality curve. So you'll see these in any study that is looking at death. And this can be prospective, randomized, this can be retrospective, but these are always gonna show up. And I think it's really worth understanding when a cap on myocurve shows you. So when the x-axis is always time and on the y-axis is always the cumulative survival.
Starting point is 00:47:38 So it's a curve that always goes from zero to one, one or 100%, and it's always decreasing monotonically, meaning it can only go down or stay flat, it can never go back up. So that's what a cumulative mortality curve looks like. Now we're looking at, you're starting it alive, and you're looking at how many people die for every year that passes.
Starting point is 00:48:02 That's right. And in each curve, there's one on the left, which is the matched singletons, and there's one on the right, which are the discordant twins. You have two lines. You have those that were on Metformin with Type 2 diabetes, and you have their matched controls. And in this figure, the matched controls
Starting point is 00:48:20 are the darker lines, and the people with Type 2 diabetes on Metformin, that's the lighter line. You've also noticed, and I like the way they've done it here, they've got shading around each one. And we should mention for those that are just listening that in both of these graphs, the downward trending line from the controls. So again, non-diabetic not taking metformin is above controls. So again, non-diabetic not taking metformin is above the line corresponding to the diabetics who are taking metformin put crudely the people who are taking metformin that have diabetes are dying at a faster rate for every single year exam. And the two lines do not overlap except at the beginning when everyone's alive. It's like a foot race where basically people with metformin and diabetes are falling behind
Starting point is 00:49:09 and dying as they fall. That's right and I'm glad you brought up a good point. It's not uncommon in treatments to see cappin-myer curves cross. They don't have to, it's not a requirement that they never cross. It's only a requirement that they're monotonically decreasing or staying flat. So I've seen cancer treatment drugs where they have like two drugs going head to head in a cancer treatment and like one starts out looking really, really bad. But then all of a sudden it kind of flattens while the other one goes bad and then it actually
Starting point is 00:49:39 crosses and goes underneath. But that's not the case here. So to your point, the people with diabetes taking metformin in both the match singletons and the discordans are dropping much faster and they always stay below. And I was just going to say that the shading is just showing you a 95% confidence interval. So you're just hooding basically error bars along this. So if this were experimental data, if you were doing an experiment with a group of mice and you were watching their survival
Starting point is 00:50:09 and you would have error bars on this which you're actually measuring. So this is because you have much more data here, you're just showing this in this fashion. For those that haven't been familiar as to statistics, no problem. Error bars correspond to like if you were just gonna measure the heights of a room full of 10th graders, there's going to be a range, right?
Starting point is 00:50:26 You have the very tall kid and the very shorter kid and you have the short kid and the medium kid. So there's a range. There's going to be an average, a mean, and then there'll be standard deviations and standard errors. So these confidence intervals just give a sense of how much range. Some people die early, some people die late. Within a given year, they're going to be different ages.
Starting point is 00:50:52 These error bars can account for a lot of different forms of variability here. You're talking about the variability is how many people in each group die. We're not tracking one diabetic taken metformin versus a control. I should have asked this earlier, but... Well, and it's also a mathematical model at this point, too, that's smoothing it out. Right. Because notice it's running for the full eight years, even though they're only following people for, you know, typically, I think the median was like three or four years at a time.
Starting point is 00:51:20 So they're using this quite complicated type of mathematics called a Cox proportional hazard, which is what generates hazard ratios and Basically any model has to have some error in it and so they're basically saying this is the error So you could argue when you look at that figure We don't know exactly where the line is in there, but we know it's in that shaded area If those sort of make one other point, if those shaded areas overlapped, you couldn't really make the conclusion. You wouldn't know for sure that one is different from the other. Yeah, that's actually a good opportunity to raise a common myth, which is a lot of people when they look at a paper, let's say it's a bar graph. And they see these error bars, and they will say people often think, oh, if the error
Starting point is 00:52:09 bars overlap, it's not a significant difference. But if the error bars don't overlap, meaning there's enough separation, then that's a real and meaningful difference. And that's not always the case. It depends a lot on the form of the experiment. I often see some of the more robust Twitter battles over how people are reading graphs. And I think it's important to remember
Starting point is 00:52:29 that you run the statistics, hopefully the correct statistics for the sample, but determining significance, whether or not the result could be due to something other than chance. Of course, your confidence in that increases as it becomes typically P value is P less than 0.001 percent chance that it's due to chance, right? So very low probably P less than 0.05 tends to be the kind of gold standard cut off. But when you're talking
Starting point is 00:52:58 about data like these, which are repeated measures over time, people are dropping out literally over time, you're dropping out literally over time. You're saying they've modeled it to make predictions as to what would happen. We're not necessarily looking at, you know, raw data points here. Yeah, the raw data was in the previous table. That's now taken and run through this Cox model
Starting point is 00:53:17 and it's smoothed out. Got it. And to your point about the bar graphs, yeah, I think the other thing you always want to understand is just because something doesn't achieve statistical significance, the only way you can say it's not significant is you have to know what it was powered to detect. And statistical power is a very important concept
Starting point is 00:53:40 that probably doesn't get discussed enough. But before you do an experiment, you have to have an expectation of what you believe the difference is between the groups. And you have to determine the number of samples you will need to assess whether or not that difference is there or not. So you use something, it's called a power table,
Starting point is 00:54:02 and you would go to the power table. So if you're doing treatment A versus treatment B, and you say, well, I think treatment A is going to have a 50% response, and I think treatment B will have a 65% response. You literally go to a power table that says 50% response, 15% difference. That gives you a place on the grid, and I want to be 90% sure that I'm right. So 90% power. I'm being a little bit. So there's going to be a statistician listening to this who's going to want to kill me, but this is directionally the way we would describe it. And that tells you, this is how many animals or people you would need in this study. You're going to need 147 in each
Starting point is 00:54:41 group. And by the way, if you now do the experiment with 147 and you fail to find significance, you can comfortably say there is no statistical difference at least up to that 15%. There may be a difference at 10%, but you weren't powered to look at 10%. Yeah, and a very important point that you're making. Another point that's just a more general, one about statistics, in general, the way to reduce variability in a data set is to increase sample size. And that kind of makes sense, right? If I just walk into a 10th grade class and I'm going to measure height and I look up
Starting point is 00:55:14 by the first three kids that I see, and I happen to look over there, and it's the three that all play on the volleyball team together. My sample size is small, and I'm likely to get a skewed representation, in this case taller than average. So increasing sample size tends to decrease variation. So that's why when you hear about a study from the UK Biobank or from half a million Danish citizens, for instance, in this study, those are enormous sample sizes. So even though this is not an experimental study, it's an epidemiological observational study, there's tremendous power by way of the enormous number of subjects in this study. And that's the way that epidemiology will make up for its deficit.
Starting point is 00:55:58 So you could never do a randomized assignment study on half a million people. a randomized assignment study on half a million people. So epidemiology makes up for its biggest limitation, which is it can never compensate for inherent biases by saying we can do infinite duration if we want. Like we could survey people over the course of their lives and we can have the biggest sample size possible, because this is relatively cheap. The cost of actually doing an experiment where you have tens of thousands of people is prohibitive. I mean, if you look at the Women's Health Initiative, which was a five-year study on,
Starting point is 00:56:33 I don't know, what was it, 50,000 women, I mean, that was a billion-dollar study. So this is the balancing act between epidemiology and randomized prospective experiments. And so they both offer something, but you just have to know they're blind spots of each one. So let's just kind of wrap this up. I mean, I think let's just go to table four, which I think is the most important table in here, which now lays out the final results in terms of the hazard ratios.
Starting point is 00:57:03 So this is the way we want to really be thinking about this. So again, hazard ratios, these are important things to understand. A hazard ratio is a number and you always subtract one from the hazard ratio and that tells you, if it's a positive number, if it's a number, sorry, if it's a number greater than one, you subtract one and that tells you the relative harm. So if the hazard ratio is 1.5, you subtract 1.5 is a 50% increase in risk. If the number is negative, you may recall on the banister paper, the hazard ratio is 0.85.
Starting point is 00:57:36 So if it's an estimate, so that means it's a 15% reduction in relative risk. And here, you can see all the hazard ratios are positive. So what it's telling you here is, and I'm going to walk through this because there's a lot of information packed here. You've got singletons, you've got twins, they're showing you three different ways that they do it. They do an unadjusted model.
Starting point is 00:57:55 If you just look at the singletons with and without Metformin and you make no adjustments, the hazard ratio is 1.48, meaning that people on Metformin had a 48% greater chance of dying in any given year than their non-diabetic counterpart. The only reason I'm smiling is not because I enjoy people dying quite to the contrary, is that this is novel for me. And that I've read some epidemiological studies before, but it's not normally where I spend the majority of my time but up until now I was thinking okay people taking metform and are dying more than those that aren't I just and I just relieved to know that I wasn't looking at all this backwards.
Starting point is 00:58:35 Yeah, yeah. So they're dying more but of course we don't have a group that's taking metformin who doesn't have diabetes and we don't have a group who has diabetes and you know is taking that form and plus something else. So again, we're only dealing with these constrained populations. Yeah, now there's an other arm to this study that I'm not getting into because it adds more complexity, which is they also have another group that's got diabetes, takes met form and and takes sulfonia rias, which is a bigger drug, and those people die even more.
Starting point is 00:59:06 Whoa. So, which again speaks to the point, right? The more you need these medications, they're never able to erase the effect of diabetes. But in this case, it seems that they might be accelerating, possibly accelerating death due to diabetes. We could never know that from this, because we don't see, we would need to see diabetics who don't take metformin, who take nothing. And I would bet that they would do even worse.
Starting point is 00:59:34 So my intuition is that the metformin is helping, but not helping nearly as much as we thought before. So my point is they make another set of adjustments. They say, okay, well, look, in the first one in the unadjusted model, we only matched for age and gender. Okay, that's pretty crude. What if we adjust for the medications that are on the cardiovascular, psychiatric, pulmonary, dementia meds, and marital status? I don't know why they threw marital status in there, but they did. I don't know, maybe being married or unmarried. I'm sure, but it just seems like a random thing
Starting point is 01:00:06 to throw in with all their meds. I would have personally done that adjustment higher up. But nevertheless, if you do that, all of a sudden, the hazard ratio drops from 1.48 to 1.32, which means you still have a 32% greater chance of dying in any given year. All right, what if we also adjust for the highest level of education, along with any of the other covariates?
Starting point is 01:00:30 Well, that doesn't really change at all. It ends up at 1.33 or a 33% chance, increase in death. Okay, I always knew that more school wasn't gonna save me. It's not doing jack. So now let's do it for the twins. If you do the twin study, which you could argue is a slightly pureer study because you at least have one genetic and environmental thing that you've attached, the unadjusted model
Starting point is 01:00:52 is brutal. 2.15. That's 115%. Think about this. These are twins who in theory are the same in every way except one has diabetes and one doesn't, and the one with diabetes on metformin still has 115% greater chance of dying than the non-diabetic co-twin. When you make that first adjustment of all the meds and marital status, you bring it down to a 70% increase in risk and when you throw education in it goes up to an 80% chance
Starting point is 01:01:19 of risk. Now they did this really cool thing which was they did the analysis on within without censoring. So everything I just said here was based on no censoring. Tell me about censoring. Sensoring is when you stop counting the metform and people who have died. Okay. So, in the Singleton group, when you unadjust it, and the reason I'm doing the unadjusted is that's where they did the sensitivity analysis.
Starting point is 01:01:47 I don't think it really matters that much. You just have to draw a line in the sand somewhere. You'll recall that that was a 48% chance of increased mortality, all cause mortality. If you stop counting, if you, pardon me, if you don't censor, meaning if you include everybody, including when people on Metformin with diabetes die, if you censor them, it comes down to 1.39. In other words, this is a very important finding. It did not undo the benefits that we saw in the banister study.
Starting point is 01:02:18 Banister saw a 15% reduction in mortality when they censored. 15% reduction in mortality when they censored. When keys censored, it got better, but not that much better. It went from 48 to 39%. In the twins, it went from 115% down to only 97%. So in some ways, this presents a little bit of an enigma because it's not entirely clear to me having read these papers many times.
Starting point is 01:02:49 Exactly why Bannister found such a different response. There's another technical detail of this paper, which is you can see on the right side of Table 4, they did something called a nested case control. But you'll see, and I was going to go into a long explanation of what nested case controls are. It's another pretty elegant way to do case control studies where you sample by year and you sort of normal, you don't count all the cases at the end, you count them one by one.
Starting point is 01:03:21 I don't think it's worth getting into Andrew because it doesn't change the answer. You can see it changes it just slightly, but it doesn't change the point. The point here is the key's paper makes it undeniably clear that in that population there was no advantage offered by Metformin that undid the disadvantage of having type two diabetes. This does not mean that Metformin wasn't helping them because we don't know what these people would have been like without metformin. It could be that this bought them a 50% reduction in relative mortality to where they'd been. But what it says is, in a way, this is what you would have expected. This is what you would have expected 10 years ago before the banister paper came out.
Starting point is 01:04:03 Or maybe even before metformin was used, because in some ways it's saying, what is the likelihood that sick people who are on a lot of medication are gonna die compared to not sick people who aren't on a lot of medication? Yep, you know, it's not quite that simple in the sense that, as you said,
Starting point is 01:04:19 there are ways to try and isolate the Metformin contribution somewhat, because they're on a bunch of other meds. And presumably that was done and analyzed in other figures where they can sort of try and they can never attach the results specifically to metformin, right? But there must be some way of waiting the percentage that are on psychiatric meds or not on psychiatric meds, is a way to tease out whether or not there's actually some contribution
Starting point is 01:04:48 to form into this result. Well, that's what they're doing in the partial adjustment is they're actually doing their best to say, you're not married, they're going variable by drug, all the way through high blood pressure, non-high blood pressure smoking non smoking etc Right, and the way they would do that presumably is by saying okay married not married. That's what that's a simple one Are you on lipid lowering meds? Yes or no? Okay, you are not
Starting point is 01:05:16 You are not they and then comparing those groups. Yeah, yeah, okay So no no differences jumping out that can be purely explained by these other variables. Yes, although again, this is a this is a great opportunity to talk about why no matter how slick you are no matter how slick your model is you can't control for everything. There is a reason that to my knowledge virtually every study that compares meat eaters to non-meat eaters finds an advantage amongst the non-meat eaters. And we can talk about all lifespan advantage. Yes.
Starting point is 01:05:46 And we can, or disease, you know, incidents studies. And yeah, it might be tempting to say, well, they're for eating meat is bad, until you realize that it takes a lot of work to not eat meat. That's a very, very significant decision that a person, for most people, is a very significant decision a person makes. And for a person to make that decision, that's a very significant decision a person makes. And for a person to make that decision,
Starting point is 01:06:06 they probably have a very high conviction about the benefit of that to their health. And it is probably the case that they're making other changes with respect to their health as well that are a little more difficult to measure. Now there's a million other problems with that. I picked a silly example because the whole meat discussion then gets into, well, you know, when we say eating meat, what do we mean?
Starting point is 01:06:27 Like, the document is like, deli meat versus grass bed. Exactly. We're out. We're out. You know, a deer that you hunted with your ball. That's right. So how do we get into all those things? But my point is, it's very difficult to quantify some of the intangible differences.
Starting point is 01:06:39 And I think that even a study that goes to great lengths, as this one does, epidemiologically, to make these corrections can never make the corrections. And so for me, the big takeaway of this study is one, this makes much more sense to me than the banister paper, which never really made sense to me. And again, I was first critical of the banister paper in 2018, about four years after Komet. That's about the time I stopped taking that form and by the way.
Starting point is 01:07:02 I stopped taking it for a different reason, which we can talk about in a sec. But that was the first time I went back and said, wait a minute, this information, this informative censoring thing is, that's a little fishy. And I think we weren't looking at a true group of real type 2 diabetics. Now that said, maybe it doesn't matter. In other words, maybe, and even the key's paper doesn't tell us that metformin wouldn't be beneficial, because it could be that those people, if they were on nothing, as their matched cohorts were on
Starting point is 01:07:32 nothing, would have been dying at, you know, a hazard ratio of three, and this brought it down to 1.5, in which case you would say there is some zero protection there. It is putting the breaks on this process. All of this is to say absent a randomized control trial,. It is putting the brakes on this process. All of this is to say absent a randomized control trial, we will never know the answer. Has there been a randomized control trial? Not a misplaced? Not when it comes to a hard outcome.
Starting point is 01:07:54 Now, there has been in the ITP. So the interventionist testing program, which is kind of the gold standard for animal studies, which is run out of three labs. So it's an NIH funded, which is run out of three labs. So it's an NIH funded program that's run out of three labs. They basically test molecules for zero protection. The ITP was the first study that really put rapamycin on the map in 2009. That was the study that's fortuitously demonstrated that even when rapamycin was given very, very
Starting point is 01:08:23 late in life, it was given to 60 month old mice It's still afforded them a 15% lifespan extension has a similar study been done in humans. I mean, it's hard to know. You can't really control with rabomis. No But when the ITP studied metformin it did not succeed So the the there have not been that many metformin, it did not succeed. So there have not been that many drugs that have worked in the ITP. The ITP is very rigorous, right?
Starting point is 01:08:50 It doesn't use an in-bred strain of mice. It is done concurrently in three labs with very large sample sizing. And so when something works in the ITP, it's pretty exciting. RAPA mice and has been studied several times. It's always worked. Another one we should talk about at a subsequent time is 17 alpha estradile.
Starting point is 01:09:10 This continues to work in male mice. And it produces comparable effects to RAPA mice and- Estrogen. Doesn't work in female mice. Makes. But this is alpha, not beta. So this is 17 alpha estradile, not beta estradile, which is the estradile that we all,
Starting point is 01:09:24 that is bioavailable in all of this. And just as a brief aside, thank you and I basically agree that unless it's a problem, males, we're talking post puberty, should try and have their estradiol as high as possible without having negative symptomology because of the importance of estrogen for libido for brain function tissue, health, bone health, this idea of crushing estrogen and raising testosterone is just silly, right? There's not, let's just leave raising testosterone out of it, but many of the approaches to raising testosterone that are pharmacologic in nature also raise estrogen.
Starting point is 01:10:01 A lot of people try and push down on estrogen. And that is just, again, unless people are getting hyperestogenic effects, like got a comastia or other issues, is the exact wrong direction to go. You want estrogen. Estrogen is a very important hormone for men and women. Can agaflose in an SGLT2 inhibitor, also very successful in the ITP, but again, interestingly, Rapa metformin not. So metformin has failed in the ITP. So you no longer take a metformin?
Starting point is 01:10:31 I stopped five years ago. I mean, you're not a diabetic, so presumably you're taking it for a geroprotection. To buffer blood glucose. Yeah, and ultimately potentially a longer. Yes, exactly. And the reason I stopped, and this will be the last thing before we move on, because you couldn't go to the dairy queen at the buffet of that. No, finally, the nausea went away after a few weeks or a month, maybe.
Starting point is 01:10:51 But once I got really into lactate testing, I noticed how high my lactate was at rest. So a resting, fasted lactate should be in a healthy person, should be below one, like somewhere between 0.3, 0.6 mill millimole and only when you start to exercise should lactate go up and 2018 was when I started blood testing for my zone two so previously when I was doing zone two testing I was just going off my power meter and heart rate But this is when this is after I met in you go son Milan and I started like wanting to use the lactate threshold
Starting point is 01:11:26 of two millimole as my determinant of where to put my wattage on the bike. And I'm like, doing fingerpricks before I start and I'm like 1.6 millimole and I'm like, what the hell is going on? I can't be 1.6. But ran the flight of stairs up the back of the Empire State Building. Well, no, that would put me a lot higher, right? But, and when I'm being generous to your fitness.
Starting point is 01:11:46 No, but that's when I started digging a little digging and realized, oh, you know what, this totally makes sense. If you have a weak mitochondrial toxin, what are you gonna do? You're gonna shunt more glucose into pyruvate and more pyruvate into lactate. I'm ana aerobic. Yeah, you need an all turn in fuel source.
Starting point is 01:12:07 That's right. So, and then my zone two numbers just seemed off. My lactate is... Could you feel it, sorry to interrupt. Could you feel it in your body? Because maybe now I'll just briefly describe. I took burberry. I during the period of maybe somewhere in the 2012 to 2015
Starting point is 01:12:24 stretch, I don't recall exactly what you're taking it for. Well, I'll tell you so I was and I still am a big fan of Tim Ferriss's slow carb or hydring diet because I like to meet and vegetables and starches. I'm an omnivore and I found that it worked very quickly, got me very lean. I could exercise. I could think. I could sleep. You know, a lot of my rationale for following one eating regimen or another, what I eat is to enjoy myself but also mental energy. I mean, because if I can't sleep at night, I'm not going to replenish. I'm not, I don't replenish.
Starting point is 01:12:55 I'm going to feel like garbage. I don't care how lean I am or what you know. So I found the slow carb diet to be, which was in the four-hour body, to be a very good plan for me. It was pretty easy. You drop some things like bread, et cetera. You don't drink calories, except after a resistance training session, et cetera. But one day a week, you have this so-called cheat day. And on the cheat day, anything goes. And so I would eat, you know, eight croissants, and then I'd alternate to sweet stuff, and then I'd go to peace. And by the end of the day, you don't want to look at an item of food at all. So the only modification I made to this slow carb diet for our body thing was the day after the cheat day,
Starting point is 01:13:31 I wouldn't eat, I would just fast. And I had no problem doing that because it was just basically, well, since you said, what was it, anal... Anal sleep at the... Yeah, I did not have that, but since you said that, I won't up the ante here, but I'll at least match your anal sleepage comment by saying, I had, let's just call it profound
Starting point is 01:13:49 gastric distress after eating like that the next day. So the last thing you want to do is eat any food I would just hydrate, and oftentimes to try and get some exercise. And what I read was that burberry, in poor man's metformin, could buffer blood glucose, and in some ways make me feel less sick when ingesting all these calories, and in many cases, spiking my blood sugar and insulin, because you're having ice cream and, you know, et cetera.
Starting point is 01:14:16 And indeed it worked. So if I took burberry, and I don't recall the milligram count, and then I ate, you know, 12 donuts, I felt fine. It was as if I had eaten one donut. Wow. I felt sort of okay in my body and it felt much, much better. Now, presumably because it's buffering the spikes in blood sugar, I wasn't crashing in the afternoon nap and that whole thing. And do you remember how much you were taking? I think it was a couple hundred milligrams.
Starting point is 01:14:39 Does that sound about right? It was a bright yellow capsule. I forget the source. But in any case, one thing I noticed was that if I took burberry and I did not ingest a profound number of carbohydrates very soon afterwards, I got brutal headaches. I think I was hypoglycemic. I didn't measure it, but I just felt I had headaches, I didn't feel good, and then I would eat a pizza or two and feel fine. And so I realized that burberine was putting me on this kind of lower blood sugar state. That was the logic anyway. And it allowed me to eat these cheat foods.
Starting point is 01:15:14 But when I cycled off of the four out, because I don't follow the slow carb diet anymore, though, I might again at some point, when I stopped doing those cheat days, I didn't have any reason to take the burberry and I feared that I wasn't ingesting enough carbohydrates in order to really justify trying to buffer my blood glucose. Also, my blood glucose tends to be... Did you ever try a carbo? No, what is that? So, a carbo is another glucose disposal. Yeah, it's actually a drug that, but it works more in the gut and it just prevents glucose absorption. A carbo is another one of those drugs
Starting point is 01:15:46 that actually found a survival benefit in the ITP. And it was a very interesting finding because the thesis for testing it, the ITP is a very clever system. Anybody can nominate a candidate to be tested. And then the panel over there reviews it and they decide, yep, this is interesting, we'll go ahead and study it.
Starting point is 01:16:03 So when I think David Allison nominated a carbose to be studied, the rationale was it would be a caloric restriction mimetic because you would literally just fail to absorb, I don't know, make up some number, right? 15 to 20% of your carbohydrates would not be absorbed. And therefore you would, the mice would effectively be caloricly restricted. It was passed them out. That's right. And what happened was really interesting.
Starting point is 01:16:28 One, the mice lived longer on a carbo. But two, they didn't weigh any less. So it lived longer, but not through calorie restriction. That's interesting. Yes. And the speculation is they lived longer because they had lower glucose and lower insulin. And I don't want to send this down some rabbit holes here, but there are all sorts of interesting
Starting point is 01:16:49 ideas about, for instance, that some forms of dementia might be so-called type three diabetes, a diabetes of the brain, and so things like burberry and metformin, lowering blood glucose, ketogenic diets, et cetera, might be beneficial there. I mean, there's a lot to explore here, and I know you've explored a lot of that on your podcast. I've done far less of that. But, well, at least it seems that we know the following things for sure. One, you don't want insulin too high, nor too low.
Starting point is 01:17:15 You don't want blood glucose too high, nor too low. If the buffering systems for that are disrupted, clearly exercise, meaning regular exercise, is the best way to keep that system in check. But in the absence of that tool, or I would say in addition to that tool, is there any glucose disposal agent, because that's what we're talking about here, that form in burberine, acarbosate, et cetera,
Starting point is 01:17:39 that you take on a regular basis because you have that much confidence in it. The only one that I take is an SGLT2 inhibitor. So this is a class of drug that is used by people with type 2 diabetes, but because of my faith in the mechanistic studies of this drug, coupled with its results in the ITP, coupled with the human trial results that show profound benefit in non-diabetics taking it even for heart failure.
Starting point is 01:18:09 I think there's something very special about that drug. I'm actually, that was another paper I was thinking about presenting this time. Maybe we'll do that the next time. But do you believe in caloric restriction as a way to extend life or are you more of the do the right behaviors and that's covered in your book Outlive and elsewhere on your podcast. And buffer blood glucose. Do you still obviously you believe in buffering blood glucose in addition to just doing all the right behaviors? Yeah, I think you can uncouple a little bit the buffering of blood glucose from the caloric deficit. So I think you can be in a reasonable energy balance and buffer glucose with good
Starting point is 01:18:48 sleep hygiene, lots of exercise, and just thoughtful eating without having to go into a calorie deficit. So, you know, it's not entirely clear if profound caloric restriction would offer a survival advantage to humans, even if it were tolerable to most, which it's not. So for most people, it's just kind of off the table. If I said, Andrew, you need to eat 30% fewer calories for the rest of your life. I'll live 30% fewer years, thank you.
Starting point is 01:19:14 Yeah, there's just not many people who are willing to sign up for that, so it's kind of a moot point. But the question is, do you need to be fasting all the time? Do you need to be doing all of these other things? And the answer appears to be outside of using them as tools to manage energy balance, it's not clear. An energy balance probably plays a greater role in glucose homeostasis than from a nutrition standpoint
Starting point is 01:19:43 than the individual constituents of the meal. Now, that's not entirely true. I can imagine a scenario where a person could be in a negative energy balance eating twix bars all day and drinking big gulps. But I also don't think that's a very sustainable thing to do because if, by definition, I'm going to put you in negative energy balance, consuming that much crap. I'm going to destroy you. Like you're going to feel so miserable. You're going to be starving, right?
Starting point is 01:20:10 You're not going to be satiated eating pure garbage and being in caloric deficit. You're going to end up having to go into caloric excess. So that's why it's an interesting thought experiment. I don't think it's a very practical experiment. For a person to be generally satiated and an energy balance, they're probably eating about the right stuff. But I don't think that the specific macros matter as much as I used to think.
Starting point is 01:20:33 I'm a believer in getting most of my nutrients from unprocessed or minimally processed sources, simply because it allows me to eat foods I like. Yeah. And more of them. And I'd just love to eat. I so physically enjoy the sensation of chewing that I'll just like cucumber slices for fun. Yeah, right.
Starting point is 01:20:55 You know, that's, I mean, that's not my only form of fun, fortunately. Mm-hmm. Ha-ha-ha. Um, this is an amazing paper for the simple reason that it provides a wonderful tutorial of the benefits and drawbacks of this type of work. And I think it's also wonderful because we hear a lot about Metformin, Rappamysin, and these anti-aging approaches, but I was not aware that there was any study of such a large population
Starting point is 01:21:26 of people, so it's pretty interesting. Yeah, so I think it remains to be seen. If, and my patients often ask me, hey, should I be on metformin? And I give them a much, much, much, much shorter version of what we just talked about. And I say, look, if the tame study, which should answer this question more definitively, right, this is taking a group of non-diabetics and randomizing them to placebo versus metformin and studying for specific disease outcomes.
Starting point is 01:21:51 If the tame study ends up demonstrating that there is a zero protective benefit of metformin, I'll reconsider everything, all right? So I think that's, you know, we just have to, I think, all walk around with an appropriate degree of humility around what we know and what we don't know. But I would say right now, the epidemiology, the animal data, my own personal experience with its impact on my lactate production and exercise performance. There's a whole other rabbit hole we could go down another time, which is the impact
Starting point is 01:22:19 on hypertrophy and strength, which appears to be attenuated as well by metformin. I still prescribe it to patients all the time if they're insulin resistant for sure. It's still a valuable drug, but I don't think of it as a great tool for the person who's insulin sensitive and exercising a lot. I can't help but ask this question. Do you think there's any longevity benefit to short periods of caloric restriction? So for instance, I decide to, by the way, I haven't done this, but let's say I were to decide to fast into a one-meal a day type thing where I'm going to be in a slight caloric deficit, 500 to a thousand calories, for a couple of days, and then go back to eating the way that I before that short caloric restriction slash fast.
Starting point is 01:23:11 Is there any benefit to it in terms of cell or health? Can you, you know, so reset the system? Is there any idea that the change is the clearing of sentencing cells that we hear about autophagy, that we, you know, that in the short term, you can glean a lot of benefits and then go back to your regular pattern of eating and then periodically, you know, once every couple of weeks or once a month, just, you know, fast for a day or two. Is there any benefit to that that's purely in the domain of longevity? Not because there's all discipline function there, there's a flexibility function. There's probably an insulin sensitivity function But is there any evidence that it can help us live longer? I think the short answer is no
Starting point is 01:23:51 For two reasons one, I don't think that that duration would be sufficient if if one is gonna take that approach, but two Even if you went with something longer like what I used to do right I used to do seven days of water only per quarter three days per month. So I was basically always like it would be three day fast three day fast seven day fast. Just imagine doing that all year rotating rotating running for many years I did that. Now I certainly believed and to this day I would say I have no idea if that provided a benefit. But my thesis was the downside of this is relatively circumscribed, which is profound misery for a few days. And what I didn't appreciate the time, which I obviously now
Starting point is 01:24:35 look back at and realize is muscle mass loss. You're just, it's very difficult to gain back the muscle cumulatively after all of that loss. But my thought was exactly, as you said, like there's got to be a resetting of the system here. This must be sufficiently long enough to trigger all of those systems. But you're getting at a bigger problem with neuroscience, which I'm really hoping the epigenetic field comes to the rescue on.
Starting point is 01:25:02 It has not come close to it to date, which is we don't have biomarkers around true metrics of aging. Everything we have to date stinks. So we're really good at using molecules or interventions for which we have biomarkers, right? Like when you lift weights, you can look at how much weight you're lifting. You can look at your dexascant and see how much muscle mass you're generating. Those are biomarkers. Those are giving you outputs that say my input is good or my input needs to be modified. When you take a sleep supplement, you can look at your eight sleep and go,
Starting point is 01:25:39 oh, my sleep is getting better. There's a biomarker. When you take metformin, when you take rapamycin, when you fast, we don't have a biomarker that gives us any insight into whether or not we're moving in the right direction. And if we are, are we taking enough? Just don't know. So I often get asked, what's the single most important topic you would want to see more research dollars put to in terms of this space? And it's unquestionably this, as unsexy as it is. Like, who cares about biomarkers?
Starting point is 01:26:14 But, like, without them, I don't think we're going to get great answers, because you can't do most of the experiments you and I would dream up. Got it. Well, I'm grateful that you're sitting across the table for me telling me all this and that everyone can hear this. But again, we will put a link to the paper or his plural that Peter just described. And for those of you that are listening and not watching,
Starting point is 01:26:40 hopefully you're able to track the general themes and takeaways. And it is fun to go to these papers. You see these big stacks of numbers and it can be a little bit overwhelming, but my additional suggestion on parsing papers is notice that Peter said that he spent, you know, he's read it several times.
Starting point is 01:26:58 Unlike a newspaper article or a Instagram post, with a paper you're not necessarily going to get it the first time. You certainly won't get everything. So I think spending some time with papers for me means reading it and then reading it again a little bit later. And tell me what? Yeah, I was about to say, what's your, because I kind of have a way that I do it, but I'm curious as to how you do it. If you're encountering a paper for the first time, do you have an order in which you like to go through the, you want to do you read it sequentially
Starting point is 01:27:27 or do you look at the figures first? I mean, how do you how do you go through it? Yeah, unless it's an area that I know very, very well where I can you know skip to some things before reading it the whole way through. My process is always the same and actually this is fun because I used to teach a class when I was a professor at UC San Diego called Neural Circuits and Health and Disease. And it was an evening course that grew very quickly from 50 students to 400 plus students. And we would do exactly this. We would parse papers. And I had everyone ask what I called the four questions.
Starting point is 01:28:01 And it wasn't exactly four questions. But I have a little three by five card next to me or a piece of a nap by 11 paper, typically. And when I sit down with a paper, I want to figure out what is the question they're asking? What's the general question? What's the specific question? And I write down the question. Then what was the approach? How did they test that question? And sometimes I can get a bit detailed and get into an immunosystem chemistry and they did a PCR for this. It's not so important for most people that they understand every method,
Starting point is 01:28:30 but it is worthwhile that if you encounter a method like PCR or chromatography or FMIRI, that you at least look up on the internet what its purpose is, okay, that will help a lot. And then it was what they found. And there you can usually figure out what they believe they found. Anyway, by reading the figure headers, right?
Starting point is 01:28:52 What are, you know, figure one, here's the header. Typically, if it's an experimental paper, it will tell you what they want you to think they found. And then I tend to want to know the conclusion of the study. And then this is really the key one. And this is the one that would really distinguish the high performing students from the others. You have to go back at the end and ask
Starting point is 01:29:13 whether or not the conclusions, the major conclusions drawn in the paper are really substantiated by what they found and what they did. And that involves some thinking. It involves really spending some time thinking about what they identified. Now this isn't something that anyone can do straight off the bat. It involves really spending some time thinking about what they identified. Now this isn't something that anyone can do straight off the bat.
Starting point is 01:29:27 It's a skill that you develop over time and different papers require different formats. But those four questions really form the cornerstone of teaching undergraduates, and I think graduate students as well of how to read a paper. And again, it's something that can be cultivated. And it's still how I approach papers. So what I do, typically
Starting point is 01:29:47 is I'll read title abstract. I usually then will skip to the figures and see how much of it I can digest without reading the text and then go back and read the text. But in fairness, journals, great journals like science, like natures, oftentimes we'll pack so much information the cell press journals to into each figure and it's coded with no definition of the acronyms that almost always I'm into the introduction and results within a couple of minutes wondering what the hell this acronym is or the had acronym is and it's just, yeah, it's just wild how much, how much nomenclature there really is. I can't remember was it you or was it our friend Paul Conte when he was here who said that, oh no, I'm sorry, it was neither.
Starting point is 01:30:30 It was Chair of Ophthalmology at Stanford, Dr. Jeffrey Goldberg, who was a guest on the podcast recently, who off camera, I think it was, told us that if you look at the total number of words and terms that a physician leaving medical school owns in their mind and their vocabulary, it's the equivalent of like two additional full languages of fluency beyond their native language. So you're trilingual at least. Now, do you speak language other than English? Corridor.
Starting point is 01:30:59 Okay. So you're at least trilingual and probably more. So no one is expected to be able to parse these papers the first time through without, you know, substantial training. Yeah. No, I, I, I think that's, that's a great format. And you're absolutely right. I have a different way that I do it when I'm familiar with the subject matter. Versus when I'm not.
Starting point is 01:31:19 Uh, well, again, if I'm reading papers that are something that I know really well, I can basically glean everything I need to know from the figures. And then sometimes I'll just do a quick skim on methods. But I don't need to read the discussion. I don't need to read the intro. I don't need to read anything else. If it's something that I know less about, then I usually do exactly what you say.
Starting point is 01:31:41 I try to start with the figures. I usually end up generating more questions like what do you mean? What is this? How do they do that? And then I got to go back and read methods typically. And one of the other things that's probably worth mentioning is a lot of papers these days have supplemental information that are not attached to the paper. So you're amazed at how much stuff gets put in the supplemental section. And the reason for that, of course, is that the journals are very specific on the format
Starting point is 01:32:10 and length of a paper. So, a lot of the times when you're submitting something, you know, like, if you want to put any additional information in there, it can't go in the main article. It has to go in the supplemental figure. So, even through this paper, there were a couple of the numbers I spouted off that I had to pull out of the supplemental paper. For example, when they did the sensitivity analysis on the censoring versus nonsensoring, that was in the supplemental figure. That was actually not even in the paper we presented. Well, should we pivot into this other paper? Yeah. It's a very different sort of paper. It's an experimental paper where there's a manipulation. I must say I love love love this paper and I don't often say that about papers. I'm so excited about this paper for so many reasons, but I want to give a couple of caveats up front. First of all, the paper is not published yet.
Starting point is 01:33:01 The only reason I was able to get this paper is because it's on bioarchive. There's a new trend over the last, I would say, five, six years of people posting the papers that they've submitted to journals for peer review online so that people can look at them prior to those papers being peer reviewed. So there is a strong possibility that the final version of this paper, which again, we will provide a link to, is going to look different maybe even quite a bit different than the one that we're going to discuss. Nonetheless, there are a couple of things that make me confident in the data that we're about to talk about.
Starting point is 01:33:33 First of all, the group that published this paper is really playing in their wheelhouse. This is what they do. They publish a lot of really nice papers in this area. I'm going to mispronounce her first name, but I think it's Charles C. Goe who's at the Econ School of Medicine Mount Sinai, runs a laboratory there studying addiction in humans and the first off author of the paper is Ofer Pearl.
Starting point is 01:34:00 This paper is wild. And I'll just give you a couple of the takeaways first as a bit of a hook to hopefully entice people into listening further, because this is an important paper. This paper basically addresses how our beliefs about the drugs we take impacts how they affect us at a real level, not just at a subjective level, but at a biological level. So just to a subjective level, but at a biological level. So just to back up a little bit, a former guest on this podcast, Dr. Ali Krum, whose name is actually Aliyah Krum, but she goes by, Ali Krum talked about belief effects. Belief effects are different than placebo effects.
Starting point is 01:34:40 Plessibo effects are really just category effects. It's, okay, I'm going gonna give you this pill, Peter, and I'm gonna tell you that this pill is molecule X5952 and that it's going to make your memory better. And then I give you a memory test, right? And your group performs better than the people in the control group who I give a pill to and I say, this is just a placebo.
Starting point is 01:35:02 Or there are other variants on this where people will get a drug and you tell them it's placebo. They'll get a placebo, you tell them it's drug. It's a binary thing. It's an honor and off thing. You're either in the drug group or the placebo group and you're either told that you're getting drug or placebo. And we know that placebo effects exist.
Starting point is 01:35:20 In fact, one of the cooler ones, I was never the subject of this, but there was kind of a lower in high school that kids would do this mean thing. It's a form of bullying. I really don't like it. Where, you know, they get some kid at a party to drink alcohol-free beer. And then that kid would start acting drunk and then they go, gotcha, you know, it doesn't
Starting point is 01:35:36 even have alcohol in it. Now that's a mean joke and just reminds me of some of the horrors of high school. Maybe that's why I didn't go very often, which I also don't suggest. But no, it's a mean joke, but it speaks to the placebo effect. And there's also a social context effect. So placebo effects are real. We know this.
Starting point is 01:35:57 Belief effects are different. Belief effects are not A or B, placebo or non-placibo. Belief effects have a lot of knowledge to enrich one's belief about a certain something that can shift their psychology and physiology one way or the other. And I think the best examples of these really of these belief effects really do come from Ali Krums lab in the psychology department at Stanford, although some of this work she did prior to getting to Stanford.
Starting point is 01:36:22 For instance, if people are put into a group where they watch a brief video, just a few minutes of video about how stress really limits our performance. Let's say at archery or at mathematics or at music or at public speaking. And then you test them in any of those domains or other domains in a stressful circumstance. They perform less well. Okay. in a stressful circumstance, they perform less well. Okay, and we know they perform less well because we're by virtue of a heightened stress response. You can measure heart rate, you can measure stroke volume,
Starting point is 01:36:52 with the heart, you can measure peripheral blood flow, which goes down when people are stressed, narrowing a vision, et cetera. You take a different group of people and randomly assign them to another group where now they're being told that stress enhances performance. It mobilizes resources, it narrows your vision
Starting point is 01:37:09 such that you can perform tasks better, et cetera, et cetera, and their performance increases above a control group that receives just useless information. Or these useless as it relates to the task. So in both cases, by the way, the groups are being told the truth. Stress can be depleting or it can enhance performance, but this is different than placebo
Starting point is 01:37:27 because now it's scaling according to the amount and the type of information that they're getting. And can you give me a sense of magnitude of benefit or detriment that one could experience in a situation like the one you just described? Yeah, so it's striking. There are opposite interactions. So the stress gets us worse, makes you, let's say,
Starting point is 01:37:45 I think that if we were to just put a rough percentage on this, it would be somewhere between 10 and 30% worse at performance than the control group. And stress is enhancing is approximately equivalent improvement. So they're in opposite directions. Even more striking is the studies that Alice Lab did and others looking at, for instance, you give people a milk shake, you tell them it's a high calorie milk shake, has a lot of nutrients, and then you measure grellin secretion in the blood. And grellin is a marker of hunger that increases the longer it's been since you've eaten, and what you notice is that suppresses grellin to a great degree and for a long period of time. You give another group a shake, you tell them it's a low calorie shake,
Starting point is 01:38:25 that it's got some nutrients in it, but that doesn't have much fat, not much sugar, etc. They drink the shake, less reliance, suppression. And it's the same shake. And it's the same shake. And satiety lines up with that also in that study. And then the third one, which is also pretty striking is they took hotel workers, they gave them a short tutorial or not, informing them that moving around during the day and vacuuming and doing all that kind of thing is great helps you lower your BMI, which is great for your health.
Starting point is 01:38:50 You incentivize them. And then you let them out into the wild of their everyday job. You measure their activity levels. The two groups don't differ. They're doing roughly the same task, leaning down, cleaning out trash cans, et cetera. Guess what, the group that was informed
Starting point is 01:39:03 about the health benefits of exercise lose 12% more weight compared to the other group. And no difference in actual movement? Apparently not. Now, how could that be? I mean, literally this was sparked by Inalai's words. You know, this was sparked by her graduate advisor saying, what if all the effects of exercise are placebo?
Starting point is 01:39:26 Which is not what anyone really believes, but it's just such a, I love that anecdote that Ali told us, because it just really speaks to how really smart people think. They sit back and they go, yeah, exercise obviously has benefits, but what if a lot of the benefits are that you tell yourself it's good for you, and the brain can actually activate these mechanisms in the body.
Starting point is 01:39:45 And why wouldn't that be the case? Because the nervous system extends through both. So so interesting. OK, so fast forward to this study, which is really about belief effects, not placebo effects. And to make a long story short, we know that nicotine, vape smoked, dipped, or snuffed, or these lusin pouches,
Starting point is 01:40:06 or taken in capsule form, does improve cognitive performance. I'm not suggesting people run out and start doing any of those things. I did a whole episode on nicotine. The delivery device often will kill you. Some other way or is bad for you. But it causes vasoconstriction, which is also not good for certain people.
Starting point is 01:40:20 But nicotine is cognitive enhancing. Why? Well, you have a couple sites in the brain, namely, in the basal four brain, nucleus, psalis, in the back of the brain, structures, like locus, seruleus, but also this, what's called, it's got a funny name, the pedunculopontine nucleus, which is this nucleus in the ponds in the back of the brain, in the brain stem, that sends those little axon wires into the thalamus. The thalamus is a gateway for sensory information. And in the thalamus, the visual information,
Starting point is 01:40:49 the auditory information, it has nicotinic receptors. And when the pedunculopontine nucleus releases nicotine or when you ingest nicotine, what it does is it increases the signal to noise of information coming in through your senses. So the fidelity of the signal that gets up to your cortex, which is your conscious perception of those senses, is increased.
Starting point is 01:41:09 And how much endogenous nicotine do we produce? Ooh, well, it's going to be a fetal colon binding to nicotinic receptors. I see. We're not making nicotine. We're just binding. So this is a nicotinic acetylcholine receptor. Of which there are at least seven
Starting point is 01:41:23 and probably like 14 subtypes, but so Right, they're called nicotinic receptors in an annoying way in the same way that cannabinoid receptors are called cannabinoid receptors, but then everyone thinks oh, you know those receptors are there So because we're supposed to smoke pot or those receptors are there because we're supposed to ingest nicotine No, the drugs that we're used to study their receptor. That's right. Yeah, exactly Receptor is named after the drug. And so the important thing to know is that whether or not it's basal four brain, a
Starting point is 01:41:49 predunculopontine nucleus or a locustsulease, that at least in the brain, because we're not talking about muscle wear, acetylcholine does something else via nicotinic receptors, there in general, it just tends to be a signal to noise enhancer. And so for the non-engineering types out there, no problem. Signal to noise, just imagine I'm talking right now and there's a lot static in the background. There are two ways for you to be able to hear me more clearly. We can reduce the static, or I can increase the fidelity,
Starting point is 01:42:14 the volume and the clarity of what I'm saying, okay? For instance, and that's really what a Cedocolean does. That's why when people smoke a cigarette, they get that boost of nicotine, and they just feel clearer. It really works. The other thing that happens is the phallamous Sends information to a couple of places. First of all, it sends information to the reward centers of the brain The mesolimic reward pathway that releases dopamine and typically when nicotine is increased in our system dopamine goes up That's one of the reasons why nicotine is reinforcing. We just like it.
Starting point is 01:42:47 It's a, we seek it out. It has done beautiful experiments with honey bees, even where, you know, you put nicotine on certain plants or it comes from certain plants and they'll forage there more. You get, you know, the kind of like buzzed that was upon bed, upon, in any event. There's also an output from this thing, the thalamus, to the venture-medial prefrontal cortex, which is an area of the forebrain that really allows us to limit our focus and our attention for sake of learning. It allows us to pay attention.
Starting point is 01:43:12 This is the circuit. You talked about this in your fantastic podcast on stimulants. Yeah. Yeah, on ADHD. Yeah. Typically ADHD drugs, so things like Adderall, ViVance, methamphetamine for that matter, Riddlein. Yeah, why it's counterintuitive that a stimulant would be a treatment for someone with difficulty focusing.
Starting point is 01:43:34 Yeah, in young kids who have difficulty focusing, if you give them something they love, they're like a laser. And the reason is that venture medial prefrontal cortex circuit engages when the kid is interested and engage. But kids with ADD, ADHD tend to have a hard time engaging their mind for other types of tasks and other types of tasks are important for getting through life.
Starting point is 01:43:56 And it turns out that giving those stimulant drugs, in many cases, can enhance the function of that circuit and it can strengthen so that ideally, the kids don't need the drugs in the long run Oh, but that's not often the way that it plays out and there are other ways to get at this You know, there's now a big battle out there, you know is ADHD real is it not real? Of course it's real does every kid need ADHD meds No, are there other things like nutrition more playtime outside? Etc that can help improve their symptoms without drugs. Yes, is the combination of all those things together known to be most beneficial? Yes.
Starting point is 01:44:30 Are the dosages given too high and generally should be titrated down maybe. Some kids need a lot, some kids need a little. I probably just gained and lost a few enemies there. The point is that these circuits are hardwired circuits. Sorry, one other question Andrew. If my memory serves correctly, doesn't nicotine potentially have a calming effect as well? And that seems a bit counterintuitive to the focusing one.
Starting point is 01:44:57 Are they, is it a dose effect or a timing effect? How does that work? Yeah, it's a dosing effect. So the interesting thing about nicotine is that it can enhance focus in the brain, but in the periphery, it actually provides some muscle relaxation. So it's kind of the perfect drug if you think about it. Again, it's reflecting on this, how when we were growing up, people would smoke on a
Starting point is 01:45:18 smoking section on the plane. People smoke all the time and now hardly anyone smokes for all the obvious reasons. But yeah, it provides that really ideal balance between being alert, but being mellow and relaxed in the body. So hence it's reinforcing properties. Okay, this study is remarkable because what they did is they had people come into the laboratory, they gave them a vape pen. These are smokers. So these are experienced smokers. Typically there's a washout before they come in, so they're not smoking for a bit,
Starting point is 01:45:52 so they can clear their system of nicotine. And they measure. How long is that needed? Typically it's a couple of days. Oh, okay. Yeah. Which must be miserable for those people. They can't have nicorette gum or anything.
Starting point is 01:46:02 No, nothing. They must be dying. And I wonder how many cheat. But they can measure. They measure the noxide, right? They measure carbon noxide, and they're measuring nicotine right gum or anything. No, nothing. They must be dying. And I wonder how many cheat they can measure. They measure carbon dioxide, right? Yeah, they measure carbon dioxide and they're measuring nicotine in the blood as well. So they do a good job there.
Starting point is 01:46:11 So then what they do is they have them vape. And they're vaping either a low medium or high dose of nicotine. The doses just don't really matter because tolerance varies, etc. And then they are putting them into a functional magnetic resonance imaging machine. So where they can look at, it's really blood flow. It's really hemodynamic response. For those of you who want to know,
Starting point is 01:46:34 it's the ratio of the oxygenated to deoxygenated blood. Because when blood will flow to neurons that are active to give it oxygen, and then it's deoxygenated, and then there's a change in what's called the bold signal. So FMRI, when you see these hot spots in the brain, is really just looking at blood flow. And then there's some interesting physics around, and I'll probably get this wrong, but I'll take an attempt at it so that I get beat up a little bit by the physicists and engineers. Do you remember the right-hand rule?
Starting point is 01:47:00 Yep. Right? Okay, so do I have this right? Correct. The right-hand rule, if you put your thumb out with your index finger, your middle finger, your thumb facing up, I think that the thumb represents the charge, the direction of the charge, right? And then isn't the electromagnetic field is the downward facing figure? And then it's, do I have that right? I have to look at the action.
Starting point is 01:47:18 Okay, so what someone will look it up. But what you do is when you put a person's head in this big magnet and then you pulse the magnet, what happens is the oxygenated and deoxygenated blood, it interacts with the magnetic field differently and that difference in signal can be detected and you can see that in the form of activated brain areas. Yeah, I mean MRI all works by proton detection. So presumably there's a difference in the proton signal when you have high oxygen versus low oxygen concentration.
Starting point is 01:47:46 Yeah. That's right. And what they'll do is they'll pulse with the magnet because my understanding is that and this is definitely getting beyond my expertise, but that the spin orientation of the protons then it's going to relax back at a different rate as well. So by the relaxation at a different rate, you can also get not just resting state activation, like, oh, look at a banana, what areas of the brain light up, but you can look at connectivity between areas and how one area is driving the activity of another
Starting point is 01:48:14 area. So very, very powerful technique. So what they do is they put people in a scanner and then you'll like this because you are. Well, what are the limitations of FMRI in terms of, I mean, how fine is the resolution? I mean, where are the blind spots of the technique? So resolution, you can get down to sub-centimeter. They talk about it always in these papers as a voxels, which are these little cubic pixels, things,
Starting point is 01:48:39 sub-centimeter, but you're not gonna get down to millimeter. There are a number of little confounds that maybe we won't go into now that have been basically worked out over the last 10 years by doing the following. You can't just give somebody a stimulus compared to nothing. I'll just tell you the experiment. It was discovered, for instance, that when someone would move their right hand, because when you're in the MRI, I just went for one of these recently for clinical, not a problem, but just for a diagnostics hand. You're leaning back in you and you can move
Starting point is 01:49:07 your right hand a bit and they would see an area and motor cortex lighting up but what they noticed was that the area corresponding to the left hand was also lighting up. So what you really have to do is you have to look at resting state how much are they lighting up just to track that out. So now you'll always see resting state versus activation state. Yeah, wasn't there a really funny study done as a spoof maybe a decade ago that could have dead salmon into an MRI machine
Starting point is 01:49:36 and did an FM MRI of a dead salmon that demonstrated like some interesting signal. I didn't know that but it... We got to find this one for the show notes. Yeah, we should do one of these wild papers. There's what there are papers of people putting, don't do this, folks, putting elephants on LSD that were published in science and things like crazy experiments.
Starting point is 01:49:57 We should definitely do a crazy experiments, journal club. In any event, you can get a sense of which brain areas are active and when with fairly high spatial resolution, fairly high, and pretty good temporal resolution on the order of hundreds of milliseconds. But it's not ultra-fast because a lot of neural transmission is happening on the tens of milliseconds, especially when you're in talking about auditory processing. Okay.
Starting point is 01:50:24 So they put people into the scanner and then they give them essentially a task that's designed to engage the thalamus, known to engage the thalamus reward centers and the venture medial prefrontal cortex. And it's a very simple game you'll like this because you have a background in finance. You let people watch a market, you know,
Starting point is 01:50:43 okay, here's the stock market or you could say that price of P's, it doesn't really matter. It goes up, it goes down, and they're looking at squiggle line, then it stops, and then they have the option, but they have to pick one option. They're either going to invest a certain number of the hundred units that you've given them,
Starting point is 01:50:57 or they can short it. They can say, it's going to go down and try and make money on the prediction, it's going to go down. You could explain shorting better than I could, for sure. So depending on whether or not they get the prediction right or wrong, they get more points or they lose points and they're going to be rewarded in real money at the end of the experiment.
Starting point is 01:51:14 So this is going to engage this type of circuitry. Now remember, these groups were given a vape pen prior to this where they've vaped, what they were told is either a low medium or high dose of nicotine, and they do this task. The goal is not to get them to perform better on the task. The goal is to engage the specific brain areas that are relevant to this kind of error and reward type circuits.
Starting point is 01:51:39 And we know that this task does that. So that includes the thalamus. That includes the mesolimic reward pathway and dopamine. It includes the ventral medial prefrontal cortex. First of all, they measure nicotine in the blood. They are measuring how much people have raped. They were very careful about this. One of the nice things about the vape pen for the sake of experiment, and not recommending
Starting point is 01:51:58 people vape, but they can measure how much nicotine is left in the vape pen before after they can measure how long they inhaled, how long they held it in. There's a lot that you can do that's harder to do with a cigarette. Okay. They measured people's belief as to whether or not they got low medium or high amounts of nicotine. And it's- They were told.
Starting point is 01:52:18 They were told they got a- this is a low amount, a medium amount or a high amount. And then, of course, they looked at brain area activation during this task. And what they found was very straightforward. Sorry, they were all given the same amount. Yes, this is the sneak. I was gonna offer it as a punchline, but that's okay.
Starting point is 01:52:33 No, I think the cool thing about this experiment is that the subjects are unaware that they all got the exact same amount of relatively low nicotine containing vape pen. So they basically, and they're measuring it from their bloodstream. So they all have fairly low levels of nicotine, but one group was told you got a lot, one group was told you got a medium amount, and the other was told you got a little bit. Now, a number of things happen, but the most interesting things are the following. First of all, people's subjective feeling of being on the drug matches what they were told.
Starting point is 01:53:08 So if they were told, hey, this is a high amount of nicotine, like, yeah, it feels like a high amount of nicotine. And these are experienced smokers. If it was a medium amount, like, yeah, that feels like a medium amount. If it was a low amount, they think it was a low amount. Now that's perhaps not so surprising. That's your just a tricky thing.
Starting point is 01:53:23 That's the placebo and That's the placebo. Yeah. But if you look at the activation of the thalamus, in the exact regions where you would predict a cedocoline transmission to impact the function of the thalamus, these include areas like what's called the central median nucleus, the ventroposterior nucleus, the names that really don't matter, but these are areas involved in attention. It scales with what they thought they got in the vape pen, meaning if you were told that you got a low amount of nicotine, you got a little bit of activation in these areas,
Starting point is 01:53:54 if you were told that you got a medium amount of nicotine, and that's what you've vaped, that you had medium amounts or moderate amounts of activation, and if you were told you got high amounts of nicotine, you got a high degree of activation. And the performance on the task, believe it or not, scales with it somewhat. So keep in mind, everyone got the exact same amount of nicotine in reality.
Starting point is 01:54:19 So here, the belief effect isn't just changing what one subjectively experiences, oh, this is the effect of high nicotine or low nicotine. It actually is changing the way that the brain responds to the belief. And that to me is absolutely wild. Now, there are a couple of other things that could have confounded this. First of all, it could have been that if you believe you got a lot of nicotine, you're just faster, where you're reading the lines better, or your response time to hit the button is quicker. I tell you you have a drug that's
Starting point is 01:54:48 going to improve reaction time, you might believe that about nicotine. And so you're quicker on the trigger, and you're getting, they have a more activation as well. More activation. They rule that out. They also rule out the possibility. How do they rule that out? By looking at rates of pressing. And there was a different thing. Nothing. And in sensory areas of the brain that would represent that kind of difference, they don't see that. The other thing that is very clear is that the connection between the thalamus and the ventral medial prefrontal cortex, that pathway scales in the most beautiful way such that people that were told they had smoked a low or vaped a low amount of nicotine got a subtle activation of that pathway People that were told that they got a moderate amount of nicotine got a more robust activation of that pathway and the people
Starting point is 01:55:35 They were told that they got a high amount of nicotine in the vape pen saw a very robust activation of the thalamus to this Ventual prefrontal cortical pathway now of course This is all happening under the hood of the thalmas to this ventral prefrontal cortical pathway. Now of course, this is all happening under the hood of the skull, simply on the basis of what they were told and what they believed. And technically the FMRI is showing the activation of those two areas, and that's how you can infer the strength of that connection. That's right. There's a separate method called diffuser tensor imaging, which was developed, I believe out of the group in Minnesota. Minnesota has a very robust group in terms of neuroimaging that can measure activation
Starting point is 01:56:11 in fiber pathways. This is not that, but you can look at the timing of activation, and it's a known what we call monosynaptic pathway. So we haven't talked so much about figures here, but I guess if we were going to look at any one figure, and I can just describe it for the audience that's not paying, doesn't have the figure in front of them, the most important figure is figure two. Remember, I said I like to read the titles of figures,
Starting point is 01:56:40 which is that the belief about nicotine strength induced a dose dependent response in the phthalmous. Basically, if you and figure 2b can tell you if they believe that they got more nicotine, that's essentially the response that they saw. So if you look at the belief rating as a function of the estimate in thalamus of what, how much activation there was. It's a mess when you look at all the dots at once, but if you just separated out by high medium and low, you run the statistics, what you find is that
Starting point is 01:57:14 there's a gradual increase, but a legitimate one from low to medium to high. In other words, if I tell you, this is a high dose of nicotine, your brain will react as if it's a high dose of nicotine. Now, what they didn't do was give people zero nicotine. Yeah, it was about to say there's a control that's missing. Yeah, right.
Starting point is 01:57:32 Yeah, so what they didn't do is give people zero nicotine and then tell them this is a high amount of nicotine, sort of the equivalent of the cruel high school experiment. No alcohol, but then the kid acts drunk. Now, in the high school example, it's unclear whether or not the kid actually felt drunk or not. It's unclear whether or not they had been drunk previously if they even knew what it would be like to feel drunk, etc. and there's the social context. What I find just outrageous and outrageously interesting about this study is simply that what we are told
Starting point is 01:58:07 about the dose of a drug changes the way that our physiology responds to the dose of the drug. And in my understanding, this is the first study to ever look at dose dependence of belief effects. Right? And why would that be important? Well, for almost every study of drugs, you look at a dose-dependent curve. You look at zero, low dose, medium dose, high dose. And here they clearly are seeing a dose-dependent response simply to the understanding of what they expect the drug ought to do. In other words, you can bypass pharmacology somewhat, right? Now, look at figure 2B.
Starting point is 01:58:48 Am I reading this correctly? So it's got four bars on there. You've got the group who were told they got a low dose, the group who was told they got a medium dose, the group that was told they had a high dose, and then these healthy controls, who presumably were non-smokers who were just put in the machine. That's right.
Starting point is 01:59:07 You mean, yeah. Yeah, this is measuring parameter estimate. What is that referring to their ability to play the trading game? The parameter estimate is the activation, reward-related activities from independent to alimists mask, right? So what they're doing is they're just saying, if we just look at the salimists, what is the level, reward-related activities from independent to alimus mask, right? So what they're doing is they're just saying,
Starting point is 01:59:26 if we just look at the phalamus, what is the level of activation? I see. So this suggests that the only statistical difference was between the low and the high. That's right. And nobody else was statistically different. That's right.
Starting point is 01:59:41 But that's not the whole story? No, that's not the whole story. So when you look at the output from the phalus to the ventramedial prefrontal cortex, that's where you start to identify the ... Is that figure four? That is. Yes. So this is where you see ... So figure four B, if you look at parameter estimates, so this is the degree of activation between the phalamus and the ventramedial prefrontal cortex, and it's called the instructed belief. You can see that there's a low, medium, and high scatter of dots for each,
Starting point is 02:00:11 and that each one of those is significant. So isn't it interesting that at the thalamus, which is immediately appreciate my stupidity when it comes to neuroscience, which is more proximate to the nicotine or nicotine that nicotine, what do you call it? The nicotine acetylcholine receptor,
Starting point is 02:00:32 you have a lower difference of signal strength, and somehow that got amplified as it made its way forward in the brain. Yeah, so that surprised you? It is surprising, and it surprised them as well, that the interpretation they give, again, as we're talking about before, important to match their conclusions against what they actually found, which
Starting point is 02:00:51 is what we're doing here. The interpretation that they give is that it doesn't take much nicotinic receptor occupancy in the thalamus to activate this pathway. But they too were surprised that they could not detect a raw difference in the activation of thalamus, but in terms of its output to the prefrontal cortex, that's when they turn it.
Starting point is 02:01:10 Because that figure for B is more convincing than figure two, because even figure two E, if you read the fine print, the R, the correlation coefficient is 0.27. That's weak. That's strong. Right. It's weak. So at the thalamus, it's kind of like, yeah, there might be a signal. By the way, this goes back to our earlier discussion.
Starting point is 02:01:30 There could be a huge signal here and we're underpowered. How many subjects were in this? You wouldn't have a lot of subjects in this experiment. Yeah. No, and this just speaks to the general challenge of doing this kind of work. It's hard to get a lot of people in and through the scanner.
Starting point is 02:01:44 Yeah, and it's expensive. It's expensive. We in and through the scanner. Yeah, it's expensive. It's expensive. We have to, I should know this, but we can, we can go back to the, but you can sort of just look at the number of dots on here. I mean, it's in the low tens, right? It's like 40, 30, something like that. It's not, so it's possible you do this with, a day in your study. Yeah, you do this with a thousand people. This could all be statistically significant. Right.
Starting point is 02:02:04 It was, um, so they talk about this, you know, based on this, we estimate that an end of 20 end of sample size in each belief condition, the final sample would provide 90% power to detect an effect of this magnitude at an alpha of 0.5 in a two-tilled test. Okay, so that's them referring to what we just talked about, which is we believe at 90% confidence to get an alpha of 0.05, which is we believe at 90% confidence to get an alpha of 0.05, which means we'll want to be 95% confidence. We need 60 people, 20 per group, right? Yeah. But if the difference is smaller than what they expected, they'll miss out on some of the significance, which that looks like they're missing between the
Starting point is 02:02:38 medium and high group. And I too was surprised that they did not see a difference between the medium and the high group, but they did in the output of the thalamus. I too was surprised that they did not see a difference in the between the medium and the high group But they did in the output of the thalamus I was also surprised that they didn't see a difference This is kind of interesting in its own right if figure three talks about their belief about nicotine strength did not Modulate the reward response the dopamine response. How was that measured also just in FMR? Exactly so if you look at figure three, other people can't see it, but basically, what you'll see is that there's no difference between these different groups
Starting point is 02:03:11 in terms of the amount of activation in these reward pathways if people got a low medium or high amount of nicotine. Now, that actually could be leveraged, I believe, if somebody were trying to quit nicotine, for instance, and they were going to do that by progressively reducing the amount of nicotine that they were taking. But you told them that it was the same amount from one day to the next. You could whittle it down presumably to a low amount before taking it to zero.
Starting point is 02:03:38 And if they believed it to be a greater amount, then it might actually not disrupt their reward pathways. Meaning they would feel presumably they'd feel rewarded by whatever nicotine they were greater amount, then it might actually not disrupt their reward pathways, meaning they would feel presumably they'd feel rewarded by whatever nicotine they were bringing in. What would be your prediction if this experiment were repeated, but it was done exactly the same way with non-smokers? Whew. Well, one thing that's sort of interesting, you asked about potential sources of artifact,
Starting point is 02:04:05 problems with FMRI. One of the challenges that they note in this study was you have to stay very still in the machine, but the subjects were constantly coughing because they're smokers. So, okay, so presumably the data would be higher fidelity, sort of chuckling at that one, but I was like, I had to read that one twice, and that makes sense. They're smokers, they're coughing, they can't stay still. So movement artifact. But in all seriousness, I think that for people
Starting point is 02:04:31 that are naive to nicotine, even a small amount of nicotine is likely to get this pathway activated to such a great degree. Sort of like the first time effective pretty much any drug. But I wonder if they would be more or less susceptible to the belief system. Yeah, that's a really good question. Right, because they have no prior to compare it to.
Starting point is 02:04:51 They have no pleasant. They have no experience to compare it to with respect to the obviously beneficial effects of nicotine that the smokers are well used to. So this is the poor kid that got duped into thinking the non-alcoholic beer was at alcohol, though they're actually the winner we know because I didn't have so an alcohol alcohol is bad for you. So in the end that kid wins and the other ones lose poetic justice,
Starting point is 02:05:13 but that kid having never been actually drunk before, presumably, I would feel like it's been more susceptible. More susceptible potentially. That's my guess as well. So my glee for this experiment is not, or this paper rather, is not because I think it's the B all end all, or it's a perfect experiment. I just think it's so very cool that they're starting to explore dose dependence of belief, because that has all sorts of implications. I mean, use your imagination, folks.
Starting point is 02:05:45 Whether or not we're talking about a drug, we're talking about a behavioral intervention. We're talking about a vaccine, and I'm not referring to any one specific vaccine. I'm just talking to vaccines generally. I'm talking about psychoactive drugs. I'm talking about illicit drugs. I'm talking about antidepressants. I'm talking about all the sorts of drugs
Starting point is 02:06:07 we were talking about before, metformin, et cetera. Just throw our arms around all of it. What we believe about the effects of a drug, presumably, in addition to what we believe about how much we're taking and what those effects ought to be, clearly are impacting at least the way that our brain reacts to those drugs. Yeah, it's very interesting. I mean, when you consider how many drugs that have
Starting point is 02:06:33 peripheral effects or peripheral outputs begin with central issues. So again, I think the GLP1 agonist are such a great example of this. Is that right? Yeah. Yeah. You know, I don't think anybody fully understands exactly how they're working, but it's hard to argue that they're impacting, that the GLP1 analog is having a central impact. It's doing something in the brain that is leading to a reduction of appetite. We believe that. Yeah. Yeah.
Starting point is 02:07:04 And I think the mouse data point to different areas of the hypothalamus that are leading to a reduction of appetite. I believe that. Yeah. And I think the mouse data point to different areas of the hypothalamus that are related to satiety, that it's at least possible. Yeah. I mean, there's no quicker way to make a mouse overeater underneath than by lesioning its hypothalamus, depending on where you do so. So presumably these drugs work there.
Starting point is 02:07:23 But again, it speaks to like what do you need to believe in order for that to be the case? Have they done placebo trials there where people get something and they're told? Oh, these are, I mean, of course, those drugs have all been tested via placebo and the placebo groups, you know, don't do anywhere near as well.
Starting point is 02:07:39 That's how we know that there's activity of the drug. But again, there's, you know, that's a little bit different than being told you are absolutely getting it, right? Because in the RCTs, you're just told you might be getting it, you might not be getting it. So it's not quite the same as this experiment. This experiment is one level up where you're being told, no, you're absolutely getting
Starting point is 02:08:01 it. You're just getting different doses of it. Yeah, to take this to maybe the ADHD realm, let's say a kid has been on ADHD meds for a while, and the parents for whatever reason, the physician decide they want to cut back on the dosage, but if they were to tell the kid it's the same dosage, they've always been taking, and it's had a certain positive effect for them. According to the results at least in this paper, which are not definitive, but are interesting. The lower dose may be as effective
Starting point is 02:08:28 simply on the basis of belief and, and this is the part that makes it so cool to me, is that, and it's not a kid tricking themselves or the parents, tricking the kid so much as the brain activation is corresponding to the belief. Right, so that's where this is. This is why because it's done in the brain activation is corresponding to the belief, right? So that's where this is. This is why because it's done in the brain, I think we can, you know,
Starting point is 02:08:49 it gets to these kind of abstract, nearly mystical, but not quite mystical aspects of belief effects, which is that, you know, your brain is a prediction-making machine, it's a data interpretation machine, but it's clear that one of the more important pieces of data are your beliefs about how these things impact you. So it's not that this bypasses physiology.
Starting point is 02:09:11 People aren't deluding themselves. The thalamus is behaving as if it's a high dose when it's the same dose as the low dose group. Wild. Yeah, I mean, I think of the implications, for example, of blood pressure. Right, like we don't really understand essential hypertension, which is the majority of people walking around with high blood pressure. It's unclear etiology. So lots of people being treated. How do we know that the belief system about it can't be changed? And yeah, this is, I don't know, this is eye opening. Yeah. It's cool stuff. And Ali Krum is onto some other really cool stuff. Like, for instance, just to highlight where these belief effects are starting to show up. If you tell a group that the side effects of a drug that they're taking are evidence
Starting point is 02:09:57 that the drug really works for the purpose that they're taking it. Even though those side effects are kind of annoying, people report the experience is less awful, and they report more relief from the primary symptoms that they're trying to target. So I believe about what side effects are. That's really impacting. We can really impact how quickly and how compatible we feel about, how quickly a drug works, excuse me, and how compatible we feel that drug is with our entire life. So wait if we call them something else, like not side effects, but like additional benefits
Starting point is 02:10:28 or something. It's kind of crazy. And you don't want to lie to people, obviously, but you also don't want to send yourself in the opposite direction, which is reading the list of side effects of a drug and then developing all of those side effects went and then maybe later coming to the understanding that some of those were raised through belief effects. We definitely see that, that's the no-cebo effect, right? That's the one we see a lot, you know, with all sorts of drugs. And it's tough because, you know,
Starting point is 02:10:59 how do you know which is which? And I think there are some people who are really impacted by that and it makes it very difficult for them to take any sort of pharmacologic agent because they basically, they can't help but incur every possible side effect. Is it true that medical students often will start developing the symptoms of the different diseases that they're learning about? Is that true? Well, you know, I'll tell you, I do think that in medical school, you start to think of the zebras more than the horses
Starting point is 02:11:28 all the time. You know, like, you know, and I'm referring to, right? You know, you see footprints, you see hoof prints, you should think of horses, but of course, medical students, you only think of the zebras. There are some really funny things in medical school. Like, there are certain conditions
Starting point is 02:11:43 that you spend so much time thinking about that you have a very warped sense of their prevalence. In medical school, there's this condition called sarcoidosis. I feel like we never stop talking about sarcoidosis. I've seen like three cases in my life. It's just not that common. Does it provide a great teaching tool or something? I don't know. Like, I just, some of these things I don't know. How much time did we spend talking about cytos and verses? This is when people embryologically have a reversed rotation
Starting point is 02:12:12 and everything in their body is flipped. Literally, everything is flipped. So their heart is on the right side, their liver is on the left side, their appendix is on the left side. And so I'm not making this up. What's the problem with this? I've never seen it, okay? on the left side, there are appendixes on the left side. And so I'm not making this up. A compliment is this. I've never seen it, okay?
Starting point is 02:12:28 I was thinking about boxing in the liver shot. Like, you could easily be going for the wrong side of the body. No, I swear to God, like as a medical student, if you were told someone had left-sided lower quadrant pain, to which the answer is almost assuredly that they have diverticulitis, you'd think, they could have appendicitis
Starting point is 02:12:44 in the context of cytosin versus. Like the fact that that would even register in the top 10 things that it could possibly be. But yes, you just have a totally warped sense of what's out there. Oh, man. Well, this has been pure pleasure for me. I don't know about you.
Starting point is 02:13:00 Yeah, this is the best. I don't know about our listeners, but for me, this is among the things that I just delight in and even more so, I don't know about you. I don't know about our listeners, but for me, this is among the things that I just delight in and even more so, because you're the one across the table for me teaching me about these incredible findings. And the gaps in those findings,
Starting point is 02:13:14 which are equally incredible because they're equally important to know about. Yeah, so let's do this again in Austin. Absolutely, next time on your home court. Very well. And bring a little bit of that, dude, if you've got, yeah. Yeah. Yeah, I'll bring a low medium and high Medium and high. Thanks Peter. You're the best. Thanks. Thank you for listening to this week's episode of The Drive. It's extremely important to me to provide all of this content without relying on paid ads.
Starting point is 02:13:45 without relying on paid ads. To do this, our work is made entirely possible by our members, and in return, we offer exclusive member-only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free. So if you want to take your knowledge of this space to the next level, it's our goal to ensure members get back much more than the price of this subscription. Premium membership includes several benefits. First, comprehensive podcast show notes that detail every topic, paper, person, and thing that we discuss in each episode. And the word on the street is nobody's show notes rival owners. Second, monthly ask me anything or AMA episodes. These episodes are comprised of detailed responses to subscribe questions, typically focused on a single topic and are
Starting point is 02:14:25 designed to offer a great deal of clarity and detail on topics of special interest to our members. They'll also get access to the show notes for these episodes, of course. Third, delivery of our premium newsletter, which is put together by our dedicated team of research analysts. This newsletter covers a wide range of topics related to longevity and provides much more detail than our free weekly newsletter. Fourth, access to our private podcast feed that provides you with access to every episode including AMAs, Sans, Lishbeel, you're listening to now, and in your regular podcast feed.
Starting point is 02:15:00 Fifth, the Qualies, an additional member-only podcast we put together that serves as a highlight reel featuring the best excerpts from previous episodes of the drive. This is a great way to catch up on previous episodes without having to go back and listen to each one of them. And finally, other benefits that are added along the way. If you want to learn more and access these member-only benefits, you can head over to PeterAtiaMD.com forward slash subscribe. You can also find me on YouTube, Instagram and Twitter, all with the handle Peter AtiaMD. You can also leave us, review on Apple podcasts or whatever podcast player you use.
Starting point is 02:15:38 This podcast is for general informational purposes only and does not constitute the practice of medicine, nursing, or other professional healthcare, including the giving of medical advice. No doctor-patient relationship is formed. The use of this information and the materials linked to this podcast is that the user's own risk. The content on this podcast is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment. Users should not disregard or delay an obtaining medical advice from any medical condition they have, and they should seek the assistance of their healthcare professionals for any such conditions. Finally, I take all conflicts of interest very seriously. For all of my disclosures and the companies I invest in or advise,
Starting point is 02:16:21 please visit peteratimd.com forward slash about where I keep an up-to-date and active list of all disclosures. you

There aren't comments yet for this episode. Click on any sentence in the transcript to leave a comment.