The Peter Attia Drive - #368 ‒ The protein debate: optimal intake, limitations of the RDA, whether high-protein intake is harmful, and how to think about processed foods | David Allison, Ph.D.
Episode Date: October 13, 2025View the Show Notes Page for This Episode Become a Member to Receive Exclusive Content Sign Up to Receive Peter’s Weekly Newsletter David Allison is a world-renowned scientist and award-winning s...cientific writer who has spent more than two decades at the forefront of obesity research. In this episode, David joins for his third appearance on The Drive to bring clarity to one of the most contentious topics in modern nutrition—protein. He explores the historical pattern of demonizing macronutrients, the origins and limitations of the RDA for protein, and what the evidence really says about higher protein intake, muscle protein synthesis, and whether concerns about harm are supported by actual data. He also discusses the challenges of conducting rigorous nutrition studies, including the limits of epidemiology and crossover designs, as well as conflicts of interest in nutrition science and why transparency around data, methods, and logic matter more than funding sources. The episode closes with a discussion on processed and ultra-processed foods, the public health challenges of tackling obesity, and whether future solutions may depend more on drugs like GLP-1 agonists or broader societal changes. This is part one of a two-part deep dive on protein, setting the stage for next week’s conversation with Rhonda Patrick. We discuss: The cyclical pattern of demonizing different macronutrients in nutrition and why protein has recently become the latest target of controversy [3:15]; The origin and limits of the protein RDA: from survival thresholds to modern optimization [6:30]; Trust vs. trustworthiness: why data, methods, and logic matter more than motives in science [13:30]; The challenges of nutrition science: methodological limits, emotional bias, and the path to honest progress [17:15]; Why the protein RDA is largely inadequate for most people, and the lack of human evidence that high protein intake is harmful [30:30]; Understanding the dose-response curve for muscle protein synthesis as protein intake increases [45:15]; Why nutrition trials are chronically underpowered due to weak economic incentives, and how this skews evidence quality and perceptions of conflict [48:15]; The limitations and biases of nutrition epidemiology, and the potential role of AI-assisted review to improve it [56:15]; The lack of compelling evidence of harm with higher protein intake, and why we should shift away from assuming danger [1:04:15]; Pragmatic targets for protein intake [1:09:30]; Defining processed and ultra-processed foods and whether they are inherently harmful [1:16:15]; The search for a guiding principle of what’s healthy to eat: simple heuristics vs. judging foods by their molecular composition [1:25:00]; Why conventional public health interventions for obesity have largely failed [1:38:15]; Two ideas from David for addressing the metabolic health problem in society [1:42:30]; The potential of GLP-1 agonists to play a large role in public health [1:46:30]; and More. Connect With Peter on Twitter, Instagram, Facebook and YouTube
Transcript
Discussion (0)
Hey, everyone. Welcome to the Drive podcast. I'm your host, Peter Attia. This podcast, my website, and my weekly newsletter all focus on the goal of translating the science of longevity into something accessible for everyone.
Our goal is to provide the best content in health and wellness, and we've established a great team of analysts to make
this happen. It is extremely important to me to provide all of this content without relying on
paid ads. To do this, our work is made entirely possible by our members, and in return, we offer
exclusive member-only content and benefits above and beyond what is available for free. If you want to
take your knowledge of this space to the next level, it's our goal to ensure members get back
much more than the price of the subscription. If you want to learn more about the benefits of our premium
membership, head over to peteratia-md.com forward slash subscribe.
My guest this week is David Allison.
David, returning for his third conversation on the drive, is a world-renowned scientist,
an award-winning scientific writer who has been at the forefront of obesity research for the last
20 years, and is currently the director of the Children's Nutrition Research Center at Baylor
College of Medicine.
I wanted to have David on because protein has been.
become one of the most contentious and confusing topics in nutrition today. What was once a fairly
straightforward subject has now turned into a debate full of conflicting claims, dogma,
unnecessary, controversy, and a whole lot of name calling. David brings both a deep understanding
of the science and a clear-eyed perspective on how to separate evidence from opinion.
This is part one of a two-part deep dive on protein, and next week I'll be joined by Rhonda Patrick
for part two, after which we'll put this protein discussion to rest once and for all.
In this episode, we discuss the historical cycle of demonizing macronutrients and why protein has
recently become the focus. The origins and limitations for the RDA for protein and what the
evidence suggests about optimal intake for health, longevity, and performance. Conflicts of interest
in nutrition science and why transparency around data, methods, and logic matter much more than
funding sources. The challenges of conducting high-quality nutrition studies, including the debate
over crossover designs, the limits of epidemiology, and the underfunding of rigorous trials
compared to pharmaceutical trials. What the evidence really says about higher protein intake,
muscle protein synthesis, and whether concerns about harm are supported by actual data. How to think
about processed and ultra-processed foods, including definitions, heuristics, and the question of whether
they're inherently harmful or simply a convenient villain. And finally, the difficulty of tackling
obesity through public health, the limits of current approaches, and whether future solutions may
rely more on drugs like GLP-1 agonists or broader societal changes. So without further delay,
please enjoy my conversation with David Allison.
Hey, David. Thanks for coming back.
This was probably the shortest trip you've made here, right?
You've got a new home?
Yeah, I got a great new gig at the Children Nutrition Research Center in Houston, Texas,
and Baylor College of Medicine in Texas Children's Hospital.
I'm having a great time.
So it was just a nice, easy car right over here.
Very well.
Well, we're going to actually start by talking about something that, believe it or not,
I don't want to talk about because I'm kind of sick and tired of talking about it,
and I'm going to apologize in advance to all the listeners because if they've been listening
at all,
sick and tired of hearing about this. But unfortunately, this is a topic that has gone from being
what I would consider pretty straightforward to somewhat contentious. And I'm going to do my
best to refrain from speculating on the reasons why it's become contentious, although I have
many views on that, and many views on the people who choose to make it contentious, which I'll also
refrain from. But let's just try to dive into the arguments around the macronutrient that is more
in the crosshairs than any other today, which is protein. And that's kind of interesting when you
consider the arc of your career. It was certainly easy to understand how people demonized fat
and then they demonized carbs. And here we are today. Come in full circle. We're demonizing protein.
Where do you place that in the arc of the historical lens of nutrition? It shows many things,
but at some level it's almost perfectly predictable, which is we all eat. We eat every day.
Eating is part of our sustenance, but it's part of culture, family, certainly part of economics,
identity, social class, religion, and so on. And so it's fun to talk about. And there's lots of
motivations. Motivations people recognize and motivations they may not recognize. And that leads
always to this attention on it. That attention drives a big economic engine of food sales.
So there's lots of interest in this and lots of stakes in this, if you'll pardon the pun.
So people shift, and it's not even just the macronutrients.
This week it's seed oils, and next week it's phytoestrogens and soybean feminizing youth,
and it's one thing after another where people look for the villains and the heroes
and the angels and the demons in food.
Only three macronutrients, so they keep looping around.
Protein has become, in the last few years, almost a fever pitch of enthusiasm and excitement
from one part of the community, and it's driving.
sales and it's driving behavior and some people like me are having fun with it. And then I think
there's always that group that sees other people having fun or making money and heaven forbid
doing something that might be seen as the easy way out or the contrived or constructed way out
as opposed to the so-called natural way out of doing something. And then that upsets them,
that offends them. You're not being prudent. You're not taking the natural course. You're
not taking the old-fashioned course. You're having too much fun. You're trying too hard
to achieve big things. We don't like that. So we're going to try to poo-poo it or shut it down
or minimize it. And I think that's where things are. So presumably at some point, this will no
longer be relevant and no one will talk about it and we'll move back to fats being the bad thing.
Although I guess we're kind of there with seed oils to your point. But let's talk a little
bit about protein. So a lot of the consternation stems, I think, from a debate, are
around the RDA and the recommendation of 0.8 grams per kilogram of body weight.
Do you want to tell folks a little bit about where that came from?
Where does this ubiquitous recommendation that we consume 0.8 grams of protein per kilogram
of body weight?
So again, just to put that in perspective, I weigh 180 pounds.
So that is probably 82 kilos.
So I should be eating about 60 to 65 grams of protein, according to the,
RDA. It's the middle of the day. I've had 60 grams of protein today, so I could basically
stop eating protein for the rest of the day now, right? Yeah, some people would say that. Now,
others, I don't know if I would go so far, say, at the other extreme, but a more active
proponent of the importance of not only greater amounts of protein, but particular types
and distributions and so on would be somebody like Don Lehman. And he might say, I hope I'm not
inappropriately putting words in his mouth, but I think he might say, Peter, you actually need
to be eating protein at least three or four times a day. Yeah, because that 60 was in one sitting,
but... Right. And at each of those sittings, you probably want to hit about 30. I emphasize
that word about. It's not like there's some mathematical proof that there's some hard threshold,
and certainly not for you who is 20% bigger than me, that your hard threshold be the same as my
hard threshold. But in the neighborhood of 30, right off the bat, if you're not, if you're not
you follow that advice, you'd need to be having double that amount of protein, and you'd have
to have it distributed differently. And if you haven't distributed the way you currently did, it'd be
more than double. So I think there are a lot of people who disagree with that. The history is
people at some point recognized that we needed protein to live. The key indicator of that
was nitrogen. And people looked at nitrogen balance. How much did you take in? How much did you excrete?
and they found that people could achieve nitrogen balance,
or at least sort of ordinary normal people of the time,
at about that level.
No one ever proved, demonstrated, or I think even claimed,
that that was the best amount or the upper limit,
or it was just that's probably enough to maintain nitrogen balance,
which probably means it's compatible with survival.
And if you think about the early days, that was pretty important.
Europe couldn't reliably feed its population until two things were entered into Europe.
One was guano, bat poop, fertilizer, and the other was potatoes.
And when after the Columbus hit Hispaniola, and eventually what came back was, among other things,
guano and potatoes, suddenly Europe could feed its population.
But it took a while for the potatoes to catch on.
Louis XIV actually wore potato butanier to get people to think they were kind of safe
two polar scientists in 1928 published a paper in which they had two young people.
They never said they were the young people, but I kind of suspected perhaps they were.
One man, one woman, and for six months they fed them nothing but potatoes,
a little bit of fat to cook the potatoes in, and a little bit of fruit to avoid deficiencies.
So the only source of protein for practical purposes was the potato,
and what they showed was nitrogen balance was perfectly fine.
And despite sort of this quote unquote bad carbohydrate, at least some people think it's a bad carbohydrate, no one got diabetes.
They didn't gain weight.
How many calories did they eat a day?
Don't remember that, but they were sort of roughly normal weight thin people of the time.
So probably I would guess a little more than 2,000, 2,500 somewhere in that neighborhood.
As I said, they did nitrogen balance studies and they were perfectly fine.
But that's entirely different than saying what if they were older people?
or what if they were pregnant or what if they were recovering from a bicep tendon tear
or what if they were bodybuilding those are all very different things or what if they were active
yeah so when we go back and look at some of the USDA based studies on this topic and we
wrote about this so the subjects for this study were if my memory serves correctly lean
inactive sedentary young men who are I think
I think you said about 150 pounds, if I recall.
Yep.
Yeah, maybe even a bit lighter.
But yeah, about that.
So these are guys that weighed 65 to 70 kilos, very inactive, and nitrogen balance was
demonstrated in them that you could achieve it at 0.8 grams of protein per kilogram.
So again, let's just do the math, make it easy.
Say 50 grams of protein was able to keep these folks in nitrogen balance.
It's interesting that everything you said, this is true of, I think, clinical research.
You have to look at the population that is studied and ask the question, how do I differ
from that population?
Am I bigger?
Am I training?
Do I have a more ambitious goal than not dying or not wasting away?
And I don't want to minimize those goals because you alluded to the fact that for the vast
majority of human history, not dying was an amazing goal.
Living to the next day, to the next harvest, to the next season was essential for 99.99% of human history.
So this idea that being optimal or thriving, it's a very, very modern luxury we have.
It's both modern and ancient all at once.
So yes, you are correct.
But also the idea that there are different kinds of goals that we optimize is actually the very nature of evolution.
what's being selected. And Steve Simpson, David Robinheimer, from Down Under, from Australia,
study something called the Protein Leverage Hypothesis. We may want to come back to at some point.
What they've shown is that what at least some animals do in experimental settings they're able to set up
is they consume enough protein to optimize their genetic fitness, meaning how many of their genes
they're able to transmit to the next generation, which is, if you want to win the evolutionary
game, that's your goal, which is different than living a lot longer. That's winning the personal
game, but not your genes games. So, yes, it matters, and it may be that we now want to shift
a little from one toward the other, and it may even change during the course of life. It may be that
at one point in life, my goal is to be as perhaps big and strong as I can. Maybe at another point in
life, it's too slow aging. I may have different strategies for those two different things.
Before we go any further, I should have done this at the outset, but I realize that some people
are going to be watching us on video and they won't hear the introduction where I'll have made this
point. But we should both disclose that we're involved with a company called David Protein
that makes protein bars. Because we're going to be talking about protein today and unrelated,
but related, we're also going to talk about processed food, which I think is a very interesting
topic. This is a company that makes high protein bars, which are by definition processed. And so I just
want to make sure everybody listening understands that I'm involved in that company. You're an
advisor as well. And I'd like to hear your thoughts to the argument that says, well, Peter and
David, you guys can't really have an open and honest discussion about this topic because you have
this conflict of interest. I mean, I have my own thoughts on an argument like that, but I'd like to
hear yours. Sure. Just a couple of factual points of clarification. So yes, I have a
have a grant from and have been a paid advisor to the David Protein Company. Second, the David
and David Protein is not my David. I don't own the company. I like the bars. I eat them.
And every time I take one out at a meeting or something, someone will look at me and say,
how vain are you? You have your own personalized bars with David printed in Big La? I said,
no, it's not me. It's Michelangelo's David. The idea is you eat the bar, you look like that
date. But I've also, we had a protein conference. We ran about six months ago. It was amazingly
successful. The degree of interest from academia and industry and others are tremendous. We must have
had somewhere on the order of 50 different companies contribute. So I'll disclose that as well.
So I've a lot of interest in this. I've had funding from National Catalan's Beef Association,
pork producers, and other groups with interests and protein. In terms of the idea of does that make
us trustworthy or not, I distinguish that from trusted. Whether it makes us trusted, that's somebody
else's judgment. Trust me or don't trust me, however much you want, that's up to you.
you. Trustworthy, I think, has to do with the processes. And my colleagues and I, we have a saying
we've sort of coined and we like to use a lot. And we say, in science, three things matter. The data,
the methods used to collect the data, which give them their probative value, which shows what they
mean, and the logic connecting the data to conclusions. And everything else is tangential.
And so some people who don't have, quote unquote, the goods on an argument, will resort to
other things. They'll resort to ad hominem attacks, they'll resort to innuendo, they'll resort to
quips. Quips are great. Innuendo and outhominum attacks, not so great, in my view. But
none of those are dispositive. And when you think about things we can really declare things known
or not known, no one needs to argue about your conflicts of interest if you say that I can prove
that there's a greatest prime number. And people say, well, no, you could prove there isn't.
And I don't have to say maybe you're paid for by the prime number company or something.
I can just say, here's the proof and you're wrong and there's no point in discussing anything else.
The prime number company.
Think of the value, David, of prime numbers if they were finite.
Can you imagine how much the value of three, five, seven, 11, 13, like those numbers would increase in value so much more if they became finite?
I think today of, like Bitcoin.
Decoding and encoding Bitcoin.
Yeah.
Yeah. That's a very elegant explanation. And I think it's worth reiterating that point, which is at the end of the day, the three things that matter are what are the data? How were the data collected? What were the methods used to collect them? And then what is the string of logic that connects those data to their conclusions? And all of these things should be quite transparent. Now, you've chosen a career, a field of inquiry, in which it can be more difficult.
to do all of the above than in, say, genetics or biochemistry or particle physics, where one of
those steps is, in your case, particularly difficult, and that is the manner in which data
are collected. In other words, I don't think nutrition scientists are at a loss for logic,
but where I think they struggle, if they're studying humans at least, is collecting these data
can be really challenging, really expensive. The species of interest is not amenable to close
quarters for long periods of time, which is how you would obviously run a controlled experiment
in a biological setting. So this is maybe more of a philosophical question, but what is the future
of nutrition science? We're going to come back to the main topic, but this is just such an interesting
tangent. Do you believe that there is a much brighter future, a step function and improvement
and the quality of nutrition science that lies ahead with AI, synthetic data collection or
creation, rather, is there something that could fundamentally change nutrition science in
terms of how we go about gathering data so that we can be potentially less reliant on epidemiology,
which I'm sure we will discuss the shortfalls of today?
I think the answer is things will get better.
Whether it's a step function or not, I'm not so sure.
I want to expand or add to the branch you've thrown in and add a parallel branch, which is,
I think there are two reasons why nutrition science is so fraught. One you've pointed out is the
methodologic challenge. Can we collect the kind of data we really want with the kind of methods
we really want? The other part is the social aspect, which you've hinted at until we've gotten
to this point, which is why is it so emotional? Why do people tack each other? Why do people go beyond
the data? And I think I see that in any area, the more that area of inquiry,
is related to economics, religion, social values, personal experiences, the more you get
the emotion and the deviation from logic and so on.
And we see it in whatever people study, child rearing, same-sex marriage, anything that
has that emotional valence that everyday experience and so on leads to more bringing in
of non-scientific points of view.
So I think that's something we have very strongly.
And then I think the other we have is the methodologic challenge of collecting the data.
I see benefits on both fronts, but I think both will be slow.
I don't think in many cases it's going to be a simple thing of, if we could just fix that,
if we could just figure out how to measure food intake and free living people well, and
we're on the horizon, and everything will be okay.
That's important.
I hope we do figure out how to measure food intake well and free living people, but that
alone will not be a solution, a sufficient solution. What I think on the front of the emotional
piece is it's going to come slow. When you look at the arc of history of much of human endeavor,
at least from my point of view and the point of view of, I think people like Stephen Levitt
and so on, you look over the long haul. Things are always getting better. You smooth the
function a little bit. Murder rates are way down. Violence rates are way down. Education rates are way
up, lifespan is up, et cetera, et cetera. Freedom is up. But there are lots of ebb and flow. So we may be
in a little bit of an ebb and flow now, but I do hope that things will get better as they
always have, some more and more rationality. And that's something that as a scientist I feel very
strongly about. As scientists, we focus too much on immediate trust in science and we're saying,
we need to get more trust in science and on immediate issues. How do we get trust in this
issue about vaccines or drugs or what have you, instead of saying, how do we get trust in the
scientific process? How do we maybe risk losing a battle? Maybe I'm not going to convince
people today that what I think about food additives or vaccines or protein or something is, quote
unquote, the right answer. And I'll have to live with that. But if I can convince them that I'm
an honest broker and here's how science works and we can work to get through science, in the long
run, I think that's better. And I think that's coming and something we need to focus on. In terms of
the nutrition per se, we have so many challenges. But as a methodologist, I like challenges. Job
security, and it's fun. I like figuring things out. So we can't randomize to everything. So how do we
get causal inference? We can't blind every aspect of diet. And if we could, we'd be missing
some of what we're trying to study because some of the effects of diet involve the effects of
perceiving what you're eating. Some of the effects of this drink I'm drinking is how it tastes.
And if you blind me to it, then you've taken away that potential effect. There are issues of
measurement. How do you know what I really ate? There are issues of adherence. If you tell me to
drink one of these every day, do I actually do it? Do I drink one and only one, as you've
instructed? There are issues of duration. Could you get me to drink it now and measure something
in me 15 minutes later? Sure. Could you get me to drink one every day for the next 20
years and measure stuff? Difficult. The issues of the model organism. One thing is, we're going
to talk about longevity a little bit. Somebody once said to me, you never want to study longevity
in an organism that lives as long as you do. It's a bit of a challenge. If you and I, at our
age, were to start, especially mine, I'm a little older than you, were to start a big study
and say, I want to study 20-year-olds and give them different nutrition and see who lives longer,
and that'll help me figure out from myself what to eat, I'll be dead long before the study is in
and not be able to benefit from it and also not be able to find the answer. And maybe, you know,
we want answers before 60 years from now. So those are just a few, and I could go on and on.
Those are just a few of the many challenges we have in nutrition. And I think we're going to chip away
at them, but a lot of it's going to have to be settling for various rough inferences to say,
this information, I need to recognize its limits. I need to be honest with the public about the
limits. I need to say, I haven't shown this unequivocally, but it looks like this is the most
reasonable answer now or the most supported answer now. And I'm willing to accept that. But let's
be honest. It's not demonstrated. Great example of that. You're seeing the feud in the literature
now between Kevin Hall and David Ludwig on the use of crossover designs as an example.
and crossover designs in which you give person, let's say, Diet A followed by Diet B,
and you give other people a randomized to Diab, B followed by Diet A,
they have an Achilles heel that is not there for, let's say, parallel groups where some get
diet A and some get Diabee.
And that is you can have what's called carryover effects.
And it turns out, I've started to study this, tip my hat to David Ludwig, who pointed
the issues out to me and I hadn't fully comprehended them before that.
There's almost no way around it.
even with a washout between the crossover?
Not an absolute way.
You get into what I call argument land.
You can say, come on, David, it was a blinded drug I gave, and I know the kinetics of it,
and I know it's out of the system by this date.
How could it possibly be having this?
And I could say, well, Peter, you're probably right.
It's a good argument, but the validity still depends on your argument.
It's not an absolute out-priority proof.
And if I said, well, maybe what the drug did is it permanently changed something.
in that person. And we should just explain for listeners why this is an important discussion
in science, especially in human trials and especially in human trials with nutrition, because
there's a real statistical power, and I don't use that in the beta sense of the word, but a
statistical gift that comes from being able to do a crossover in that you can now leverage a student
test, for example, as a very powerful statistical tool that allows you to use fewer subjects and
therefore a fraction of the cost. So I'm guessing, I don't follow this debate, by the way,
but I'm going to guess that you're going to say Kevin Hall favors a crossover, not to speak for
Kevin, but I would bet the reason Kevin favors it is because the type of work Kevin does is insanely
expensive. He's putting patients in metabolic chambers and therefore the fewer patients that he
needs to do that with the easier he can do his work. Is that basically the argument?
You're absolutely correct. And when you said statistics in the power, rather,
in the beta sense, that is what it is. It is statistical power, the probability of rejecting
the null hypothesis if the null hypothesis is false, which is one minus beta, the type 2 error rate.
There's actually a little bit more, but I think 90 plus percent of the motivation is what
you've described. The other, as actually Kevin pointed out to me in some dialogue we were
having, was sometimes it's just throughput. Even if I had infinite money, I can't put people
through the procedure fast enough because there's only so many chambers.
Someone else pointed out to me, it could be patient availability.
If you said, I'm studying this in this rare population, I can't get a thousand people
even if had the money because they don't exist.
I can only get 10 or 20 people.
So all of those things strongly favor the crossover, which in almost all circumstances
will be much more statistically powerful, meaning you can get the same amount of precision
out of many fewer subjects.
But the problem is that you have this carryover
and so that the difference between the two groups
who get treatment A and treatment B in the second period
could be a function of the true effect of treatment A
versus treatment B in the second period
or could be an effect of what treatment A and treatment B did
in the first period carrying over.
And now you don't have a clean estimate
of the effect of treatment A or treatment B anymore.
Now, what you can do is what you said is a washout.
If you said, I understand how this thing works, and it's a molecular effect, it's not a
social effect, it's not learning, it's not a surgical thing, and then cut a piece of their
anatomy out that doesn't reverse.
This thing is completely reversible.
It's completely blinded.
I gave a long enough washout.
Then, by that argument, you can say I rule it out.
But you can never say absolutely rule it out.
If you're dealing with things other than that, where you can't have a long enough.
long enough washout, or it might be a psychosocial effect or a learning effect or some
permanent effect. Anything from bariatric surgery to something like an allergen, which may permanently
sensitize the body. I know people say, MRNA vaccines. Maybe the MRNA hangs around for a while.
Is that a permanent effect? Or a vaccine itself is a, we hope in some cases is a permanent
effect, like a measles vaccine. All of those things, they're not going to wash out. So those are
things where the crossover has a limit. And the question is, does that mean you try to do what
David Ludwig is, I think, arguing again, I hope I'm not inappropriately putting words in his
mouth, is saying they're just invalid, just either don't use them at all, or you can only use them
in this way. And if you get what looks like it might be a period by treatment interaction,
then discard the study, it's invalid, so on. Or do you say, which I think Kevin would say,
and I would side with is accept the limitations of the study. It doesn't mean it's a flawed
or incorrect study. It means it's a limited study. And as long as you point those limitations
out, you may or may not want to accept it. In the same sense, if you have an observational
epidemiologic study. I think most of us, again, they're exceptions, but most of us would not say
never do one again. They have zero value. What we'd say is they're not invalid studies in and
of themselves, what they do is they leave open alternative explanations for findings other than
causation. Could be some bias of measurement error. It could be some bias of sampling. It could be some
reporting bias. It could be some confounding, et cetera, et cetera. And we say, as long as you
acknowledge those, then the study shows what it shows. It's weak inference, but it's not
nothing. And I think the same thing is with crossover. And we may have to accept that with
lots of stuff. That's the idea of nutrition. We may have to accept there are these limitations
or our knowledge. So, for example, if you do a study of cheddar cheese made in Wisconsin
versus some appropriate control, and you say, look, this is what the effects I get of this,
and so therefore cheese consumption has this effect. I said, well, Peter, are you sure it's
cheese consumption in general, or is it just cheddar cheese? Is it just cheddar cheese made in
Wisconsin, or is it any cheddar cheese? Is it only cheddar cheese when eaten with these other
things or these. And I think that's something with so hard to control a nutrition that will wind
up with these statements of saying, it looks like it's cheddar cheese in general, and maybe we can
do some other studies to suss it out, or it looks like it's cheese in general. But we'll always
have this sort of saying, this looks like what it is. Here's a good recommendation, but it's not
absolute knowledge. Well, with that, let's go back to where we started, which is the RDA for
protein consumption. Now, many folks, Don Lehman and others, have argued that the RDA is insufficient
if you're actually trying to optimize health. And if you're actually in pursuit of another
agenda, which might be avoiding sarcopenia later in life, achieving your peak in physical
performance. That's not the peak of physical performance, right? But if your objective is
beyond survival, you might want to have more. And the numbers for what more tends to be,
seem to converge in the ballpark of 1.2 to 1.6 grams per kilogram if you're trying to go for a minimum
effective dose, but easily up to two. So what is your best aggregation of the data on where you
start to reach diminishing returns? I hesitate to talk this way because it's the way I think,
but I know it's not the way others do. I always think about the concavity of a curve. So the more
concave down it is, the more negative the second derivative, the quicker you get to that point
of diminishing return. It just means the curve is shaped that way. And so most things in biology work
that way. They're not positive second derivatives where the more you do, the better it gets
and the rate at which it gets better goes up. That's very rare. So what is your gestalt on
the optimal zone based on all of the above? Yeah, I think it's important.
important to distinguish between concave downward or a curve that doesn't keep accelerate,
a decelerating curve, a curve with a negative second derivative versus non-monotonic.
Those can be non-monetotic, but it's not the same thing.
So one is the diminishing returns.
It keeps going up, but it goes up ever more slowly, and there may be a point at which it never
reaches, and that point that it never reaches may not be 100% of whatever it is you're
thinking about.
That's different than saying it else actually goes down at some point.
So a lot of things go down at some point, right?
Too little will kill you, too much will kill you.
And that's actually more common in biology, right?
So too much thyroid hormone, too little thyroid hormone, a very bad thing.
Too few calories, too many calories.
That's right.
In the case of protein, if we're thinking about it not in an absolute sense,
but as a percent of calories, let's just say, and let's assuming your calories are
at an appropriate level for whatever it is you want or your life,
What I think we can say with pretty good confidence is there's some level that's too low.
I think we can also say with pretty good confidence that there's some level above the RDA
that with very, very rare exceptions perhaps, is at most not worse than, and in the vast majority
of cases likely better than the RDA level.
You might think of it as the real base level like economy rental.
And then there's the higher end good rental.
And then there's something further out there where I would say there's less certainty,
and that's where probably a little more the debate is.
So there's not no debate between the base level and what I'm saying is closer to the,
perhaps, I don't want to use the word optimal because that implies a dip,
but superior, known superior level, little debate in between, but not so much in my view.
And then there's more debate about beyond the known,
superior level and a lot of uncertainty there. I think the evidence is very clear that when you
go from the RDA level of 0.4-ish grams per pound or 0.8-ish grams per kilogram, up into roughly double
or even a little bit more of that, roughly two, I know no evidence of harm in any group other
than perhaps, again, the very rarest folks. And even that would typically not be protein in general.
It would be specific type.
So if you said to me, phenylkaternuric can't have phenylalanine, okay, fine.
Someone who's got allergy to weight protein can't have weight protein, but that does mean protein in general.
I know of no evidence for harm even in people with chronic kidney disease or anything else.
I think there's lots and lots of evidence for benefit in at least medium-term observable phenomena like body weight, like appetite,
control, like bone, strength, muscle, and so on for people to be consuming more. And I think
it's especially true in people who are recovering from injury, bodybuilding, looking for performance
in athletics, looking for strength, who are older, who are growing, all of those things.
I mean, sometimes, David, I think it's easier to flip it and just say, why don't we just
identify all the people who don't benefit from a higher amount of protein than the RDA? Because
I worry that when we start to carve out a bunch of categories and say if you're in one of
these categories, you should be consuming more. You're always going to kind of miss something or
someone might not identify. So for example, you've used the term bodybuilder. Most people would
never identify themselves as a bodybuilder because when they think bodybuilder, they think of
the caricature type bodybuilder we see on a magazine cover when you're at the airport that doesn't
actually look anything like what you want to look like. But the truth of the matter is,
It's hard for me to imagine, I don't know that I can think of one of my patients.
Let me just start with that as a sample size.
One of my patients who doesn't need to, at a minimum, work hard to maintaining their muscle mass.
And many of my patients are working hard to add muscle mass.
And so what differentiates them from a bodybuilder?
The difference is the bodybuilder, of course, has the benefit of using superphysiologic doses of androgens,
consuming, basically training all day and doing nothing but optimizing around that. But the reality
of it is we're all sort of bodybuilders. Everybody's a bodybuilder if they're really thinking about it
in the lens of what are we on this earth for? We're on this earth to create the most robust body
we can have. And it doesn't have to look like it's bulging with muscles. First of all, most of us
couldn't achieve that if we wanted to. Not with saying the fact that most of us could never
achieve it anyway. Let's think through who should be consuming the RDA. I would say with rare
exceptions, the answer's probably no one. Okay. I mean, that's a very important statement. There are rare
exceptions. It goes back to goals. You might say you might find people, in fact, not might, I think
you almost certainly would, who say, I don't care about any of those things you just mentioned.
The only thing I care about is saving the planet, and my thing is I should eat as little as possible
and as little protein as possible and as little animal product as possible for that.
Somebody else could say, my goal is to be nearer to God, and this gets me nearer to my God
in my way.
Someone else in my goal is an aesthetic.
I want to look like a heroin addict in a doorway in Manhattan in 1970.
That's your aesthetic, but those people are rare.
I think it's really important for people to understand that this argument around the RDA is adequate and that's what you need to eat.
And anybody who is suggesting you eat more than that is wrong, we have to actually flip the question and say, okay, who is best served by eating at the RDA versus, say, 2X the RDA at 1.6 grams, which for me would put me at 150 to 160 grams of protein per day instead of 60.
I think here's where we get into that issue of recognizing the limits of our knowledge
and then being able to wrangle with them rationally as opposed to irrationally state the
limits of our knowledge.
So I put up a LinkedIn post, and I put up many about protein in the last 12 or more months.
Yeah, we'll link to them all in the show notes here so that people can kind of, I don't
want to make this a discussion where we're plowing through papers because that's already been
done and we'll link to all those things.
But yes, I'll make sure that everyone goes back to them.
So feel free to reference specific ones, so they'll have access to them immediately.
And one of them, I included a quotation, was from one of my old mentors in graduate school,
Harold Uker, who famously said, I'm a data nut.
Students came a T-shirt that said that, and he would say, show me the data.
It was the time I think of that Jerry McGuire movie being popular.
Show me the money.
He was like, show me the data.
So what I sort of said is people keep raising these questions of harms.
I said, show me the data.
Can anybody send me?
And it was sincere.
I said, this is an open call, and I sent it to like some of the top people in the world,
including those who are a little bit hesitant on protein intake or denigrated.
And I said, can anybody send me one or more papers that are intervention studies,
not observational ones, that are in humans, ideally randomized,
but I'll take an intervention even if it's not randomized,
but it's got to be controlled.
Controlled intervention study in humans feeding different levels of protein,
in which the different levels of protein intake are separable from other effects that show
deleterious effects on a clinically or intrinsically meaningful endpoint.
Don't show me that this molecule changed or this gut microbiota changed.
What do I do with that?
Why do I care that that gut microbiota changed?
Nobody cares about those things intrinsically.
We care about them only insofar as if they give us heart attacks or strokes or earlier
or later death or greater strength or make us better looking.
We care about how long we live, how good we look, how we feel our strength, what we can do.
We don't intrinsically care about whether this molecule in our body is higher than that
molecule or this gut microbe.
And I said, can anybody send me one?
Nobody.
I was on part of that thread, David.
And wasn't there one paper that did surface that was a TPN trial, total parental nutrition?
in patients in the ICU?
I think there were some, I think, that Dudley Lamming sent,
and Dudley's a great scientist and a good friend who studies protein,
mainly in mice and other things, but also a little bit humans.
I think he cited a couple of references,
and I think they were from Luigi Fontana, if I recall correctly,
and there was some short-term trials in patients with cancer.
And I don't remember all the details.
Yes, I'm working my hardest to give an honest look,
because I think what you did is the right thing to do, which is, look, guys, we're having
religious debates on social media where people are using their Twitter platforms to lambast
people they disagree with and call them names and do all this sort of nasty stuff.
Why don't we just do this like grownups and show me the data?
And so the data is show me human clinical trial intervention studies that demonstrate the
deleterious effects of quote unquote high protein.
And yeah, the only thing that I saw was you took these patients who were very, very sick in the ICU so sick that they can't consume enteral nutrition, which means they can't eat because they're probably ventilated, and their guts aren't even working, so you can't actually put feeding tubes in them.
So you use a central line, you put an intravenous catheter into one of the major central veins in their body, and you give them all of their nutrition through that conduit,
which is called parental nutrition.
And with total parental nutrition,
you are chemically crafting the exact composition of what they consume,
exactly how much glucose,
exactly how much fat,
what type of fat,
how much protein,
what type of protein,
what micronutrients,
et cetera,
et cetera.
And again,
I don't remember the exact study,
but it showed that there was no benefit
to a higher protein diet.
And this was counterintuitive.
Now that I'm remembering it,
I believe the study sought to ask the question, wouldn't patients in the intensive care unit benefit from a higher protein diet?
Because they're very catabolic.
The body is incredibly catabolic in that setting.
And so I think the surprising outcome of that study was that the patients on the higher dose of protein did know better.
If my memory serves me correctly, they didn't do worse.
Am I remembering that correctly?
If my memory also serves me correctly, there was no statistically significant effect.
on what I would call an intrinsically, clinically important outcome.
There wasn't a statistically significant effect on lifespan.
Right.
And in that patient population, death mortality is the most important outcome.
There are other things.
Days in the ICU, days on the ventilator, those things all matter tremendously.
But you're looking at such a sick population that's on the precipice of death that when you look at ACM
or all-cause mortality, you're going to get some interesting and valuable insights.
And we're also going to have to really dig into this idea of when we've got different
sources of data and none of which are the data we really want.
What we really want is the randomized controlled trial in tens of thousands of people
so we can look at subgroups and we'd have a lot of power.
We want perfect adherence.
We want it free living.
We want it in people eating foods under the circumstances about which we're going to make
claims. Most of us are not asking, well, if I am on TPN and unconscious and being tube fed by a
surgeon, then what? Most of us are saying, when I go to the grocery store and decide what I want
to bring home for dinner tonight, then what? And those are not the same thing. So we won't have
that. We'll have lousy epidemiologic studies with lousy self-reported data following people,
large groups for long periods of time and causal inference will be fraught. We'll have mouse studies,
in which we're not sure we can generalize from the mouse to the human.
We'll have short-term studies of people being tube-fed.
We want to talk about long-term studies of people eating, quote-unquote, ordinary foods
in ordinary ways.
And we're going to have to sort of think about if they all line up perfectly.
Smoking is an example of that where almost everything lines up.
You may not have the perfect study you want, but the cell stuff, the mouse stuff,
the animal stuff, the epidemiology, the clinical trials, we get people to stop smoking,
They all line up to say, smoke is really bad. Don't smoke. If they all line up, great, then
it's easy. If they don't all line up, we also have to start to talk about how strong is each
piece of evidence, both in terms of its generalizability to what we really want, as well as in and of itself,
is it strong? And I think those studies that we've just described are not especially dispositive
to me. Yeah, I think another type of inquiry that can be misinterpreted, but there's a great
analogy for it is around the dose response curve for muscle protein synthesis as protein dose increases.
There was a study that was referenced that looked at, as you went from 0.8 to 1 to 1.2 to 1.4
to 1.6 grams of protein per kilogram of body weight, what did the rate of muscle protein synthesis
do? In other words, where did you start to achieve the plateau and beyond which you were not going
to get more. And what I think this study demonstrated was that in less trained individuals,
you will achieve higher levels of MPS for lower amounts of amino acids. But again, two things
stand out here. The first is be careful what patient population you're looking at in the study
and make sure it applies to you. So in an individual who's training an hour a day, I don't
think they can compare themselves to someone who went from sitting on a couch to training 90 minutes
a week, very different.
The other place, I would say that there's a perfect parallel there, and I want to make sure
everybody who's trying to figure out what bucket they belong in can sort of do the mental
gymnastics here, if you take an untrained individual, a person who is 100% sedentary, which
sadly is the majority of people in the United States, and you put them on a fitness regimen
of three 30-minute whole-body workouts a week, take that sedentary person, I take them into a gym,
and I get them to push around weights 30 minutes three times a week, not to failure, not to profound
exhaustion, there is no desire to maim them or make it such that they can't get out of bed
the next day. Will they achieve a training benefit? Will they achieve some benefit? And the
The answer is unbelievably, unbelievable benefit.
David, if you put me into their workouts, would I achieve any benefit?
I would argue virtually none.
Why?
Why the difference?
Well, because you're already sort of on the asymptote, right?
It's like anything else.
Take someone who's never had any value of it and you get a big benefit.
Give a mouse that has no leptin, just a tiny little bit of leptin, and it immediately starts
to slim down quite a lot.
Give a mouse that has a normal amount of leptin a little bit more, you're going to observe almost
nothing. Give a kid who's been studying algebra for an hour a day vigorously and diligently
an extra 10 minutes of studying algebra, you're probably not going to get that much benefit.
Give a kid who's never been exposed to algebra at all, 10 minutes a day of tutoring algebra,
probably start to get some real benefits soon.
By the way, that's a beautiful example.
When I hear people say you don't need much more protein than 0.8 because of that stuff,
You know what it makes me think? That's like telling a kid they only need to study algebra
10 minutes a day if they want to master it. Absolutely. One of things that irks me about the
field of nutrition, and I don't know the solution to this, goes back a little bit to the
challenges we have, but this is really an economic challenge, not an intrinsic challenge.
Look at the sample sizes of studies in nutrition in general, randomized controlled trials
of nutrition in general, especially of things like protein intake. And then look at the sample
sizes of studies of statins, randomized control stress, statins, GLP-1 agonists, vaccines,
etc. They're different by multiple orders of magnitude. It's not uncommon to read these
studies saying, well, we're interested in the effects of protein consumption on African-American women
over age 50 with diabetes and without, so we had six in each group. You're like six in
each group. You had 60,000 over there in that pharma study. And so we have very much. We have
really weak data on this. And so it's not surprising that often we don't show these big effects.
This is important, David. I'm sorry to interrupt. I want to tie this back to the discussion we had
around disclosures. There's sort of a reason that virtually everybody in nutrition science
is taking some money from food industry. Now, there are someone who works at the NIH, who is funded
entirely at the NIH? Kevin obviously doesn't need. Is Kevin back at the NIH, by the way?
my knowledge. But most everybody who's at a university is cobbling together money from both
the government and industry. And that's demonstrated by the belt and suspenders bootstrapping
approach that comes into nutrition science studies, which are not well funded. So maybe just explain
why is it that it's easy to fund a pharma study with 60,000 people in it? And it's hard to get
funding to study 600 people in a nutrition study?
The first question is very simple. It's the economic model of it. Pharmaceuticals are patentable,
and the way our country works is, in general, you cannot market a pharmaceutical without the
FDA's approval, and the FDA will generally not give approval unless you've met their bar for
having demonstrated as the act of Congress for the FDA's structure mandates. They must have a reasonable
basis for concluding that the benefits outweigh the harms under proposed conditions of use.
And so the FDA says, this is what it's going to take to convince us. And it's going to be
these big randomized controlled trials, as well as a few other things. And so the companies say,
we've got to do it. Then there's the economic model that makes it feasible for them to do it
in most cases. Sometimes the reason we don't have certain drugs is not because they can't be
made, is because the farmer companies say, it's not worth it for us because we won't
make enough money to offset the development platform. That's a problem there. But in any case,
in other situations like a GLP-1 agonist, it does. So they develop them. They spend hundreds of
millions of dollars. They do them to the utmost rigor. They're probably the most rigorous
human health studies done on the planet these days. To your point, if you're willing to spend,
let's say it's $2 billion today, $3 billion, whatever the number is, 10 years and a couple billion
is an enormous investment. But if you can recoup it, it's worth it. How do you recoup that in
nutrition science? You don't. Their margins in the food industry are much lower. They often can't
patent stuff quite so easily. It's hard to patent the grapefruit, right? So if you're the grapefruit
sellers, you know, and you want to do some study, you're not going to patent grapefruit
even though you may have some benefit from it. So that creates a problem, which sometimes
leads to why people want supplements and things so maybe they can get some patent protection.
but even that can be limited at times.
You don't have the economic model for it, and so they just don't.
I don't have the numbers at my fingertips.
I don't know if anybody does.
But if you said to me, we're going to take all the money spent on research looking at the
effects of food, not how do you make food, not how do you make a better chocolate bar
or a better macaroni and cheese or something, but what are the effects of eating that
chocolate bar or macaroni and cheese?
and you added it up across every single commodity group, the dairy council, the egg board,
every single food company, every dietary supplement company, and you added all of what they
spend on research.
I would be very surprised if it exceeds a billion dollars across the entire country.
So there's very little money, relatively speaking, there.
They don't have the economic wherewithal to do it.
They also don't have the mandate to do it.
My group, 20 plus years ago, we did the first randomized controlled trial ever commissioned
of their products by the Fridolet Company.
And my gosh, were they scared about this?
Didn't know what they're getting into.
It turned out.
We compared chips fried in corn oil to low-fat chips and cookies and crackers and things
to traditional chips and cookies and crackers that had more saturated fat and trans fat.
And the idea was, is low-fat better than corn oil?
and the answer was no.
Assuming you can control your calories, you're better off eating the full fat,
corn oil chips.
What about the saturated, the high saturated fat and the trans fat?
They were the worst.
Yeah.
What were the outcomes?
CVT type outcomes.
So biomarkers, obviously.
With the exception I think of triglycerides, my recollection is the traditional trans fat fat fat
fat fat fat fat were worse.
I think the low fat high carb was worse for triglycerides and the high fat corn oil type stuff
was better for everything.
That's published in A.J.C. and Marie P. Saint-Ange was the first author of that. And it was interesting. The criticisms we got again, it was Marian Nessel was one of the few critics. And it was typical. Never touched the science. They never said, well, the design was wrong or the measurements were wrong. It was like, well, they were funded by industry. And I call this, this was her words. I call this a calorie distractor. If you have something to say about the science, why don't you stick to the science and do that instead of quips and ad hominative attacks. But that's all we got.
Anyhow, so the food industry traditionally has not done a lot of this.
These studies are expensive.
That study we did, I don't remember the exact number, but it's probably in the neighborhood,
especially if we inflated it to today's costs, might be getting close to a million dollars
or something.
But that's nothing compared to what we talked about with pharma.
The other issue is the other big funder is NIH.
And NIH, I think both has seen often these things as something the industry should fund.
Well, I mean, if it's there selling it, let them fund it. That's one. Sometimes it's not
seen as really deep, big science. Yes, it's practically interesting, Dr. Allison, but where's
the big, deep scientific hypothesis? And then the last thing is, in my opinion, here's clearly
an opinion, the misprioritization of funding. And this is something that now we see the NIH
addressing very vigorously. Again, I would agree with some of what they're doing. I disagree with
some of what they're doing, but Jay Badacharya, he's a real smart guy and he, with others, are trying
to say, let's repurpose some of the funding. Less here, more here. And I look at the observational
epidemiology, which can be very expensive in nutrition. And I think the new information yield,
and I underline that phrase, new information, is often very low. If somebody else comes out with a
new study tomorrow and says, we did another one and it was a million subjects and we measured
the food intake as carefully as we can with self-report in this population. We had a couple
of biomarkers, and there's something interesting about this. And here's what we showed with
protein intake and longevity. I don't care which directions. You showed greater longevity,
showed lesser longevity. I'm like, I already knew it was a question. You haven't answered the
question for me. You gave me a little thing to scratch my head about maybe, but it didn't really
move the needle. Let's talk a bit about the epidemiology in this space. So I think everybody listening
to this podcast knows what epidemiology is, and we've talked a lot about the limitations of it
and what a healthy user bias is, but give us the landscape of how epidemiology has looked
specifically at this question of the relationship between protein intake and outcomes of health.
What are some of the near, unique, or particular circumstances of epidemiology that lend
itself to confusion here?
I think the most important thing that I want to say is a sort of maybe a little bit of a weird kind of left turn on this, but I think the greatest limit or problem with the nutrition epidemiology in a context like we're discussing now or in this exact context of protein consumption and things like longevity and long-term major health is the opportunity cost. It's that we're spending the money and often a non-trivial amount of money. These big epidemiology studies can be very expensive and we're not spending it therefore in the big
randomized, well-done, controlled clinical trials.
That's, I think, the biggest problem.
If you said to me, well, we did some epidemiologist, something we can glean, and it's interesting
and fun, and there was no cost, okay.
But if you say, we did that, but we could have done one really good or maybe 10 medium
size, randomized controlled trials for that, that was a big loss.
Now, in and of themselves, if we just stick to the epidemiology, you and many others
frequently and correctly point out the issue of confounding.
We all say correlation is not necessarily causation.
The example of ice cream consumption and murder rates is trotted out.
We say, you know, more ice cream consumption associated with more murder rates.
Guess what?
It's heat.
People eat more ice cream.
It's hot.
They murder more.
All true.
All fine.
But that's actually just a tiny piece of it.
There's so much more.
There's the measurement problem.
And that measurement is not random.
And even if it was random, it's usually not taken into account.
If it was random and we knew it was random and we took it into accounts,
Statistically, we could make the problem kind of go away.
But it's often not random.
It may be correlated.
People who eat more of this may systematically bias they're reporting down,
then people eat less of that.
There's selection bias.
People may choose to be in the study or not choose to be in the study,
and that may affect things.
There's what's called collider bias.
I control for something, and I think I'm doing a good thing by that,
but in fact I've created an inadvertent association.
I could go on and on with statistical obscura.
What do you think are the three most important biases that impact this particular question
when asked through an epidemiologic lens?
I think it's the confounding, particularly, but not only by culture and socioeconomic status
and social class education.
Second would be the measurement error, in particular, again, the non-systematic, the non-random measurement error.
And then I would probably say some selection biases that are a little hard to specify, Miguel Hernan
at Harvard is perhaps one of the most thoughtful people on talking about the other ways these biases
can creep in in terms of when people start the study, who gets in the study, and when we
consider their exposure as occurring. He does some ways to try to correct that. He thinks that's
more important than confounding. So I think those are probably the three biggest ones. Those are
the intrinsic issues. I think the other one, which is very big, but not intrinsic, is I'm going
to say honesty, and maybe that's a little too strong, but I think it's the honesty, or maybe the
sincerity, perhaps is a better way of saying it, the sincerity of reporting by the investigators.
I don't think many investigators are lying in an explicit sense, but I do think there are both
intentional and unintentional efforts at distorting. That is, people want to tell a story.
And they emphasize some things and deemphasize others.
They hide some things and don't hide others.
So it's not an explicit lie, but there's a manipulation of the information.
And why do you think the editors at journals are unable to address that in the review process?
For the majority of editors, it's lack of ability, lack of resources, and lack of courage.
for a minority of editors, it's limitations on the ability to really go in and suss it all out.
So the New England journals of the world, the sciences of the world, the jammers of the world,
they have the resources within reason and the sophistication and to sort of go after this,
but they can't get everything.
Often when I think about the idea of peer reviewing, but also to some extent the editorial review,
which has a little more teeth than the peer reviewers themselves.
I look at it like restaurant reviews.
If Zaget or whoever goes in to review a restaurant
or something give them Michelin Stars,
they can tell you, did they like the offerings on the menu?
Was the food tasty?
Did it look good?
Was the service good?
They're not going back and doing a microbial count in the kitchen.
They're not checking how often the chef washed his or her hands.
Those are things you need a health inspector,
who's got some authority, who can do spot checking, surprise visits,
who's got equipment and so on. That's what you need there. And I think peer reviewers are like
restaurant critics. Now, does it look good? Is it interesting? As a peer reviewer, I can't go back
and look at everybody's raw data. In some cases, we'll see something that looks funny. And then through
the journal, we're doing a couple of these now. We will get the raw data from people. And then we
often see things that are quite funny. And we often get a lot of fights with review authors who don't
want to let us look at their raw data and kind of tells you something.
Where is AI in this process? I mean, why are we not, or are we using LLMs to serve as peer editors?
Short answer is we are. Long answer is we're at the stage of infancy and amateurishness with it.
So it's coming. It'll get better and better. But we can do some very simple things now.
People look for these so-called tortured phrases. When you find these phrases that kind of look like a word salad, that's sort of a hint often that you've got something plagiarized or just fabricated.
we can look in some circumstances for things that literally don't add up.
So some people made something called the Grim test.
I forget what it's an acronym, but it's basically when you know, let's say you have a
Lycrot scale and you know you have a certain number of subjects, then the mean of that scale
can only have certain values.
And if you say, hey, it doesn't have one of those values, something must be wrong.
Those are examples.
And do we know if these AI agents, these peer review agents, I'll call them, are
being trained on known fraudulent manuscripts because we certainly have an abundance of things
that were demonstrated to be frauds. So it would be, I assume, a reasonable thing to do to start
training these AI agents on that to start identifying the patterns? Again, the answer is yes,
but very much in its infancy. Okay. Who's leading the charge on this? I don't know if there's one
person who's leading it. I think James Heathers, who's now got a position part of the challenge
with a lot of these so-called data sleuths is it's hard to get paid to do that.
So I get paid to be, I was a paid dean, and now I'm a paid center director, and kind of
in my spare time, I do a little sleuthing. People like James Heather's where it's more
his full-time gig. It's hard to get paid, but Retraction Watch set something up for him.
See, he's one, Tracy Weisberger over in Europe, is another. There's a woman whose name I'm
unfortunately going to mispronounce, but who's a Dutch scientist who came up with something
called stat check and stat check. If the statistics are poured in the format of the American
psychological association with a very clear format, has software that will specifically go and
make sure those all check.
So those are some examples, but there are many others that people are working on.
What do you think is the most compelling piece of epidemiologic data against the idea
of exceeding the RDA?
I don't think there are any compelling observational epidemiologic data.
When I think of the things where you've got relatively hard endpoints, relatively large sample sizes,
Usually they've not looked at hard, big thresholds, and even if they have, they've looked
more continuously at protein intake.
Even if they have, I can show you studies in either direction, studies that make it look
like more protein is beneficial and studies that look the other way.
And I don't think any of them are dispositive.
I think protein intake is highly confounded with social class as well as type of protein intake.
Let's flip the question now again, which is, okay, I'm going to argue that lower protein
is better because I've just demonstrated you are not going to starve to death at, let's just
round up and call it one gram per kilogram of body weight. Peter, you should be eating 85 grams
because I know that that is safe. But I don't know that if you double that, that you're not going
to get cancer. So what can we say about quote unquote high protein diets and cancer or heart
disease. So what I think we can say is absolute knowledge is beyond us in this context,
but we can have reasonable degrees of certainty, certainty for practical purposes, as long as we
need to be open with them. You know, one of the things that I often get frustrated with the
nutrition science and some other elements of scientific community is we say science is always
evolving and the public needs to understand that it's not something wrong when we, in their
view flip-flop and we change our mind. We change our mind when new data become available.
Well, that's true. But then the implication is we were honest with the public all along
and saying, this is what we think today. It's not absolutely certain. And the one thing that
always just stands out in my mind is early 1990s, Michael Jacobson, Dr. Michael Jacobson,
PhD, nutrition scientist, director of something called Center for Science in the Public Interest,
There's the word science in it, coming out on national TV on a camera, holding a plate of
Fetuccini Alfredo outside an Italian restaurant after a new reporter come out on composition of
what people ate in Italian restaurants, got huge press.
And as he holds it out to the camera, he says, this is a heart attack on a plate.
This is a heart attack on a plate is not a tempered statement that says, this is what I think
we know today, but it could change later.
So I think we need to be more honest about that.
What was he referring to that was causing the heart attack in that?
I think the implication was the saturated fat, or maybe the saturated fat and sodium.
So in any case, I think we need to do better with that.
I know of no compelling evidence for harm.
That doesn't mean there couldn't be any, but I know of no compelling evidence for harm.
I know of no studies showing that humans get more cancer with this.
I think we need to be skeptical of the mouse studies, the epidemiologic studies, for reasons,
I've indicated. I think at some point we need to recognize, as people did when I put up that
LinkedIn post, and I said, show me the data. Can anybody send me one? Many people responded with
something akin to, I can't show you that study, David, but can you show me the complement or
the opposite study that meets all your criteria and shows there isn't harm? And the answer may be
No, I'm not sure, but what I can say is at some point when you've looked enough and
you've failed to see harm, where is the burden of proof? So if you don't have a really strong
appriory rationale, now we can argue about that. There probably is legitimate debate.
You might legitimately say, I have a strong a prior rationale there isn't harm, and Dudley
Laming might say I have a strong a priori rationale there is harm, and you're entitled to
your opinion. Those are opinions. And then we can both look and say, well, neither one of you
has the absolute definitive data, at what point, where's the burden of proof? And I think
right now the notion is the burden of proof is on those who want to argue for higher protein intake.
Well, the evidence isn't clear enough to show that the RDA is too low, to which I would reply.
Well, the evidence isn't clear that it isn't. Now, where is the needle point? Where does prudence point?
And I don't think the status quo is necessarily what's always prudent.
saying leave everything the way it is is always the best way. Sometimes it is. But in this case,
I think we've left it alone long enough. I think it's time to say, we've looked and looked and looked.
If I look at a thousand swans and I cannot find a black swan, does that mean that no black swans
exist? Of course not. But if I looked at 10,000 black swans, if I sent a helicopter in the air
and we scoured and we looked with binoculars, if I sent teams of undergraduate students
to walk around ponds and measure, look for every black swan, if I put drones with cameras out,
and I have failed time after time after time to find a black swan at one point say,
maybe for practical purposes, I can say black swan's probably not something we need to worry about.
Yeah, I think that is probably the most logical way to frame this,
which is, I do not believe we're ever going to get a dose toxicity study for protein,
the way we do for figuring out what the LD50 of a drug is, where you push the toxicity
and you figure out, okay, at this dose, we will kill 50% of people. So the real question
becomes, what would be the bracket you would put around for 90% of the population to exist
within this range is an appropriate way to interact at the grocery store? To your point,
what's the problem we're trying to solve here? The people listening to us don't care about
most of what we've said today. I mean, we had to say it, but they actually just want to know,
look, man, I'm really confused because I'm reading people who have a lot of piss and vinegar
in what they're saying on this topic. How much protein should I be eating? What should my family be
eating? Should I be avoiding protein in my kids? Should they not be eating snacks with
whatever the argument is. So our guidance to our patients is pretty straightforward. And it
varies based on their preference. We have some patients who are vegetarians, who have been lifelong
vegetarians, who can't stand the feel of meat. This is not like a belief that they have that
meat is bad for them, but they genuinely don't like meat. We have other patients who won't
eat any animal products. So they're going to have a harder time reaching the upper limits of
protein consumption. And so with those people, we're just trying to nudge them as high as we can get
them, and that's probably not going to get much higher than about 1.2 grams per kilogram of body weight.
But the guidance we're giving people is we really like to see you at about 1.6 to 2. It's an easy
heuristic. It's easy to remember too because you're basically consuming almost a gram per pound
of body weight. So how would you advise people based on the, tell me the for all intents and purposes,
you don't need to worry about Black Swan's argument. So let's separate the
idea of what I think people ought to do if they have these goals that most of us have,
like live longer, be stronger, be healthy, et cetera. How do I think we should tell them to
achieve that? The latter one, I'll loop back to in a minute, but I'm not really the expert on
that. But the former one, what I would say is, I think you and I are largely aligned on this.
I would say, if you just want to survive, or if you just want to survive, the RDA is probably
okay for most people. But if you want to thrive in these goals that most of us share, then I would
aim for in the neighborhood of two grams per kilogram per day per person spaced out throughout the
day. So, for example, if you want to pass, you should study this many hours per day and
you will get a C. Right. But if you want to have the best shot at getting into the best college
you could get into because you want to study engineering, you should probably study this much
and you're probably going to need to try to get A's.
Great analogy. My uncle, my great uncle, who was a professor from the old country and
the old school and a philosophy professor, when his kids would come home in America from
school, he would say, good. Now playtime's over. Now you sit down and we study Latin and
math and logic and you do the real work. And then there's probably yet another example.
You may know that in South Korea, they actually had to pass some laws recently or chose
to pass some laws recently that I forget the details of them, but that students could only
study so much because they're really starting to get worried of students just going too
far. And I think those are good examples. To me, it's sort of like the RDA for protein intake
is like what my uncle saw the American schools as. It's a, yes, it's just enough to pass
high school and do something. The next level,
level up is what my uncle did, which was, we're going to really do some work and get you closer to an optimum level. Maybe what some students do is maybe go too far. But I also think it's interesting to see in what way were they going too far? Did anybody ever say or show that if you study algebra or anything else, 12 hours a day instead of two hours a day, it directly causes harm. And I don't know of anything like that. Now, if you said to me,
If you study algebra or whatever for 12 hours a day, ipso facto, you are not exercising,
you are not socializing, you may be not sleeping enough, et cetera, then you have a problem.
It's not the direct effect of the studying.
It's the substitution effect.
Right.
So if I literally drank, I handed you a drink when we came in, one of my favorite playtime things,
I love to play with these different protein products and have fun with them.
and this is a drink that has 20 grams of protein and 90 calories, almost pure protein.
And there are other things like that.
If you or I were to say, I just want as much protein as I can, so I'm just going to live on that stuff.
There would be no, to my knowledge, no direct harm.
I'm not worried about my kidney shutting down or something.
But I would say, well, wait a minute, did you have any vitamins and minerals?
Did you have any pleasure from eating other foods?
did you have enough energy to work out hard by not having any carbohydrate?
Maybe a little carbohydrate would make you work able to work a little harder.
So I think there are other losses, but beyond those other losses,
or you get a fatty acid deficiency because you have any linoleic acid,
beyond the losses, I don't know of any risk.
So to me, I look at it and say life support, basic maintenance, RDA,
strong evidence for thriving, twoish.
above two, probably more benefit, but you're starting to hit the asymptote.
I don't know that it's going to come down.
I don't know of anything that says that it's not going to be monotonic, that's going to turn around,
but diminishing benefit and then starting to get into more costs of economic costs,
of buying fancy products, of the costs of your time and attention on it,
the costs of not eating something else you might like.
I know you and I have struggled with is the right word, but wrangled with
fruit. We both like fruit, and we've both played with diets at times where we've minimized
the consumption of fruit for other goals, and yet both perhaps come back to it a little bit
and said, I don't want to give up fruit. And those are examples I think where there's some
optimization, but I see no harm. And I think the more refined your goals are, then the more
it's reasonable to push it a little bit. If I wanted to win the Olympics, I'm more motivated
to push it. For me, if I can lift one more pound of weight on the bench press, who cares?
But if I'm trying to win the Olympics, then it matters.
So let's pivot from here into the extension, the logical extension, I think, of where we're going,
which is not just as it pertains to protein, but to a broader discussion around processed foods.
So this is also a very, very hot topic today.
So I want to just talk about it broadly and then we can talk about protein as a subset of this
because obviously a lot of processed foods are optimized around protein and not just pleasure.
But I will say I have read quite a bit on this topic.
and I've read some pretty compelling arguments on all sides that if you just took processed and
ultra processed foods off the market, people would be better. You would force a change in the system
that would lead to healthier outcomes. And of course, I've read other very compelling arguments
that say, look, if you actually correct for caloric intake, there's nothing per se that is wrong
with a processed food, at least to the first order, second order, potentially. Let's maybe
start with just some of the definitional stuff? What separates a processed food from an ultra-processed food?
There's not a single accepted definition. The most commonly used classification systems
called Nova. It's very controversial for many reasons, some social, some scientific, some
linguistic or definitional. It has to do with degrees of steps and the types of steps and so on
involved. These are very popular topics because they give us a new demon. We talked earlier about
the importance of demons. Kelly Brownell, who, as much as anybody, has been one of the leading
thinkers in the field of obesity research in the last half of the 20th century.
Is he at Yale still? No, no. He moved to Duke years ago, and I think he may be semi-retired now.
But anyway, really good thinker. And I can remember being in a meeting with Kelly. He and I were
both speakers. The meeting was convened by leaders in the food industry. They were inviting
him, and this is just as he was ramping up the rhetoric of toxic environment, toxic food
environment, the epidemiologic environment, the public health environment argument. This was when
it had just begun emerging, the N-Haines 3 and then the later in Haines data, was suddenly
this wake-up call for the country. Yeah, we knew there was obesity and we knew it was getting
worse. We didn't realize how rapidly it had started to get worse in the last couple of decades.
and now there was this hype, this panic.
And Kelly spoke, and he said, we need social movement here.
He said, we can't do it on our own.
I've been the behavioral psychologist type.
He himself struggles with obesity.
It's not education.
The guy's brilliant.
He's enormously educated.
But he still struggled, as did many other people, of a similar degree of education
and expertise.
And he said, we're treating this too much as an individual problem.
He said, you've got companies like McDonald's,
have a goal of nobody should ever be more than X minutes away from McDonald's if they're driving
in the United States. And he said, that's a problem. It's a problem when I'm driving down
the street and I'm being assaulted by all the signage and so on. And he said, we need to change
this. We need a social movement. Now, he's looking at all these executives from the food
industry. And he says, history shows us over and over that social change happens when there's
a villain. He says, we need a villain. And he says, guess who it is? You. So that was the start of
this villainization. What year was that? I don't remember exactly, but I would say it was mid-90s.
And it was Michael Mudd, who was from Kraft at the time, convened that meeting. Good meeting.
And he's like, I invited this guy?
Michael had courage. Anyway, so that was that start of it. And now ultra-process is just a great
way to demon-stuff. It's just another demon. We're off of soybean oil,
phytoestrogens. We dealt with fat for a while. We dealt with sugar for a while. Those are still
floating around. Let's give folks a couple of examples of processed versus ultra-processed. So like
dried fruit is processed. Sure. Virtually everything we eat is processed. Yeah, almost everything
you eat is processed. But how do you then cross the chasm to ultra-processed? Even if you say
there's a single definition like Nova, I can't recite for you the exact criteria, but it's the number
of steps, it's the degrees of steps, it's the types of steps. Most things that come in a
package are now viewed as ultra-processed. That's right. Number of Greens, number of steps.
But you're right. Dried fruit, even fruit that's cut up is processed. Wine is processed.
Cheese is processed. Milk is homogenized. Sometimes it's pasteurized, hopefully. Almost everything
wheat is processed. And for that matter, I don't want to eat a lot of certain unprocessed foods.
There's lots of evidence that unprocessed dairy products cause a lot of harm. So processing is good.
Now let's talk about the state of the evidence because, again, a good story goes a long way.
I mean, our ancestors, and to be clear, my view on this is that the story is much more nuanced than the one I'm about to lay out.
But the one I'm about to lay out makes sense, and I get it.
We didn't evolve eating ultra-processed foods, let alone processed foods, and ultra-processed foods are engineered to be highly palatable.
and part of that engineering process also makes them calorie dense because part of making things
hyper palatable is putting in a lot of sugar and a lot of fat. So it's not that the food companies
who make these things are trying to make us fat. They're not trying to hurt us. Marlboro's not trying
to hurt you with cigarettes. They just want you to smoke them forever. It's an unfortunate consequence
of the product. So they really want to create something that tastes remarkable that you just want to
keep buying over and over again. And the problem is you're going to end up eating more of it
in terms of calories because of the very nature of the product they're trying to sell you.
So clearly this is a problem. We can't have these foods around because we can't eat them
in moderation. We're going to overeat them. This is one thing we can agree on. Overconsumption
of calories always leads to bad things relative to what your needs are. So again, that number
is variable by individual, but for any given individual, eating more than they require.
require leads to physiologic harm. So by that logic, why are we having this discussion?
Why don't we just get rid of ultra-processed foods?
Wow, you've given me so much to work with. One threat I just want to throw out quickly,
we may or may not have time and interest in coming back to it, is the idea that fewer calories
are better, at least to a point. To a point is true. But one of things, particularly with protein,
is we look at some of these studies that people are citing. So I realize I'm sort of looping
back to our prior conversation, but these are from the mouse looking at longevity. And it's
interesting, they're very dependent on ambient temperature. So the studies of caloric restriction
in general and protein restriction in particular, which show benefit, are much more present
at 22 degrees Celsius, which for a mouse is a thermogenic challenge, thermoregulatory challenge,
as opposed to thermoneutral conditions, roughly 27 to 30 degrees Celsius. We don't live our
own lives in the chronic cold. So we'll leave that out there and say it depends on the conditions.
There's several threads you've allowed me to pick up here. One has to do with the idea of categories.
All categories are social constructs. We often hear that said about things like race, and it's true.
Race is a social construct. So is furniture. So is vaccine. So is medicine, et cetera. What you choose
to use as a category and how you choose to define membership in that.
category is a judgment call. And it's not intrinsically written in stone what that should be.
They're useful or not useful for your purposes. That's the first thing to point out. So same thing is
ultra process. There's not a correct definition of ultra process. You can define it any way you want.
The words mean what we say they mean. Once you've defined it clearly, then we can say what's its
value. How can we use it? What's its utility? Second thing I would say is, as with any category,
if it permits a lot of variability in that category, which ultra-process certainly does,
it starts to get a little silly to talk about the overall category because you say,
so wait a minute, you want me to consider a meal replacement shake in the same category as
I consider a big gulp and a 7-Eleven, in the same category I consider a chocolate bar,
in the same category I consider a TPN nutrition, these are all arguably ultra-1%.
process in very different things. And I think we want to talk about the things. The next thing I
want to bring up is what are we trying to do with it? And this is probably the intellectually most
important. If you say to me, David, what I am looking for is a heuristic. And this loops back to
something I said we'd address later, or earlier I mentioned, I said we'd come back to it. The distinction
between how much protein do I think somebody ought to eat for a certain goal versus how would I
tell them to eat it, or what would I tell them about? And those are two different things.
If you said, David, I just want something I can tell people so that when I tell them this,
it has a beneficial effect. Does telling them not to eat ultra-process foods or to eat as few
of them as possible defining ultra-process in this way help? And the answer is, might help a lot.
It might vary a little bit, depending on how you said it. We need more studies. We need to figure
out, my guess is, as with everything else, it won't last long. So you could tell them not to drink
sugar-sweetened beverages. You could tell them not to eat fettuccini, Alfredo, because it's a heart
attack on a plate. All these things have a small effect for a while and usually not so much for
a long term. But it might help. We need to study that. That says nothing, however, about the effects
of the food per se. If you said, no, David, my goal is to tell people about the foods they should
eat if they actually ate them what the effects would be, or my goal is to determine the effects
of foods. Then I would go back to a statement from a wonderful book called A Fly in the Ointment
by someone named Joe Schwartz, and Dr. Schwartz, who's a food scientist, says something like,
there is a motto, repeat after me, the effect of substances in the body depends on their
molecular structure, not their ancestry. So if you give me this molecule, or a collection of
them, to eat, and you extracted that molecule from some berry and it's natural, or you synthesize
that molecule in a laboratory, but in the end, it's the same molecule. We agree, I mean,
you could try to synthesize it might have some slight difference, but let's assume it's
literally the same molecule, and it's the same structure. You give it to the same molecule. You give
to me in liquid form and a liquid form or gastroform or whatever it is. If you say these are
going to have different effects because of where they came from, seems to me we're in homeopathy
now. This makes no sense. My favorite example of this is natural sugar versus processed
sugar, which is, I don't know if the people who say this are ignorant of what fructose and
glucose are, or if it's deliberate marketing shenanigans. I think it's a mixture of deliberate
marketing shenanigans, but it's also the marketing of ideas, not just food products,
by people with particular philosophical and other bents who are anywhere from the interest
to make themselves famous to push a philosophical thing, to push an anti-industry thing,
whatever it is. But yes, I agree with that. And there's so many others. Natural vanilla
versus synthetic vanilla, if it's still vanilla, it's vanillin. There's a wonderful book by
Alan Levinovitz called Natural, in which he will blow away every common conception of what
natural is and what its value is. And he talks about the idea of, you know, lots of people
want the natural vanilla. Most of the vanilla flavoring we get in this country is not so-called
natural from the vanilla plant. But if it was, it would probably have a much worse environmental impact
than the other sources.
So it's not always so simple.
In any case, I think Schwartz is right.
It's not the ancestry.
So whether you give me the molecule and it was locally grown or not locally grown, organic
or not organic, ultra-processed or not ultra-processed.
It's the molecules and their structure that matter.
Conditional upon the molecules and the structure doesn't matter where it came from.
But, in defense of the argument that ultra-processed foods must be worse, if you look at the
ingredient list, David, the sheer number of molecules there would suggest, we're playing
Russian roulette here.
I don't recognize half the names of the things on the bag of Doritos.
I'm making that up.
I haven't looked at a bag of Doritos in a while.
Corn, corn, corn, oil, salt, and spices.
Maybe not a great example.
You could certainly find an ultra-processed food at the grocery store in which you cannot comprehend 50% of what's in it.
And you don't really know the dose either because the only thing that the FDA requires is that you list them in order of abundance.
But it could be that the first one represents 99% of it and the other 12 represent 1% of it.
And even amongst that there's an uneven distribution, et cetera, et cetera.
And it could be that these are just preservatives and color additives and they have no physical bare.
But you just don't know.
Point is, when I eat an apple, or even if I eat a processed apple, in the sense that it's
been pre-cut up, or it's just a dried apple that's dried apple chips, where I can look at the
ingredient list and it says, apples, I got to be safer than if I'm eating, come on, 20 things
of which I can't pronounce 13 of them.
If that's the logic of your thinking, I have a great product I'd like to sell you.
It's gluten-free.
It's seed oil-free.
It's not ultra-processed.
It's free of any harmful thing.
It's chemical-free.
It has no chemicals in it.
I call it vacuum.
And I would like to sell you this vacuum, and basically that means nothing, because we are chemicals.
As my friend, Fred Clydesdale, who's the former head of the nutrition food science department
at UMass Amherst, used to say, the whole purpose of eating is to get chemicals into the body
to replace the chemicals the body loses through the process of living.
All food is chemicals, we are chemicals.
When you eat that apple and you say, I understand it, that's apple, unless you have a lot
of chemical knowledge I don't have, and most people don't have, you don't understand that
any better than you understand something else that says benzodia phosphate or what have
you in it.
You just think you do because you think you understand what an apple is at a chemical level.
You understand what is at a fruit level, maybe, but not at a chemical level.
There are many chemicals in an apple and an orange that you or I couldn't pronounce
and that if someone wrote out what the chemicals are, we would say, what is that scary-sounding
thing?
And we also know that things that we think of as natural can be just as harmful.
Foxglove, hemlock, Socrates killed by being forced to drink all-natural hemlock.
It was all-natural?
Very harmful.
So poisons, drugs often come from natural things.
There's also a misperception that what we think of as natural is somehow has been around for thousands of years.
And in some cases, it's true.
In many cases, it's not.
So the oranges and the apples and the grains that you're eating today were largely not around years ago.
They've been bred.
Even if it's not transgenic, they've been bred by ordinary breeding things.
The cows, the chickens, the pigs have been bred to be different.
They are not indigenous species.
All the chicken, all the cow, all the pig that we eat in this country, the soybeans,
none of that's indigenous.
They're all invasive species.
Turkey, that's indigenous to North America.
Peacons and black walnuts, but not the other walnuts that they love so much in the Mediterranean
diet over there.
They're not American.
I think this is all silliness.
The only thing that's artificial here is our creation of these categories, and we should
just recognize that.
We create the categories.
let's make them meaningful and useful.
So back to ultra-processed foods, if you said to me,
I'm just looking to give my patients or my friends or my mom or myself a hint, a heuristic.
There are lots of heuristics that don't require that classification.
Right.
But there are also lots of heuristics that work,
even though there may be some illogic embedded in them
or some incompleteness or some variability.
If you said to me, for example,
don't talk to people on the street who look like this if I was your kid.
And you said, look, you're walking down the street.
You're in a big city.
Someone comes up, asks you have a match.
Just keep walking.
It's probably a good heuristic.
They probably don't really want to match.
But some might, and you might miss some, but it's not a bad way to stay safe.
Similarly, if I said to you, you know what, don't eat anything from the center aisles at the
grocery store or as little as you can.
Get as much as you can from the periphery, the dairy, the produce, the meat,
meats, the fish, and so on, you might be better off. You will wind up eating less ultra-processed
foods. You will wind up eating less energy-dense foods in many cases, et cetera, et cetera. But that
doesn't mean there's something intrinsic about the periphery of the aisles. It doesn't mean if I took
twinkies and put them in the periphery and I took fat-free Greek yogurt and I put it in the
middle, that suddenly it becomes bad and twinkies become good. It's a heuristic and it might
work for you. To the extent you can stick to it and to the extent it's correlated with these
things that might work. But it says nothing about the causal effect of being in the periphery
or the central aisle. And I think as long as we take ultra-processed foods at that level,
hey, power to you. In the same sense, as if we were talking to that person back with the
protein, you said, how do you explain it to them? Maybe you don't say eat two grams per kilogram
because that's too hard. You have to use the metric system. And Americans hate the metric system
and you have to do math and so on, and you have to count stuff.
But if you just said, make sure you have two servings of lean fish per week
and one serving of lean this and eat some egg whites and so on, maybe that works.
And we all have our own foods.
During World War II, they tried to get Margaret Mead.
They hired Margaret Mead, the federal government, to try to get people to eat more organ
meats because they wanted to send the steak and all to the soldiers overseas.
Didn't work.
Even Margaret Mead couldn't get people to eat a lot more organ meats.
I happen to love organ meats, and some of my go-to-protein sources are chicken gizzards,
fantastic protein-to-calorie ratio, and I love them.
A lot of people look at that and go, no, we need to figure out the way to get people to do this,
but it's the food that matters, not this.
Now, if we go back to those idea of wanting to understand the food, the effects of the food,
then I think working with ultra-processed food is just silly.
I think it's not a meaningful category.
I think any attempts to say, let's find the right definition, I wouldn't even bother.
I don't even think it's worth even discussing.
I would say is let's talk about the substances in the food.
What is the effect of eating dried apples or what is the effect of eating things with this composition?
Or what is the effect of eating bottled wine or dried dates or grilled chicken gizzards?
That's, I think, where the knowledge is.
I want to continue on this point a little bit more from a public health perspective.
I think we could sit here and convince ourselves, I think, quite easily and everybody else
that at the individual level, the heuristic of don't eat ultra-processed foods is simply not
helpful.
It's too coarse a tool to parse out too nuanced of a subject, is the bottom line, right?
I'll go 10% some percent of the way with you on that.
I would say it's a very suboptimal tool.
They're probably better tools.
There certainly could be better tools.
I don't think it would be very effective, and I don't think it would be effective for the long-term.
You're talking at the level of the individual or at society, because I want to distinguish these.
Either one, I think at the heuristic level.
And what I mean by that, again, is nothing to do with causation or really understanding the foods.
If you said, give me a rule.
And it's different for you.
You're very sophisticated.
You have a lot of knowledge.
If you were to say to my dad, when he was alive, who was a smart man, but probably
intentionally kept himself not too smart about nutrition so he could feign ignorance and then
make the choices he wanted. For him, something like that might work a little bit. It would only
work a little until he got sick of it and bored with it, and until good marketers were smart
and said, we'll market you things that skirted the definition of ultra-processed, so we could
say not an ultra-processed food, but that still had the same degree of fat, sugar, calories,
and whatever. David Kessler's said this nicely in some of his work. He makes the distinction
between, I think he says, ultra-processed and ultra-formulated. And he says, ultra-processed, waste
of time. Waste of time as a terminology. As a thing we should focus our attention on for helping
people. He says, ultra-formulated is better because then it talks about what's in the food.
Process talks about the process. I don't care how you got there. I want to care where you got, too.
And so that's one way of making the thinking a little better.
Do you think that there are public health solutions to the metabolic situation we're in as a country?
If you define public health solution as things that exclude things we really consider clinical, usually like pharmaceuticals and surgery,
and you say things where there's palpable and demonstrable success at present, I would say, no.
It's painful to say that, but I would say that after, depending on your point of view, after
roughly 50 years of looking at this, no, I just put up a LinkedIn post about one of the latest
papers that came out of this group, I think it was an Australian group, that just said we
looked at all the parent training type stuff with kids and obesity and a big meta-analysis of
study after study after study of year after year, if there's nothing there.
You could argue that the only area in which public health has changed the course of societal health in this country, smoking cessation seems to be a success. There have been public health measures.
Well, I thought it's about only an obesity. I am. I am. I'm now trying to talk about other areas. So I'm saying smoking cessation efforts at the policy level. So excise taxes, advertising laws, rules for where you can smoke, those have appeared to have a significant reduction in the number of people.
Yeah, great one.
So we've got some wins in public health.
Why do you think public health has been unsuccessful in this arena?
And it can't be for a lack of trying.
It can't be for lack of trying at all.
It may be for lack of trying in a little more intelligent and unbiased manner.
I think there's two really big reasons.
One is intrinsic to the problem.
can never start smoking. For some people, as difficult it is, one can abandon it entirely. One
can't never start eating and one can't, for practical purposes, abandon it entirely. So it's a different
problem. You've got a very strong intrinsic push there. I mean, it's so linked to our survival.
It's a hard goal to manipulate. I think it's intrinsically just very, very difficult. We like our
freedom. We like our variety. I don't want to give up my freedom and variety. I don't want to give up my freedom
and variety. If somebody were to say, if we eliminated all of these choices, or you're only
allowed to shop on Monday, or you're only ought to buy X calories, it's like rationing in a war,
because some people are obese. Even though I myself have struggled with my weight, I would say,
I don't want to live in that world. I don't want to give up my freedom of choice, even if it means
I and some other people are going to struggle with our weight. So I think there's some real
intrinsic difficulties. Everything we do in the public health realm almost seems like either we
can't get it to stick, or we didn't think through enough if it stuck it would work, or it's like
whackamol. That is, you get me to consume fewer calories here or expend more energy there,
and then I expend less here or consume more there. So I think that's the intrinsic problem.
And then I think the other problem is that we started off with the obvious stuff. We looked for the
keys under the lamppost because that's where the light was best. We said,
ah, school-based approaches, farmers markets, walking trails, calories on the menu, so on.
Good ideas. All good ideas. Nudge. We tried them. None of them really seemed to work
meaningfully. At the beginning, that was fine. What I think we have is a failure of courage,
honesty, and creativity to say, we've shown that none of those things have large, demonstrable
meaningful effects. We've not shown that none of them could ever have any effect under any circumstance,
but I think for practical purposes we've shown that those things don't have large demonstrable
meaningful effects. And I think it's time to stop pretending that they might, and proposing and
funding the next study that's only a trivial variant on the many, many such studies that went
before and were shown to not be successful. I think we need to start taking radically different views
and say, the next time someone comes and says, I've got this parent-based or school-based
or community-based idea, tell us how it's radically different than what's gone before,
not trivially different.
Do you have any radical ideas that if you ignore the challenges or issues around implementation,
you would want to implement?
I think you asked me this once before, and I'm going to change my answer slightly,
but not that much.
I think before I said two things. If you really want to decrease suffering now for some subset
of the population for which we could afford it, make bariatric surgery freely available,
and then for investing in research, invest in research on the effects of general quality
of upbringing and general education, especially but not only for women and girls.
I'm going to stick with the second one, say we still need that.
We need to look at what are the effects of general education, not nutrition education.
general education, general parent training, general security, financial and other kinds of security
growing up on obesity levels. That would be my research public health piece.
Sorry, what's the hypothesis there?
The hypothesis that general education, security so that you're not constantly worried about,
can I pay the bills, will I be able to get food at all?
general good parenting, which delivers whatever psychological benefit it delivers.
There's, I would say, at minimum circumstantial evidence that all of those things
lead to reductions in obesity and diabetes decades later.
The two best studies I know of this, or it's called the Moving to Opportunity Study,
funded by the Department of Housing and Urban Development, and the abecadarian study done by
the Ramees, Craig and Sharon Ramey.
But of those three things, isn't education and financial well-being higher today than it was 50 years ago?
The third one, parental support is probably less today.
There are probably more broken families today.
But two of those three are better today than 50 years ago, aren't they?
I'm not sure.
I don't want to say they're better.
But I think there's tremendous disparity.
So the quality of the education and security and so on that some people get is very different than others.
And I think Confucius famously said, we are not concerned about poverty.
We are concerned about differentials of wealth.
It's possible that the public health solution to obesity is becoming a Marxist.
I'm not sure we all want to volunteer.
But I think some of these things are worth looking at.
We do see that there seem to be these long-term benefits.
In studies, as I said, I mentioned what I think are the two best, but they're not the only ones,
where they weren't meant to be primarily nutrition, obesity studies.
But how does that explain what you see in the Middle East, for example, where you have countries
that are remarkably wealthy, there is no poverty. Obviously, there is disparity in wealth
between the very affluent, which is effectively everybody, and then the ultra affluent. Families
aren't broken. Everybody is educated. And yet obesity and diabetes rates are greater than even in
the U.S. Yeah. I think there's at least two ways to look at that. One way is the idea of interaction.
I'm talking about Gulf nations, obviously not.
The same exposures may not have the same effects in one place or another.
For example, we consistently see that in less developed countries, poorer countries,
less industrialized countries, greater wealth is associated with greater obesity.
We consistently see in more industrialized, wealthy countries, greater wealth and education,
especially among adult women, tends to be associated with lesser obesity.
So you have the interaction.
The other is, as you and I said earlier, the strength of the evidence, one of things people frequently like to say is when you know, you say, well, we think temperature has this effect. And they say, what about those people in Iceland? They don't have. Well, alcohol is this. Yes, but those people over there, they drink a lot and they don't have. They're different. They're different in many ways. We don't know that, you know, any one factor has to explain why Qatar has this versus Samoa or something. But I think it's a good hypothesis. So that's what I would look at at the public health level in what we think of as traditional public health.
Now, with the GLP-1 agonist-related drugs and some other drugs being as profoundly beneficial
as they appear to be, I think we're going to get to the point where it's going to be hard,
I think we're already asking the question, and I think it's going to be hard not to take
the question seriously of should it almost be the default?
That is, just like we've asked the question, and people give different answers, but we've
asked the question, should it be that the default is you get this vaccine when you're a kid,
that you get your teeth fluoridated at the dentist, that it's not, hmm, maybe some people should
get that.
I think we are at the point where we're saying, as these drugs continue to be tested and
experienced, if we continue to see the effects we're getting, are we getting to the point
where we should start to say, you know what, that idea of the polypill that came up decades
ago where young adults, even if you don't have diabetes, obesity, hypertension, et cetera,
you'd get a low-dose diuretic, low-dose metformin, et cetera.
low-dose statin. All right, the point where we probably still should do that and then say,
anybody get a low-dose g-lp-1 agonist-related drug, and we'll roll it out and we'll pay for it,
and anybody in the country who wants it can get it or almost anybody. I think that may be the
future. I'm too hungry having this discussion. So, David, thank you for making the trip down.
As always, it's a pleasure. Truly my pleasure, Peter. Let's keep doing it.
Thank you for listening to this week's episode of The Drive.
peteratia md.com forward slash show notes if you want to dig deeper into this episode.
You can also find me on YouTube, Instagram, and Twitter, all with the handle Peter Atia
MD. You can also leave us review on Apple Podcasts or whatever podcast player you use.
This podcast is for general informational purposes only and does not constitute the practice
of medicine, nursing, or other professional healthcare services, including the giving of medical
advice, no doctor-patient relationship is formed. The use of this information and the materials
linked to this podcast is at the user's own risk. The content on this podcast is not intended to be a
substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment. Users should not disregard or
delay in obtaining medical advice from any medical condition they have, and they should seek
the assistance of their healthcare professionals for any such conditions. Finally, I take all conflicts of
interest very seriously. For all of my disclosures and the companies I invest in or advise,
please visit peteratiamd.com forward slash about where I keep an up-to-date and active list
of all disclosures.